Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-17T19:27:20.876Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Intrauterine environment and cognitive development in young twins

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 June 2013

E. E. Antoniou*
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Psychological Science, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, The Netherlands
T. Fowler
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, Heart of Birmingham Teaching PCT, Birmingham, UK
E. Thiery
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, Ghent University Hospital, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
T. R. Southwood
Affiliation:
Institute of Child Health, University of Birmingham, UK
S. van Gestel
Affiliation:
Centre of Human Genetics, University Hospitals Leuven & Department of Human Genetics, KU Leuven, Belgium
N. Jacobs
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Open University of the Netherlands, Heerlen, The Netherlands Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, Maastricht University Medical Centre, The Netherlands
R. Vlietinck
Affiliation:
Centre of Human Genetics, University Hospitals Leuven & Department of Human Genetics, KU Leuven, Belgium
J. van Os
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, Maastricht University Medical Centre, The Netherlands
F. V. Rijsdijk
Affiliation:
King's College London, MRC Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, London, UK
C. Derom
Affiliation:
Centre of Human Genetics, University Hospitals Leuven & Department of Human Genetics, KU Leuven, Belgium
M. P. Zeegers
Affiliation:
Department of Complex Genetics, Cluster of Genetics and Cell Biology, NUTRIM School for Nutrition, Toxicology and Metabolism, Maastricht University Medical Centre, Maastricht, The Netherlands
*
*Address for correspondence: E. E. Antoniou, Department of Clinical Psychological Science, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, The Netherlands. (Email eva.antoniou@maastrichtuniversity.nl)

Abstract

Intrauterine factors important for cognitive development, such as birth weight, chorionicity and umbilical cord characteristics were investigated. A total of 663 twin pairs completed the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised and scores were available for Performance, Verbal and Total Intelligence Quotient (IQ). The intrauterine factors examined were birth weight, placental weight and morphology, cord knots, cord length and cord insertion. IQ scores for the varying levels of the intrauterine markers adjusting for gender and gestational age were calculated. The heritability of IQ and the association between IQ and intrauterine environment were examined. Twins with lower birth weight and cord knots had lower IQ scores. The aetiology of IQ is largely distinct from that of birth weight and cord knots, and non-shared environment may influence the observed relationships.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press and the International Society for Developmental Origins of Health and Disease 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Shared last authorship.

References

1.Hoet, JJ, Hanson, MA. Intrauterine nutrition: its importance during critical periods for cardiovascular and endocrine development. J Physiol. 1999; 514(Pt 3), 617627.Google Scholar
2.Barker, DJ, Martyn, CN. The maternal and fetal origins of cardiovascular disease. J Epidemiol Community Health. 1992; 46, 811.Google Scholar
3.Baird, J, Osmond, C, MacGregor, A, et al. Testing the fetal origins hypothesis in twins: the Birmingham twin study. Diabetologia. 2001; 44, 3339.Google Scholar
4.Law, CM, Barker, DJ, Bull, AR, Osmond, C. Maternal and fetal influences on blood pressure. Arch Dis Child. 1991; 66, 12911295.Google Scholar
5.Deary, IJ, Spinath, FM, Bates, TC. Genetics of intelligence. Eur J Hum Genet. 2006; 14, 690700.Google Scholar
