Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-17T16:32:20.823Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Bids for joint attention by parent–child dyads and by dyads of young peers in interaction*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 February 2015

ANAT NINIO*
Affiliation:
Hebrew University of Jerusalem
*
Address for correspondence: Anat Ninio, Department of Psychology, The Hebrew University, Jerusalem 91905, Israel. e-mail: Anat.Ninio@huji.ac.il

Abstract

Before they are 3;0–3;6, children typically do not engage with peers in focused interaction, although they do with adults. With parents, children interact around the ‘here-and-now’. We hypothesize that young peers do not attempt to establish joint attention to present objects. Using the CHILDES database, we compared attention-directives produced by parents to children, children to peers, and children to parents. Of 391 English-speaking parents, 88% generated attention-directives, mostly Look!, See!, and Watch! Of 15 children (2;10–3;7) engaging in dyadic peer-interaction, only 26% produced such utterances. By comparison, 62% of 268 children (1;2–3;3) addressed such directives to parents. Interaction with peers in young children does not involve joint attention to a shared environmental focus, although it does with parents. The reason may be pragmatic: shared attention in parent–child dyads is a means to get information or help; it may seem pointless for a child to address such directives to a peer.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

Construction of the speech corpora and syntactic annotation were supported under Grant 200900206 to Anat Ninio by the Spencer Foundation. Parts of this research were presented at the 13th International Congress for the Study of Child Language, Amsterdam, the Netherlands, July 2014. I would like to thank two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments.

