No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Microscopic and macroscopic approaches to the mental representations of second languages
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 10 November 2017
Abstract
With a particular reference to second language (L2), we discuss (1) how structural priming can be used to tap into L2 representations and their relationships with first and target language representations; and (2) how complex networks additionally can be used to reveal the global and local patterning of L2 linguistic features and L2 developmental trajectories.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017
References
Borodkin, K., Kenett, Y. N., Faust, M. & Mashal, N. (2016) When pumpkin is closer to onion than to squash: The structure of the second language lexicon. Cognition
156:60–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cai, Z. G., Pickering, M. J. & Branigan, H. P. (2012) Mapping concepts to syntax: Evidence from structural priming in Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Memory and Language
66(4):833–49. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2012.03.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Čech, R. & Mačutek, J. (2009) Word form and lemma syntactic dependency networks in Czech: A comparative study. Glottometrics
19:85–98.Google Scholar
Chan, A. Y. (2004) Syntactic transfer: Evidence from the interlanguage of Hong Kong Chinese ESL learners. Modern Language Journal
88:56–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, B., Jia, Y., Wang, Z. & Dunlap, S. (2013) Is word-order similarity necessary for cross-linguistic structural priming?
Second Language Research
29:375–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H. & Felser, C. (2006) Grammatical processing in language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics
27(1):3–42. doi:10.1017/S0142716406060024.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cong, J. & Liu, H. (2014) Approaching human language with complex networks. Physics of Life Reviews
11(4):598–618.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corominas-Murtra, B., Valverde, S. & Solé, R. V. (2009) The ontogeny of scale-free syntax networks: Phase transitions in early language acquisition. Advances in Complex Systems
12:371–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eubank, L. (1996) Negation in early German-English interlanguage: More valueless features in the L2 initial state. Second Language Research
12(1):73–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferrer i Cancho, R., Solé, R. V. & Köhler, R. (2004) Patterns in syntactic dependency networks. Physical Review E, 69:051915.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartsuiker, R. J., Beerts, S., Loncke, M., Desmet, T. & Bernolet, S. (2016) Cross-linguistic structural priming in multilinguals: Further evidence for shared syntax. Journal of Memory and Language
90:14–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., Pickering, M. J. & Veltkamp, E. (2004) Is syntax separate or shared between languages? Cross-linguistic syntactic priming in Spanish-English bilinguals. Psychological Science
15(6):409–14. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00693.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ke, J. & Yao, Y. (2008) Analysing language development from a network approach. Journal of Quantitative Linguistics
15(1):70–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, H. (2009) Statistical properties of Chinese semantic networks. Chinese Science Bulletin
54:2781–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, H. & Li, W. W. (2010) Language clusters based on linguistic complex networks. Chinese Science Bulletin
55(30):3458–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonough, K. & Mackey, A. (2006) Responses to recasts: Repetitions, primed production, and linguistic development. Language Learning
56(4):693–720. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9922.2006.00393.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mehler, A., Lücking, A., Banisch, S., Blanchard, P. & Job, B. (2015) Towards a theoretical framework for analyzing complex linguistic networks. Springer.Google Scholar
Rowland, C. F., Chang, F., Ambridge, B., Pine, J. M. & Lieven, E. V. M. (2012) The development of abstract syntax: Evidence from structural priming and the lexical boost. Cognition
125(1):49–63. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.06.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siew, C. S. & Vitevitch, M. S. (2016) Spoken word recognition and serial recall of words from components in the phonological network. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
42(3):394.Google ScholarPubMed
Steyvers, M. & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2005) The largescale structure of semantic networks: Statistical analyses and a model of semantic growth. Cognitive Science
29(1):41–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2000) Do young children have adult syntactic competence?
Cognition
74(3):209–53. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00069-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Patten, B. (2014) The psycholinguistics of SLA. In: Research methods in second language psycholinguistics, ed. Jegerski, J. & VanPatten, B., pp. 1–19. Routledge.Google Scholar
Wilks, C. & Meara, P. (2002) Untangling word webs: Graph theory and the notion of density in second language word association networks. Second Language Research
18(4):303–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Target article
An experimental approach to linguistic representation
Related commentaries (30)
A usage-based cognitive linguistic (re-)interpretation of priming evidence
Acceptability judgments still matter: Deafness and documentation
Action sequences instead of representational levels
Can structural priming answer the important questions about language?
Considering experimental and observational evidence of priming together, syntax doesn't look so autonomous
Converging on a theory of language through multiple methods
Developmental psycholinguistics teaches us that we need multi-method, not single-method, approaches to the study of linguistic representation
Don't forget the neurobiology: An experimental approach to linguistic representation
Don't shoot the giant whose shoulders we are standing on
Horses for courses: When acceptability judgments are more suitable than structural priming (and vice versa)
If priming is graded rather than all-or-none, can reactivating abstract structures be the underlying mechanism?
Microscopic and macroscopic approaches to the mental representations of second languages
Moving beyond the priming of single-language sentences: A proposal for a comprehensive model to account for linguistic representation in bilinguals
On the nature of structure in structural priming
Priming is swell, but it's far from simple
Priming methods in semantics and pragmatics
Setting the empirical record straight: Acceptability judgments appear to be reliable, robust, and replicable
Structural priming can inform syntactic analyses of partially grammaticalized constructions
Structural priming is a useful but imperfect technique for studying all linguistic representations, including those of pragmatics
Structural priming is most useful when the conclusions are statistically robust
Structural priming is not a Royal Road to representations
Structural priming supports grammatical networks
Structural priming, action planning, and grammar
Syntactic levels, lexicalism, and ellipsis: The jury is still out
The limitations of structural priming are not the limits of linguistic theory
The logic of syntactic priming and acceptability judgments
The malleability of linguistic representations poses a challenge to the priming-based experimental approach
The relationship between priming and linguistic representations is mediated by processing constraints
The syntax of priming
What structural priming can and cannot reveal
Author response
Structural priming and the representation of language