6.Batty, GD, Deary, IJ. Early life intelligence and adult health. BMJ. 2004; 329, 585586.Google Scholar
7.Deary, IJ, Whiteman, MC, Starr, JM, Whalley, LJ, Fox, HC. The impact of childhood intelligence on later life: following up the Scottish mental surveys of 1932 and 1947. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2004; 86, 130147.Google Scholar
8.Hart, CL, Taylor, MD, Davey Smith, G, et al. Childhood IQ, social class, deprivation, and their relationships with mortality and morbidity risk in later life: prospective observational study linking the Scottish Mental Survey 1932 and the Midspan studies. Psychosom Med. 2003; 65, 877883.Google Scholar
9.Bouchard, TJ Jr. Genetic and environmental influences on adult intelligence and special mental abilities. Hum Biol. 1998; 70, 257279.Google Scholar
10.Bergvall, N, Iliadou, A, Tuvemo, T, Cnattingius, S. Birth characteristics and risk of low intellectual performance in early adulthood: are the associations confounded by socioeconomic factors in adolescence or familial effects? Pediatrics. 2006; 117, 714721.Google Scholar
11.Posthuma, D, de Geus, EJ, Boomsma, DI. Perceptual speed and IQ are associated through common genetic factors. Behav Genet. 2001; 31, 593602.Google Scholar
12.Rijsdijk, FV, Vernon, PA, Boomsma, DI. Application of hierarchical genetic models to Raven and WAIS subtests: a Dutch twin study. Behav Genet. 2002; 32, 199210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13.Hoekstra, RA, Bartels, M, Boomsma, DI. Longitudinal genetic study of verbal and nonverbal IQ from early childhood to young adulthood. Learn Individ Differ. 2007; 17, 97114.Google Scholar
14.Nichols, R. Twin studies of ability, personality and interests. Homo. 1978; 29, 158173.Google Scholar
15.Bouchard, TJ Jr. McGue, M. Familial studies of intelligence: a review. Science. 1981; 212, 10551059.Google Scholar
16.Bergen, SE, Gardner, CO, Kendler, KS. Age-related changes in heritability of behavioral phenotypes over adolescence and young adulthood: a meta-analysis. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2007; 10, 423433.Google Scholar
17.Breslau, N, DelDotto, JE, Brown, GG, et al. A gradient relationship between low birth weight and IQ at age 6 years. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 1994; 148, 377383.Google Scholar
18.Koeppen-Schomerus, G, Spinath, FM, Plomin, R. Twins and non-twin siblings: different estimates of shared environmental influence in early childhood. Twin Res. 2003; 6, 97105.Google Scholar
19.Wolke, D, Rizzo, P, Woods, S. Persistent infant crying and hyperactivity problems in middle childhood. Pediatrics. 2002; 109, 10541060.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20.Botting, N, Powls, A, Cooke, RW, Marlow, N. Cognitive and educational outcome of very-low-birthweight children in early adolescence. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1998; 40, 652660.Google Scholar
21.Boomsma, DI, van Beijsterveldt, CE, Rietveld, MJ, Bartels, M, van Baal, GC. Genetics mediate relation of birth weight to childhood IQ. BMJ. 2001; 323, 14261427.Google Scholar
22.Devlin, B, Daniels, M, Roeder, K. The heritability of IQ. Nature. 1997; 388, 468471.Google Scholar
23.Bleker, OP, Buimer, M, van der Post, JA, van der Veen, F. Ted (G.J.) Kloosterman: on intrauterine growth. The significance of prenatal care. Studies on birth weight, placental weight and placental index. Placenta. 2006; 27, 10521054.Google Scholar
24.Ananth, CV, Vintzileos, AM, Shen-Schwarz, S, Smulian, JC, Lai, YL. Standards of birth weight in twin gestations stratified by placental chorionicity. Obstet Gynecol. 1998; 91, 917924.Google Scholar
25.Naeye, RL, Benirschke, K, Hagstrom, JW, Marcus, CC. Intrauterine growth of twins as estimated from liveborn birth-weight data. Pediatrics. 1966; 37, 409416.Google Scholar
26.Gielen, M, Derom, C, Derom, R, Vlietinck, R, Zeegers, MP. Can birthweight discordancy within monozygotic twin pairs be used as an indicator of chorionicity? Twin Res Hum Genet. 2009; 12, 169174.Google Scholar
27.Knaus, HH. On the factors determining the size of the newborn. J Obstet Gynaecol Br Emp. 1949; 56, 856859.Google Scholar
28.Mc, KT, Record, RG. The influence of placental size on foetal growth in man, with special reference to multiple pregnancy. J Endocrinol. 1953; 9, 418426.Google Scholar
29.Papageorghiou, AT, Bakoulas, V, Sebire, NJ, Nicolaides, KH. Intrauterine growth in multiple pregnancies in relation to fetal number, chorionicity and gestational age. Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol. 2008; 32, 890893.Google Scholar
30.Loos, RJ, Derom, C, Derom, R, Vlietinck, R. Birthweight in liveborn twins: the influence of the umbilical cord insertion and fusion of placentas. BJOG. 2001; 108, 943948.Google Scholar
31.Sarwono, E, Disse, WS, Oudesluys, M, Oosting, H, DeGroot, CJ. Umbilical cord length and intrauterine well-being. Peadiatr Indones. 1991; 31, 136140.Google Scholar
32.Benirschke, K, Kaufmann, P. Pathology of the Human Placenta, 1995. Springer-Verlag: New York.Google Scholar
33.Adinma, JIB. The umbilical cord: a study of 1000 consecutive deliveries. Int J Fertil. 1993; 38, 175179.Google Scholar
34.Baergen, RN, Malicki, D, Behling, C, Benirschke, K. Morbidity, mortality, and placental pathology in excessively long umbilical cords: retrospective study. Pediatr Dev Pathol. 2001; 4, 144153.Google Scholar
35.Taweevisit, M, Thorner, PS. Massive fetal thrombotic vasculopathy associated with excessively long umbilical cord and fetal demise: case report and literature review. Pediatr Dev Pathol. 2010; 13, 112115.Google Scholar
36.Heifetz, SA. The umbilical cord: obstetrically important lesions. Clin Obstet Gynecol. 1996; 39, 571587.Google Scholar
37.Naeye, R. Umbilical cord length:clinical significance. J Pediatr. 1985; 107, 278281.Google Scholar
38.Naeye, RL, Peters, EC. Antenatal hypoxia and low IQ values. Am J Dis Child. 1987; 141, 5054.Google Scholar
39.Sornes, T. Umbilical cord encirclements and fetal growth restriction. Obstet Gynecol. 1995; 86, 725728.Google Scholar
40.Bhate, V, Deshpande, S, Bhat, D, et al. Vitamin B12 status of pregnant Indian women and cognitive function in their 9-year-old children. Food Nutr Bull. 2008; 29, 249254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
41.Posthuma, D, De Geus, EJ, Bleichrodt, N, Boomsma, DI. Twin-singleton differences in intelligence? Twin Res. 2000; 3, 8387.Google Scholar
42.Ronalds, GA, De Stavola, BL, Leon, DA. The cognitive cost of being a twin: evidence from comparisons within families in the Aberdeen children of the 1950s cohort study. BMJ. 2005; 331, 1306.Google Scholar
43.Gordon, N. Some influences on cognition in early life: a short review of recent opinions. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 1998; 2, 15.Google Scholar
44.Derom, C, Vlietinck, R, Thiery, E, et al. The East Flanders Prospective Twin Survey (EFPTS). Twin Res. 2002; 5, 337341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45.Wechsler, D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-R (Dutch version), 1986. Swets & Zeitlinger BV: Lisse.Google Scholar
46.Derom, R, Derom, C, Vlietinck, R. Placentation. In Mulitiple Pregnancy: Epidemiology, Gestation & Perinatal outcome (eds. Keith L, Papiernik E, Keith D, Luke B), 1995; pp. 113128. The Parthenon Publishing Group: New York.Google Scholar
47.Stata. Statistical Software, 2009. Stata Corp LP: College Station, TX.Google Scholar
48.Neale, MC, Cardon, LR. Methodology for Genetic Studies of Twins and Families, 1992. Kluwer Academic Publishers: Dordrecht.Google Scholar
49.Neale, MC. MX: Statistical Modelling, 1999. Department of Psychiatry, Medical College of Virginia: Richmond, VA.Google Scholar
