Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-18T08:15:42.836Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Selective Processing of Food– and Body–Related Information and Autonomic Arousal in Patients with Eating Disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 April 2014

Conxa Perpiña*
Affiliation:
University of Valencia
Thierry Leonard
Affiliation:
Hospital Center Saint-Anne of Paris
Janet Treasure
Affiliation:
University of London
Alyson Bond
Affiliation:
University of London
Rosa Baños
Affiliation:
University of Valencia
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Dr. Conxa Perpiñá, Departamento de Personalidad. Facultad de Psicología. Avda. Blasco Ibáñez, 21. 46010 – Valencia (Spain). E-mail: perpinya@uv.es

Abstract

Both attentional bias (using the modified Stroop Task) and autonomic reactivity (skin conductance level) to food- and body-related information were assessed in 25 patients with eating disorders (15 patients with anorexia, 10 patients with bulimia) and 18 women controls. Patients with anorexia showed the greatest interference in color-naming food-related words. However, on this occasion there were no differences in body condition, probably because of heterogeneity of clinical samples and because the control group were staff members, so the target information was very familiar to them. The groups differed in their autonomic reactivity while performing the Stroop, the patients with anorexia responded with higher skin conductance (p < .036). The discussion focuses on the differential profiles shown by samples and on the relationship between cognitive biases and autonomic arousal reactivity.

Se evaluó el sesgo atencional (Tarea de Stroop Modificado) y la reactividad autonómica (Nivel de conductancia de la piel) a la información relacionada con comida y cuerpo en 25 pacientes con trastornos alimentarios (15 anoréxicas y 10 bulímicas) y en 18 mujeres normales. Las anoréxicas mostraron la mayor interferencia al nombrar el color de las palabras relacionadas con la comida, aunque las diferencias no resultaron significativas en cuanto a las palabras relacionadas con el cuerpo. Probablemente este resultado puede deberse tanto a la heterogeneidad de las muestras clínicas como a que el grupo control estaba compuesto por personal del hospital, por lo que la información presentada era igualmente familiar para todos ellos. Los grupos se mostraron diferentes en su reactividad autonómica mientras realizaban la tarea Stroop, siendo las anoréxicas las que mostraron una mayor activación (p < .036). Se analizan los perfiles diferenciales exhibidos por las distintas muestras, así como la relación entre sesgos cognitivos y reactividad autonómica.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1998

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd edition, revised). Washington, DC: APA Press.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edition). Washington, DC: APA Press.Google Scholar
Beck, J. G., Stanley, M. A., Averill, P. M., Baldwin, L. E., & Deagle, E. A. III (1992). Attention and memory for threat in panic disorder. Behavior Research and Therapy, 30, 619629.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ben-Tovim, D. I., & Walker, M. K. (1991). Further evidence for the Stroop test as a quantitative measure of psychopathology in eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 10, 609613.3.0.CO;2-M>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ben-Tovim, D. I., Walker, M. K., Fok, D., & Yap, E. (1989). An adaptation of the Stroop test for measuring shape and food concerns in eating disorders: A quantitative measure of psychopathology? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 8, 681687.3.0.CO;2-#>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calloway, P., Fonagy, P., & Wakeling, A. (1983). Autonomic arousal in eating disorder: further evidence for the clinical subdivision of anorexia nervosa. British Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 3842.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Channon, S., Hemsley, D., & de Silva, P. (1988). Selective processing of food words in anorexia nervosa. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 27, 259260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clarke, M. G., & Palmer, R. L. (1983). Eating attitudes and neurotic symptoms in university students. British Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 299304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, M. J., & Fairburn, C. G. (1992). Selective processing of eating, weight, and shape in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 30, 501511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, M. J., & Fairburn, C. G. (1993). Demographic and clinical correlates of selective information processing in patients with bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 13, 109116.3.0.CO;2-C>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehlers, A., Margraf, J., Davies, S., & Roth, W. (1988). Selective processing of threat cues in subjects with panic attacks. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 201219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fairburn, C. G., Cooper, P. J., Cooper, M. J., McKenna, F. P., & Anastasiades, P. (1991). Selective information processing in bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 10, 415422.3.0.CO;2-R>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fairburn, C. G., Cooper, Z., & Cooper, P. J. (1986). The clinical features and maintenance of bulimia nervosa. In Brownell, K.D. & Foreyt, J.P. (Eds.), Physiology, psychology, and treatment of the eating disorders (pp. 389404). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Foa, E. B., & McNally, R. (1986). Sensitivity to feared stimuli in obsessive-compulsives: a dichotic listening analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 10, 477485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garner, D. M., Olmstead, M. P., & Polivy, J. (1983). Development and validation of a multidimensional eating disorder inventory for anorexia nervosa and bulimia. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 2, 1534.3.0.CO;2-6>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, M. W., & McKenna, F. P. (1993). Developmental onset of eating related color-naming interference. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 13, 391397.3.0.CO;2-U>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, M. W., McKenna, F. P., & De Silva, M. S. L. (1994). Habituation patterns to colour naming of eating-related stimuli in anorexics and non-clinical controls. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 33, 499508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herman, C. P., & Polivy, J. (1980). Restrained eating. In Stunkard, A. J. (Ed.), Obesity (pp. 208225). Philadelphia: Saunders.Google Scholar
Hope, D. A., Rapee, R. M., Heimberg, R. G., & Dombeck, M. J. (1990). Representations of the self in social phobia: vulnerability to social threat. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 177189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, T., Perpiñá, C., Bond, A., & Treasure, J. (in preparation). Assessment of test-meal induced autonomic arousal in anorexic, bulimic, and control females.Google Scholar
MacLeod, C., & Rutherford, E. M. (1992). Anxiety and the selective processing of emotional information: mediating roles of awareness, trait and state variables, and personal relevance of stimulus material. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 30, 479491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mathews, A., & McLeod, C. (1985). Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23, 563568.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mavissakalian, M. (1987). Trimodal assessment in agoraphobia research: Further observations on heart rate and synchrony/desynchrony. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 9, 8998.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNally, R. J., Kaspi, S. P., Riemann, B. C., & Zeitlin, S. B. (1990). Selective processing of threat cues in post-traumatic stress disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 398402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNally, R. J., Riemann, B. C., & Kim, E. (1990). Selective processing of threat cues in panic disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 407412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perpiñá, C., Hemsley, D., Treasure, J., & de Silva, P. (1993). Is the selective information processing of food and body words specific to patients with eating disorders? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 14, 359366.3.0.CO;2-G>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salkind, M. R., Fincham, J., & Silverstone, T. (1980). Is anorexia nervosa a phobic disorder?: A psychophysiological enquiry. Biological Psychiatry, 15, 803808.Google ScholarPubMed
Schotte, D. E., McNally, R. J., & Turner, M. L. (1990). A dichotic listening analysis of body weight concern in bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 9, 109113.3.0.CO;2-P>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. E. (1970). State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, Ca.: Consulting Psychologist Press.Google Scholar
Stewart, S. H., & Samoluk, S. B. (1997). Effects of short-term food deprivation and chronic dietary restraint on the selective processing of appetitive-related cues. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 21, 129135.3.0.CO;2-N>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vitousek, K., & Manke, F. (1994). Personality variables and disorders in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 137147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watts, F. N., McKenna, F. P., Sharrock, R., & Trezise, L. (1986). Color Naming of phobia-related words. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 25, 253259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zigmond, A. S., & Snaith, R. P. (1983). The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 67, 361370CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed