Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-18T04:24:04.580Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Apprehending attitudes and actions: Separable abilities in early development?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 March 2009

R. Peter Hobson*
Affiliation:
Tavistock Clinic and UCL Medical School, London
*
R. Peter Hobson, Tavistock Clinic, 120 Belsize Lane, London, NW3 5BA, UK.

Abstract

In this article, I explore whether there might be two separable components to person-perception and interpersonal engagement in infancy: the ability to apprehend other people's attitudes on the one hand, and the ability to perceive actions on the other. In support of this approach, I summarize some evidence from recent study of autistic adolescents' judgments of attitudes and actions manifest in moving point-light displays of people. I highlight the far-reaching cognitive as well associal-developmental implications of early interpersonal-affective engagement. I conclude by speculating that certain abnormalities that would stem from autistic individuals' impaired ability to perceive and engage with people's attitudes may be somewhat mitigated by their relative ability to perceive the actions of others.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baron-Cohen, S., Spitz, A., & Cross, P. (1993). Do children with autism recognise surprise? A research note. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 507516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, E. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Ontogeny and phylogeny. In Collins, W. A. (Ed.), Children's language and communication, Minnesota symposia on child psychology (Vol. 12, pp. 121155). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bates, E. (1993, February). Nature, nurture and language development. Paper presented to the Society for Research in Child Development, New Orleans LA.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1979). Cognition and communication from nine to thirteen months: Correlational findings. In Bates, E. (Ed.), The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy (pp. 140). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 21, 205226.Google Scholar
Bertenthal, B. I., Proffitt, D. R., & Cutting, J. E. (1984). Infant sensitivity to figural coherence in biomechanical motions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 37, 213230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brazelton, T. B., Koslowski, B., & Main, M. (1974). The origins of reciprocity: The early mother-infant interaction. In Lewis, M. & Rosenblum, L. A. (Eds.), The effect of infancy on its caregiver (pp. 4977). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bretherton, I., McNew, S., & Beeghly-Smith, M. (1981). Early person knowledge as expressed in gestural and verbal communication: When do infants acquire a “theory of mind”? In Lamb, M. E. & Sherrod, L. R. (Eds.), Infant social cognition: Empirical and theoretical considerations (pp. 333373). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1983). Child's talk. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Camaioni, L. (1992). Mind knowledge in infancy: The emergence of intentional communication. Early Development and Parenting, 1, 1522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campos, J. J., & Stenberg, C. R. (1981). Perception, appraisal and emotion: The onset of social referencing. In Lamb, M. E. & Sherrod, L. R. (Eds.), Infant social cognition: Empirical and theoretical considerations (pp. 273314). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Caron, A. J., Caron, R. F., & Meyers, R. S. (1982). Abstraction of invariant face expressions in infancy. Child Development, 53, 10081015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohn, J. F., & Tronick, E. (1983). Three-month-old infants' reaction to simulated maternal depression. Child Development, 54, 185193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L., & Whetton, C. (1982). British picture vocabulary scale. Windsor: NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar
Feinman, S. (1982). Social referencing in infancy. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 28, 445470.Google Scholar
Feinman, S., & Lewis, M. (1983). Social referencing at ten months: A second-order effect on infants' responses to strangers. Child Development, 54, 878887.Google ScholarPubMed
Field, T. M. (1985). Neonatal perception of people: Maturational and individual differences. In Field, T. M. & Fox, N. A. (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 3152). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Field, T. M., Woodson, R., Cohen, D., Greenberg, R., Garcia, R., & Collins, K. (1983). Discrimination and imitation of facial expressions by term and preterm neonates. Infant Behavior and Development, 6, 485489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, T. M., Woodson, R., Greenberg, R., & Cohen, D. (1982). Discrimination and imitation of facial expressions by neonates. Science, 218, 179181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fodor, J. (1980). Methodological solipsism considered as a research strategy in cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 63109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, R., & McDaniel, C. (1982). The perception of biological motion by human infants. Science, 218, 486487.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frye, D., Rawling, P., Moore, C., & Myers, I. (1983). Object-person discrimination and communication at 3 and 10 months. Developmental Psychology, 19, 303309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gergely, G., Nadasdy, Z., Csibra, G., & Biro, S. (in press). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition.Google Scholar
Gomez, J.-C., Sarria, E., & Tamarit, J. (1993). The comparative study of early communication and theories of mind: Ontogeny, phylogeny, and pathology. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cohen, D. (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 397426). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Grice, H. P. (1957). Meaning. Philosophical Review, 66, 377388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haviland, J. M., & Lelwica, M. (1987). The induced affect response: 10-week-old infants' responses to three emotion expressions. Developmental Psychology, 23, 97104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, R. P. (1989). Beyond cognition: A theory of autism. In Dawson, G. (Ed.), Autism: Nature, diagnosis, and treatment (pp. 2248). New York: Gunford.Google Scholar