References

REFERENCES

Akhtar, N., Dunham, F. & Dunham, P. J. (1991). Directive interactions and early vocabulary development: the role of joint attentional focus. Journal of Child Language, 18, 41–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bakeman, R. & Adamson, L. B. (1984). Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother–infant and peer–infant interaction. Child Development, 55, 1278–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, E., Bretherton, I. &Snyder, L. (1988). From first words to grammar: individual differences and dissociable mechanisms. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bellinger, D. & Gleason, J. (1982). Sex differences in parental directives to young children. Journal of Sex Roles, 8, 1123–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernstein, N. (1982). Acoustic study of mothers' speech to language-learning children: an analysis of vowel articulatory characteristics. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Boston University.Google Scholar
Bernstein-Ratner, N. (1984). Patterns of vowel modification in motherese. Journal of Child Language, 11, 557–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bliss, L. (1988). The development of modals. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 9, 253–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, L. (1970). Language development: form and function in emerging grammars. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1973). One word at a time: the use of single-word utterances before syntax. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Hahn, C. S. & Haynes, O. M. (2008). Maternal responsiveness to young children at three ages: a longitudinal analysis of a multidimensional, modular and specific parenting construct. Developmental Psychology, 44, 867–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brent, M. R. & Siskind, J. M (2001). The role of exposure to isolated words in early vocabulary development. Cognition, 81, 3144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bronson, W. (1981). Toddlers’ behavior with agemates: issues of interaction, cognition, and affects (Monographs on Infancy, Vol. 1). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Brooks, P. J., Aldrich, N., Yuksel-Sokmen, O. & Ragir, S. (2014). Natural pedagogy in twin infants’ early communicative acts. Paper presented at the 13th International Congress for the Study of Child Language, Amsterdam, the Netherlands.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: the early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1982). The language of education. Social Research, 49, 835–53.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1983). Child's talk. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. & Sherwood, V. (1976). Early rule structure: the case of peekaboo. In Bruner, J. S., Jolly, A. & Sylva, K. (Eds), Play: its role in evolution and development (pp. 699701). London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Camaioni, L. & Laicardi, C. (1985). Early social games and the acquisition of language. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3, 31–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlson-Luden, V. (1979). Causal understanding in the 10-month-old. Unpublished PhD thesis, University of Colorado, Boulder.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1978). Discovering what words can do. In Farkas, D., Jacobsen, W. M. & Todrys, K. W. (Eds), Papers from the Parasession on the Lexicon, CLS 14 (pp. 3457). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Demetras, M. (1989a). Changes in parents’ conversational responses: a function of grammatical development. Paper presented at ASHA, St Louis, MO.Google Scholar
Demetras, M. (1989b). Working parents’ conversational responses to their two-year-old sons. Unpublished working paper. Tucson, University of Arizona.Google Scholar
Dickinson, D. K., Griffith, J. A., Golinkoff, R. M. & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2012). How reading books fosters language development around the world. Child Development Research, 2012, 115. Online http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2012/602807.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunham, P. J., Dunham, F. & Curwin, A. (1993). Joint-attentional states and lexical acquisition at 18 months. Developmental Psychology, 29, 827–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckerman, C. & Stein, M. (1982). The toddler's emerging interactive skills. In Rubin, K. H. & Ross, H. S. (Eds), Peer relations and social skills in childhood (pp. 4172). New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckerman, C. O., Whatley, J. L. & Kutz, S. L. (1975). Growth of social play with peers during the second year of life. Developmental Psychology, 11, 42–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, A. (1998). Constructing grammar: fillers, formulas, and function. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Colorado, Boulder.Google Scholar
Fischer, K. W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: the control and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychological Review, 87, 477531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fletcher, K. L., Perez, A., Hooper, C. & Claussen, A. H. (2005). Responsiveness and attention during picture-book reading in 18-month-old to 24-month-old toddlers at risk. Early Child Development and Care, 175, 6383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forrester, M. A. (1992). The development of young children's social-cognitive skills. Hove: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Francis, W. N. & Kučera, H. (1979). Brown corpus manual of information to accompany a standard corpus of present-day American English, revised and amplified. Providence, RI: Brown University, Department of Linguistics.Google Scholar
Garvey, C. (1976). Some properties of social play. In Bruner, J., Jolley, A. & Sylva, K. (eds), Play: its role in development and evolution (pp. 570–83). Middlesex: Penguin.Google Scholar
Garvey, C. & Hogan, R. (1973). Social speech and social interaction: egocentrism revisited. Child Development, 44, 562–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldfield, B. (1990). Pointing, naming, and talk about objects: referential behaviour in children and mothers. First Language, 10, 231–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, B. & Ross, H. (1978). Social skills in action. In Glick, J. & Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (Eds), Studies in social and cognitive development. Vol. 1: development of social understanding (pp. 177212). New York: Gardiner Press.Google Scholar
Goodman, J. C., Dale, P. S. & Li, P. (2008). Does frequency count? Parental input and the acquisition of vocabulary. Journal of Child Language, 35, 515–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, J. A., Gustafson, G. E. & West, M. J. (1980). Effects of infant development on mother–infant interactions. Child Development, 51, 199207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gustafson, G. E., Green, L. A. & West, M. J. (1979). The infant's changing role in mother–infant games: the growth of social skills. Infant Behavior and Development, 2, 301–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hay, D., Ross, H. & Davis, B. (1979). Social games in infancy. In Sutton-Smith, B. (Ed.), Play and learning (pp. 83107). New York: Gardiner Press.Google Scholar
Hayes, D. P. (2000). The Cornell Corpus. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University.Google Scholar
Higginson, R. P. (1985). Fixing-assimilation in language acquisition. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Washington State University.Google Scholar
Howe, C. (1981). Acquiring language in a conversational context. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M. & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27, 236–48CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaye, K. & Charney, R. (1980) How mothers maintain dialogue with two-year-olds. In Olson, D. R. (ed.), The social foundations of language and thought (pp. 211–30). New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Korman, M. (1984). Adaptive aspects of maternal vocalizations in differing contexts at ten weeks. First Language, 5, 44–5.Google Scholar
Kuczaj, S. (1976). -ing, -s and -ed: a study of the acquisition of certain verb inflections. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota.Google Scholar
Lee, J. N. & Naigles, L. R. (2005). Input to verb learning in Mandarin Chinese: a role for syntactic bootstrapping. Developmental Psychology, 41, 529–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieven, E. V. M. (1978). Conversations between mothers and young children: individual differences and their possible implications for the study of language learning. In Waterson, N. & Snow, C. (Eds), The development of communication (pp. 173–87). Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk, 3rd ed.Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. & Snow, C. (1985). The Child Language Data Exchange System. Journal of Child Language, 12, 271–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McMillan, J. (unpublished). Videos collected in Brian MacWhinney's course in psycholinguistics, 2004, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA.Google Scholar
Morisset, C. E. (1991). Environmental influences on language development of high social risk toddlers. Unpublished PhD thesis, University of Washington.Google Scholar
Ninio, A. (1980). The ostensive definition in vocabulary teaching. Journal of Child Language, 7, 565–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ninio, A. (1983). Joint bookreading as a multiple vocabulary acquisition device. Developmental Psychology, 19, 445–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ninio, A. (2011). Syntactic development, its input and output. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ninio, A. & Bruner, J. (1978). The achievement and antecedents of labelling. Journal of Child Language, 5, 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ninio, A. & Snow, E. C. (1996). Pragmatic development. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Ninio, A. & Wheeler, P. (1984). Functions of speech in mother–infant interaction: designing a coding scheme for the description and classification of verbal–social acts. In Feagans, L., Garvey, G. J. & Golinkoff, R. (Eds), The origins and growth of communication 196207. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Pan, B. A., Imbens-Bailey, A., Winner, K. & Snow, C. E. (1996). Communicative intents of parents interacting with their young children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 42, 248–66.Google Scholar
Parten, M. (1932). Social participation among preschool children. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 28, 136–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Post, K. (1992). The language learning environment of laterborns in a rural Florida community. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University.Google Scholar
Ratner, N. & Bruner, J. S. (1978). Games, social exchange and the acquisition of language. Journal of Child Language, 5, 391401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rollins, P. R. (2003). Caregivers’ contingent comments to 9-month-old infants: relationships with later language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 221–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rome-Flanders, T., Cossette, L., Ricard, M. & Gouin Decarie, T. (1995). Comprehension of rules and structures in mother–infant games: a longitudinal study of the first two years of life. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 18, 83103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rowland, C. F. & Fletcher, S. L. (2006). The effect of sampling on estimates of lexical specificity and error rates. Journal of Child Language, 33, 859–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sachs, J. (1983). Talking about the there and then: the emergence of displaced reference in parent–child discourse. In Nelson, K. E. (Ed.), Children's language, Vol. 4, (pp. 128). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Schaffer, H. R., Hepburn, A. & Collis, G. M. (1983). Verbal and nonverbal aspects of mothers’ directives. Journal of Child Language, 10, 337–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. E. (1977). The development of conversation between mothers and babies. Journal of Child Language, 4, 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1979). The role of social interaction in language acquisition. In Collins, W. A. (ed.), Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology 12 (pp. 157–82). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E., de Blauw, A. & van Roosmalen, G. (1978). Talking and playing with babies: the role of ideologies of child rearing. In Bullowa, M. (Ed.), Before speech (pp. 269–88). London: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E., Pan, B., Imbens-Bailey, A. & Herman, J. (1996). Learning how to say what one means: a longitudinal study of children's speech act use. Social Development, 5, 5684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suppes, P. (1974). The semantics of children's language. American Psychologist, 29, 103–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tardif, T., Gelman, S. A. & Xu, F. (1999). Putting the ‘Noun Bias’ in context: a comparison of English and Mandarin. Child Development, 70, 620–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E. V. M., Pine, J. M. & Rowland, C. F. (2001). The role of performance limitations in the acquisition of verb–argument structure: an alternative account. Journal of Child Language, 28, 127–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. & Farrar, M. J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57, 1454–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valian, V. (1991). Syntactic subjects in the early speech of American and Italian children. Cognition, 40, 2181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Houten, L. (1986). Role of maternal input in the acquisition process: the communicative strategies of adolescent and older mothers with their language learning children. Paper presented at the Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston.Google Scholar
Warren-Leubecker, A. (1982). Sex differences in speech to children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Georgia Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Warren-Leubecker, A. & Bohannon, J. N. (1984). Intonation patterns in child-directed speech: mother–father speech. Child Development, 55, 1379–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wells, C. G. (1981). Learning through interaction: the study of language development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, B. & Peters, A. M. (1988). What are you cookin’ on a hot? Movement constraints in the speech of a three-year-old blind child. Language, 64, 249–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, J. & Henry, A. (1998). Parameter setting within a socially realistic linguistics. Language in Society, 27, 121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, D. J., Bruner, J. S. & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal of Child Psychiatry and Psychology, 17, 89100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zamuner, T. S., Gerken, L. A. & Hammond, M. (2005). The acquisition of phonology based on input: a closer look at the relation of cross-linguistic and child language data. Lingua, 115, 1403–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar