Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-01T19:51:20.539Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Coherence, Muller’s Ratchet, and the Maintenance of Culture

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2022

Abstract

I investigate the structure of an argument that culture cannot be maintained in a population if each individual learns only from a single person. This appears to conflict with (1) many models of cultural transmission and (2) real-world cases. I resolve the first problem by showing that one of the models central to the argument is conceptually analogous and mathematically equivalent to one used to investigate the evolution of sexual reproduction. I resolve the second by arguing that probabilistic models of epistemological coherence can be reinterpreted as models of support between cultural variants—illustrating the idea with a new model.

Type
Game Theory and Formal Models
Copyright
Copyright © The Philosophy of Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

I am grateful for feedback from Ted Benditt, Scott Brande, Justin Bruner, Mark Colyvan, Bruce Glymour, Chris Hitchcock, Robin Jeshion, Jack Justus, Stephen Merritt, Bart Moffat, Bence Nanay, Michael Nelson, Samir Okasha, Grant Ramsey, Tonia Schwartz, Joel Velasco, Ken Waters, Bill Wimsatt, Kevin Zollman, and audience members at the University of Alabama at Birmingham, Mississippi State University, the Society for Exact Philosophy (2014), and the Philosophy of Science Association (2014).

References

Abrams, Marshall. 2013. “A Moderate Role for Cognitive Models in Agent-Based Modeling of Cultural Change.” Complex Adaptive Systems Modeling 1 (16): 133.10.1186/2194-3206-1-16CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abrams, Marshall 2014. “Maintenance of Cultural Diversity: Social Roles, Social Networks, and Cognitive Networks.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 37:254–55.10.1017/S0140525X13002811CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abrams, Marshall 2015. “Cultural Variant Interaction in Teaching and Transmission.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 38, forthcoming.10.1017/S0140525X14000648CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alexander, J. McKenzie. 2007. The Structural Evolution of Morality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511550997CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bransford, John, and National Research Council. 2000. How People Learn: Brain, Mind, Experience, and School. Washington, DC: National Academy.Google Scholar
Caporael, Linnda R., Griesemer, James R., and Wimsatt, William C., eds. 2014. Developing Scaffolds in Evolution, Culture, and Cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Enquist, Magnus, Strimling, Pontus, Eriksson, Kimmo, Laland, Kevin, and Sjostrand, Jonas. 2010. “One Cultural Parent Makes No Culture.” Animal Behaviour 79:1353–62.10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.03.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitelson, Branden. 2003. “A Probabilistic Theory of Coherence.” Analysis 63:194–99.10.1093/analys/63.3.194CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitelson, Branden 2004. “Two Technical Corrections to My Coherence Measure.” Author’s website. http://fitelson.org/coherence2.pdf.Google Scholar
Grim, Patrick, Singer, Daniel J., Reade, Christopher, and Fisher, Steven. 2015. “Germs, Genes, and Memes: Function and Fitness Dynamics on Information Networks.” Philosophy of Science 82 (2): 219–43.10.1086/680486CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haigh, John. 1978. “The Accumulation of Deleterious Genes in a Population: Muller’s Ratchet.” Theoretical Population Biology 14:251–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kline, Michelle Ann. 2015. “How to Learn about Teaching: An Evolutionary Framework for the Study of Teaching Behavior in Humans and Other Animals.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 38, forthcoming.10.1017/S0140525X14000090CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, C. I. 1946. An Analysis of Knowledge and Valuation. La Salle, IL: Open Court.Google Scholar
Maynard Smith, John. 1978. The Evolution of Sex. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Olsson, Erik J. 2005. Against Coherence: Truth, Probability, and Justification. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/0199279993.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Otto, Sarah P., and Lenormand, Thomas. 2002. “Resolving the Paradox of Sex and Recombination.” Nature Reviews Genetics 3 (4): 252–61.10.1038/nrg761CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, Alan R. 1988. “Does Biology Constrain Culture?American Anthropologist 90 (4): 819–31.10.1525/aa.1988.90.4.02a00030CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schupbach, Jonah N. 2011. “New Hope for Shogenji’s Coherence Measure.” British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 62:125–42.10.1093/bjps/axq031CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shogenji, Tomoji. 1999. “Is Coherence Truth Conducive?Analysis 59 (4): 338–45.10.1093/analys/59.4.338CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smaldino, Paul. 2014. “The Cultural Evolution of Emergent Group-Level Traits.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 37:243–54.Google ScholarPubMed
Thagard, Paul. 2012. “Mapping Minds across Cultures.” In Grounding Social Sciences in Cognitive Sciences, ed. Sun, Ron, 3562. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Waxman, David, and Loewe, Laurence. 2010. “A Stochastic Model for a Single Click of Muller’s Ratchet.” Journal of Theoretical Biology 264:1120–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wimsatt, William C. 2007. Re-engineering Philosophy for Limited Beings: Piecewise Approximations to Reality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.10.2307/j.ctv1pncnrhCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wimsatt, William C., and Griesemer, James R.. 2007. “Reproducing Entrenchments to Scaffold Culture: The Central Role of Development in Cultural Evolution.” In Integrating Evolution and Development, ed. Sansom, Roger and Brandon, Robert N., 227323. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Young, H. Peyton. 1998. Individual Strategy and Social Structure: An Evolutionary Theory of Institutions. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.10.1515/9780691214252CrossRefGoogle Scholar