50.van Beijsterveldt, CE, Felsenfeld, S, Boomsma, DI. Bivariate genetic analyses of stuttering and nonfluency in a large sample of 5-year-old twins. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2010; 53, 609619.Google Scholar
51.Luciano, M, Wright, MJ, Martin, NG. Exploring the etiology of the association between birthweight and IQ in an adolescent twin sample. Twin Res. 2004; 7, 6271.Google Scholar
52.Scrimshaw, N, Gordon, J. Proceedings of an International Conference on Malnutrition, Learning and Behaviour, 1967. Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press ed: Cambridge.Google Scholar
53.Gordon, N. Nutrition and cognitive function. Brain Dev. 1997; 19, 165170.Google Scholar
54.Winick, M. Malnutrition and brain development. J Pediatr. 1969; 74, 667679.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
55.Shenkin, SD, Starr, JM, Deary, IJ. Birth weight and cognitive ability in childhood: a systematic review. Psychol Bull. 2004; 130, 9891013.Google Scholar
56.Breslau, N. Psychiatric sequelae of low birth weight. Epidemiol Rev. 1995; 17, 96106.Google Scholar
57.Richards, M, Hardy, R, Kuh, D, Wadsworth, ME. Birth weight and cognitive function in the British 1946 birth cohort: longitudinal population based study. BMJ. 2001; 322, 199203.Google Scholar
58.Vlietinck, R, Derom, R, Neale, MC, et al. Genetic and environmental variation in the birth weight of twins. Behav Genet. 1989; 19, 151161.Google Scholar
59.Morton, NE. The inheritance of human birth weight. Ann Hum Genet. 1955; 20, 125134.Google Scholar
60.van Baal, CG, Boomsma, DI. Etiology of individual differences in birth weight of twins as a function of maternal smoking during pregnancy. Twin Res. 1998; 1, 123130.Google Scholar
61.Petersen, I, Jensen, VM, McGue, M, Bingley, P, Christensen, K. No evidence of genetic mediation in the association between birthweight and academic performance in 2,413 Danish adolescent twin pairs. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2009; 12, 564572.Google Scholar
62.Berger, A. Insulin-like growth factor and cognitive function. BMJ. 2001; 322, 203.Google Scholar
63.van Dam, PS, Aleman, A, de Vries, WR, et al. Growth hormone, insulin-like growth factor I and cognitive function in adults. Growth Horm IGF Res. 2000; 10(Suppl B), S69S73.Google Scholar
64.Gunnell, D, Miller, LL, Rogers, I, Holly, JM. Association of insulin-like growth factor I and insulin-like growth factor-binding protein-3 with intelligence quotient among 8- to 9-year-old children in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Pediatrics. 2005; 116, e681e686.Google Scholar
65.Lunde, A, Melve, KK, Gjessing, HK, Skjaerven, R, Irgens, LM. Genetic and environmental influences on birth weight, birth length, head circumference, and gestational age by use of population-based parent-offspring data. Am J Epidemiol. 2007; 165, 734741.Google Scholar
66.Sornes, T. Umbilical cord knots. Acta Obstet Gynecol Scand. 2000; 79, 157159.Google Scholar
67.Chan, JS, Baergen, RN. Gross umbilical cord complications are associated with placental lesions of circulatory stasis and fetal hypoxia. Pediatric Dev Pathol. 2012; 15, 487494.Google Scholar
68.Rees, S, Harding, R, Walker, D. An adverse intrauterine environment: implications for injury and altered development of the brain. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2008; 26, 311.Google Scholar
69.Anastario, M, Salafia, CM, Fitzmaurice, G, Goldstein, JM. Impact of fetal versus perinatal hypoxia on sex differences in childhood outcomes: developmental timing matters. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 2012; 47, 455464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
70.Luciano, M, Smith, GA, Wright, MJ, et al. Genetic covariance between processing speed and IQ. In Behaviour Genetics in the Postgenomic Era (eds. Plomin R, DeFries JC, McGuffin P, Craig I), 2003; pp. 163182. APA Books: Washington, DC.Google Scholar