Hobson, R. P. (1990a). On the origins of self and the case of autism. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 163181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, R. P. (1990b). Concerning knowledge of mental states. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 63, 199213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobson, R. P. (1990c). On acquiring knowledge about people and the capacity to pretend: A response to Leslie. Psychological Review, 97, 114121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobson, R. P. (1991). Against the theory of “Theory Mind.” British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 3351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, R. P. (1993a). Understanding persons: The role of affect. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 204227). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hobson, R. P. (1993b). The emotional origins of interpersonal understanding. Philosophical Psychology, 6, 227249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, R. P. (1993c). Autism and the development of mind. Hove, Sussex: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hobson, R. P. (1994). Perceiving attitudes, conceiving minds. In Lewis, C. & Mitchell, P. (Eds.), Origins of an understanding of mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hoffman, M. L. (1984). Interaction of affect and cognition in empathy. In Izard, C. E., Kagan, J., & Zajonc, R. (Eds.), Emotions, cognition and behaviour (pp. 101131). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hornik, R., Risenhoover, N., & Gunnar, M. (1987). The effects of maternal positive, neutral and negative affective communications on infant responses new toys. Child Development, 58, 937944.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception and Psychophysics, 14, 201211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kagan, J. (1982). The emergence of self. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 25, 363381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaye, K. (1982). The mental and social life of babies. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Klinnert, M. D., Campos, J. J., Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., & Svejda, M. (1983). Emotions as behavior regulators: Social referencing in infancy. In Plutchik, R. & Kellerman, H. (Eds.), Emotion: Theory, research and experience. Vol. 2: Emotions in early development (pp. 5786). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
La Barbera, J. D., Izard, C. E., Vietze, P., & Parisi, S. A. (1976). Four- and six-month-old infants' visual responses to joy, anger, and neutral expressions. Child Development, 47, 535538.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of “theory of mind.” Psychological Review, 94, 412426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leslie, A. M. (1994). ToMM, ToBy, and agency: Core architecture and domain specificity. In Hirschfeld, L. & Gelman, S. (Eds.), Domain specificity in cognition and culture: Mapping the mind (pp. 119148). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self and society. Chicago and London: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. (1990). Foundations for developing a concept of self: The role of imitation in relating self other and the value of social mirroring, social modeling, and self practice in infancy. In Cicchetti, D. & Beeghly, M. (Eds.), The self in transition (pp. 139164). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 7578.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Development, 54, 702709.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1989). Imitation in newborn infants: Exploring the range of gestures imitated and the underlying mechanisms. Developmental Psychology, 25, 954962.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, D., Hobson, R. P., & Lee, A. (1994). Components of person-perception: An investigation with autistic, nonautistic retarded and normal children and adolescents. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
Murray, L., & Trevarthen, C. (1985). Emotional regulation of interactions between two-month-olds and their mothers. In Field, T. M. & Fox, N. A. (Eds.), Social perception of infants (pp. 177197). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Premack, D. (1991). The infant's theory of self-propelled objects. In Frye, D. & Moore, C. (Eds.), Children's theories of mind (pp. 3948). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Russell, J. (1994). Agency and early mental development. In Bermudez, J., Marcel, A. J., & Eilan, N. (Eds.), The body and the self. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Sagi, A., & Hoffman, M. L. (1976). Empathetic distress in the newborn. Developmental Psychology, 12, 175176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simner, M. L. (1971). Newborns' response to the cry another infant. Developmental Psychology, 5, 136150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, D. (1977). The first relationship: Infant and mother. Glasgow: Fontana/Open Books.Google Scholar
Sylvester-Bradley, B. (1985). Failure to distinguish between people and things in early infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3, 281292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1993). On the interpersonal origins of self-concept. In Neisser, U. (Ed.), The perceived self (pp. 174184). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Barton, M. (in press). Learning words in non-ostensive contexts. Developmental Psychology.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In Bullowa, M. (Ed.), Before speech pp. 321347). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1982). The primary motives for cooperative understanding. In Butterworth, G. & Light, P. (Eds.), Social cognition (pp. 77109). Brighton: Harvester.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C., & Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary inter-subjectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year. In Lock, A. (Ed.), Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language pp. 183229). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walden, T. A., & Ogan, T. A. (1988). The development of social referencing. Child Development, 59, 12301240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker, A. S. (1982). Intermodal perception of expressive behaviors by human infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33, 514535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young-Browne, G., Rosenfeld, H. M., & Horowitz, F. D. (1977). Infant discrimination of facial expressions. Child Development, 48, 555562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar