Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-xtgtn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T22:17:18.141Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Auditory–visual speech perception in three- and four-year-olds and its relationship to perceptual attunement and receptive vocabulary*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 June 2017

DOĞU ERDENER*
Affiliation:
Psychology Program, Middle East Technical University, Northern Cyprus Campus, Güzelyurt/Morphou, Northern Cyprus
DENIS BURNHAM
Affiliation:
The MARCS Institute for Brain, Behaviour and Development, Western Sydney University, Australia
*
Address for correspondence: Dr Doğu Erdener, Psychology Program, Middle East Technical University–Northern Cyprus Campus, Kalkanlı, Güzelyurt (Morphou), KKTC via Mersin 10 Turkey. tel: +90 392 6613424; fax: +90 392 6612049; e-mail: d.erdener@gmail.com and vdogu@metu.edu.tr

Abstract

Despite the body of research on auditory–visual speech perception in infants and schoolchildren, development in the early childhood period remains relatively uncharted. In this study, English-speaking children between three and four years of age were investigated for: (i) the development of visual speech perception – lip-reading and visual influence in auditory–visual integration; (ii) the development of auditory speech perception and native language perceptual attunement; and (iii) the relationship between these and a language skill relevant at this age, receptive vocabulary. Visual speech perception skills improved even over this relatively short time period. However, regression analyses revealed that vocabulary was predicted by auditory-only speech perception, and native language attunement, but not by visual speech perception ability. The results suggest that, in contrast to infants and schoolchildren, in three- to four-year-olds the relationship between speech perception and language ability is based on auditory and not visual or auditory–visual speech perception ability. Adding these results to existing findings allows elaboration of a more complete account of the developmental course of auditory–visual speech perception.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This research was partially supported by an Australian Postgraduate Award from the University of Western Sydney, and by an Australian Research Council Discovery grant (DP0558698) to the second author. The authors express their gratitude to Ms Amanda Reid for her invaluable contribution to the manuscript and analyses as well as the children and their parents, without whom this study would not have been possible.

References

REFERENCES

Bates, T. C. & D'Oliveiro, L. (2003). PsyScript: a Macintosh application for scripting experiments. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers 35(4), 565–76.Google Scholar
Bundgaard-Nielsen, R. L., Best, C. T., Kroos, C. & Tyler, M. D. (2012). Second language learners’ vocabulary expansion is associated with improved second language vowel intelligibility. Applied Psycholinguistics 33(3), 643–64.Google Scholar
Bundgaard-Nielsen, R. L., Best, C. T. & Tyler, M. D. (2011a). Vocabulary size is associated with second-language vowel perception performance in adult learners. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 33(3), 433–61.Google Scholar
Bundgaard-Nielsen, R. L., Best, C. T. & Tyler, M. D. (2011b). Vocabulary size matters: the assimilation of second-language Australian English vowels to first-language Japanese vowel categories. Applied Psycholinguistics 32(1), 5167.Google Scholar
Burnham, D. (1998) Language specificity in the development of auditory-visual speech perception. In Campbell, R., Dodd, B. & Burnham, D. (eds), Hearing by eye II: advances in the psychology of speechreading and auditory-visual speech (pp. 2760). Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Burnham, D. (2003). Language specific speech perception and the onset of reading. Reading and Writing 16, 573609.Google Scholar
Burnham, D. & Dodd, B. (1998) Familiarity and novelty in infant cross-language studies: factors, problems, and a possible solution. In Rovee-Collier, C. (ed.), Advances in infancy research, 12 (pp. 170–87). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Burnham, D. & Dodd, B. (2004). Auditory-visual speech integration by pre-linguistic infants: perception of an emergent consonant in the McGurk effect. Developmental Psychobiology 44, 209–20.Google Scholar
Burnham, D. K., Earnshaw, L. J. & Clark, J. E. (1991). Development of categorical identification of native and non-native bilabial stops: infants, children and adults. Journal of Child Language 18, 231–60.Google Scholar
Burnham, D. & Sekiyama, K. (2012). Investigating auditory-visual speech perception development. In Bailly, G., Perrier, P. & Vatikiotis-Bateson, E. (eds), (in preparation). Advances in audio-visual speech processing (pp. 6275). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Campbell, R., Dodd, B. & Burnham, D. (eds), (1998). Hearing by eye II: advances in the psychology of speechreading and auditory-visual speech. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Desjardins, R. N., Rogers, J. & Werker, J. F. (1997). An exploration of why preschoolers perform differently than do adults in audiovisual speech perception tasks. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 66, 85110.Google Scholar
Desjardins, R. N. & Werker, J. F. (2004). Is the integration of heard and seen speech mandatory for infants? Developmental Psychobiology 45, 187203.Google Scholar
Dodd, B. & Burnham, D. K. (1988). Processing speechread information. The Volta Review: New Reflections on Speechreading 90, 4560.Google Scholar
Dodd, B., McIntosh, B., Erdener, D. & Burnham, D. (2008) Perception of the auditory-visual illusion in speech perception by children with phonological disorders. Clinical Phonetics and Linguistics 22, 6982.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M. & Dunn, L. M. (1997). PPVT-III: Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Third Edition. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Elliott, L. L., Hammer, M. A. & Scholl, M. E. (1989). Fine-grained auditory discrimination in normal children and children with language-learning problems. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 32(1), 112–9.Google Scholar
Erdener, D. & Burnham, D. (2013). The relationship between auditory-visual speech perception and language-specific speech perception at the onset of reading instruction in English-speaking children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 116, 120–38.Google Scholar
Hockley, N. & Polka, L. (1994). A developmental study of audiovisual speech perception using the McGurk paradigm. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 96, 3309.Google Scholar
Horlyck, S., Reid, A. & Burnham, D. (2012). The relationship between learning to read and language-specific speech perception: maturation versus experience. Scientific Studies of Reading 16(3), 218–39.Google Scholar
Jerger, S., Damian, M. F., Spence, M. J., Tye-Murray, N. & Abdi, H. (2009). Developmental shifts in children's sensitivity to visual speech: a new multimodal picture–word task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 102(1), 4059.Google Scholar
Jerger, S., Damian, M., Tye-Murray, N. & Abdi, H. (2014). Children use visual speech to compensate for non-intact auditory speech. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 126, 295312.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Conboy, B. T., Padden, D., Nelson, T. & Pruitt, J. (2005). Early speech perception and later language development: implications for the ‘critical period’. Language Learning and Development 1(3/4), 237–64.Google Scholar
Massaro, D. W. (1984). Children's perception of visual and auditory speech. Child Development 55, 1777–88.Google Scholar
McGurk, H. & MacDonald, J. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature 264, 746–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenblum, L. D., Schmuckler, M. A. & Johnson, J. A. (1997). The McGurk effect in infants. Perception & Psychophysics 59, 347–57.Google Scholar
Sekiyama, K. & Burnham, D. (2008). Impact of language on development of auditory-visual speech perception. Developmental Science 11(2), 306–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sekiyama, K. & Tohkura, Y. (1991). McGurk effect in non-English listeners: few visual effects for Japanese subjects hearing Japanese syllables of high auditory intelligibility. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 90(4), 1797–805.Google Scholar
Sekiyama, K. & Tohkura, Y. (1993). Inter-language differences in the influence of visual cues in speech perception. Journal of Phonetics 21, 427–44.Google Scholar
Sumby, W. H. & Pollack, I. (1954). Visual contribution to speech intelligibility in noise. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 26, 212–5.Google Scholar
Tsao, F. M., Liu, H. M. & Kuhl, P. K. (2004). Speech perception in infancy predicts language development in the second year of life: a longitudinal study. Child Development 75(4), 1067–84.Google Scholar
Vance, M., Rosen, S. & Coleman, M. (2009). Assessing speech perception in young children and relationships with language skills. International Journal of Audiology 48(10), 708–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weikum, W. M., Vouloumanos, A., Navarra, J., Soto-Faraco, S., Sebastian-Galles, N. & Werker, J. F. (2007). Visual language discrimination in infancy. Science 316(5828), 1159.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F. & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development 7(1), 4963.Google Scholar