Article contents
The evolutionary benefit of less-credible affective musical signals for emotion induction during storytelling
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 30 September 2021
Abstract
The credible signaling theory underexplains the evolutionary added value of less-credible affective musical signals compared to vocal signals. The theory might be extended to account for the motivation for, and consequences of, culturally decontextualizing a biologically contextualized signal. Musical signals are twofold, communicating “emotional fiction” alongside biological meaning, and could have filled an adaptive need for affect induction during storytelling.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Arnal, L. H., Flinker, A., Kleinschmidt, A., Giraud, A. L., & Poeppel, D. (2015). Human screams occupy a privileged niche in the communication soundscape. Current Biology, 25(15), 2051–2056. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.06.043.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bown, J., & Gackenbach, J. (2016). Video games, nightmares, and emotional processing. In Tettegah, S. Y. & Huang, W. D. (Eds.), Emotions, technology, and digital games (pp. 3–14). Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryant, G. A. (2020). Evolution, structure, and functions of human laughter. In Floyd, K. & Weber, R. (Eds.), The handbook of communication science and biology (pp. 63–77). Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engelberg, J. W. M., & Gouzoules, H. (2019). The credibility of acted screams: Implications for emotional communication research. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(8), 1889–1902. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021818816307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frühholz, S, & Schweinberger, S. (2020). Nonverbal auditory communication – evidence for integrated neural systems for voice signal production and perception. Progress in Neurobiology, 199, 101948.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frühholz, S., Trost, W., & Grandjean, D. (2014). The role of the medial temporal limbic system in processing emotions in voice and music. Progress in Neurobiology, 123, 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2014.09.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frühholz, S., Trost, W., & Kotz, S. A. (2016). The sound of emotions – towards a unifying neural network perspective of affective sound processing. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 68, 1–15. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.05.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Juslin, P. N., & Laukka, P. (2003). Communication of emotions in vocal expression and music performance: Different channels, same code?. Psychological Bulletin, 129(5), 770–814. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.5.770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paquette, S., Takerkart, S., Saget, S., Peretz, I., & Belin, P. (2018). Cross-classification of musical and vocal emotions in the auditory cortex. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1423(1), 329–337. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.13666.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Revonsuo, A. (2000). The reinterpretation of dreams: An evolutionary hypothesis of the function of dreaming. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(6), 877–901.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scherer, K. R. (1995). Expression of emotion in voice and music. Journal of Voice, 9(3), 235–248. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0892-1997(05)80231-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, J. W., Engelberg, J. W., & Gouzoules, H. (2019). What is a scream? Acoustic characteristics of a human call type. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 145(3), 1776–1776. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.5101500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scrivner, C., Johnson, J. A., Kjeldgaard-Christiansen, J., & Clasen, M. (2020). Pandemic practice: Horror fans and morbidly curious individuals are more psychologically resilient during the COVID-19 pandemic. Personality and Individual Differences, 168, 110397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, D., Schlaepfer, P., Major, K., Dyble, M., Page, A. E., Thompson, J., … Astete, L. (2017). Cooperation and the evolution of hunter-gatherer storytelling. Nature Communications, 8(1), 1–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(5), 645–665.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teigen, K. H. (2008). Is a sigh “just a sigh”? Sighs as emotional signals and responses to a difficult task. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 49(1), 49–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trevor, C., Arnal, L. H., & Frühholz, S. (2020). Terrifying film music mimics alarming acoustic feature of human screams. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 147(6), EL540–EL545. https://doi.org/10.1121/10.0001459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trevor, C., & Huron, D. (2018). Are humoresques humorous? On the similarity between laughter and staccato. Empirical Musicology Review, 13(1–2), 66. https://doi.org/10.18061/emr.v13i1-2.5608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vassilakis, P. N., & Kendall, R. A. (2010). Psychoacoustic and cognitive aspects of auditory roughness: Definitions, models, and applications. Proceedings of Human Vision and Electronic Imaging XV, 7527, 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1117/12.845457.Google Scholar
Walton, K. L. (1990). Mimesis as make-believe: On the foundations of the representational arts. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
- 1
- Cited by
Target article
Music as a coevolved system for social bonding
Related commentaries (24)
A boldly comparative approach will strengthen co-evolutionary accounts of musicality's origins
A neurodevelopmental disorders perspective into music, social attention, and social bonding
Beyond “consistent with” adaptation: Is there a robust test for music adaptation?
Clarifying the link between music and social bonding by measuring prosociality in context
Ecological and psychological factors in the cultural evolution of music
Evolutionary linguistics can help refine (and test) hypotheses about how music might have evolved
Human evolution of gestural messaging and its critical role in the human development of music
If it quacks like a duck: The by-product account of music still stands
Is neural entrainment to rhythms the basis of social bonding through music?
Is the MSB hypothesis (music as a coevolved system for social bonding) testable in the Popperian sense?
Isochrony, vocal learning, and the acquisition of rhythm and melody
Music and dance are two parallel routes for creating social cohesion
Music as a social bond in patients with amnesia
Music as a trait in evolutionary theory: A musicological perspective
Not by signalling alone: Music's mosaicism undermines the search for a proper function
Oxytocin as an allostatic agent in the social bonding effects of music
Pre-hunt charade as the cradle of human musicality
Progress without exclusion in the search for an evolutionary basis of music
Rapid dissonant grunting, or, but why does music sound the way it does?
Sex and drugs and rock and roll
Social bonding and music: Evidence from lesions to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex
The evolution of music as artistic cultural innovation expressing intuitive thought symbolically
Where they sing solo: Accounting for cross-cultural variation in collective music-making in theories of music evolution
Why don't cockatoos have war songs?
Author response
Toward a productive evolutionary understanding of music
Toward inclusive theories of the evolution of musicality