Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-18T04:27:02.893Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Production and on-line comprehension of definite articles and clitic pronouns by Greek sequential bilingual children and monolingual children with specific language impairment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 May 2014

VASILIKI CHONDROGIANNI*
Affiliation:
Bangor University
THEODOROS MARINIS
Affiliation:
University of Reading
SUSAN EDWARDS
Affiliation:
University of Reading
ELMA BLOM
Affiliation:
University of Utrecht
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Vasiliki Chondrogianni, School of Linguistics and English Language, Bangor University, Bangor LL57 2DG, UK. E-mail: v.chondrogianni@bangor.ac.uk

Abstract

The present study compared production and on-line comprehension of definite articles and third person direct object clitic pronouns in Greek-speaking typically developing, sequential bilingual (L2-TD) children and monolingual children with specific language impairment (L1-SLI). Twenty Turkish Greek L2-TD children, 16 Greek L1-SLI children, and 31 L1-TD Greek children participated in a production task examining definite articles and clitic pronouns and, in an on-line comprehension task, involving grammatical sentences with definite articles and clitics and sentences with grammatical violations induced by omitted articles and clitics. The results showed that the L2-TD children were sensitive to the grammatical violations despite low production. In contrast, the children with SLI were not sensitive to clitic omission in the on-line task, despite high production. These results support a dissociation between production and on-line comprehension in L2 children and for impaired grammatical representations and lack of automaticity in children with SLI. They also suggest that on-line comprehension tasks may complement production tasks by differentiating between the language profiles of L2-TD children and children with SLI.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Alexiadou, A., & Stavrakaki, S. (2006). Clause structure and verb movement in a Greek English speaking bilingual patient with Broca's aphasia: Evidence from adverb placement, Brain & Language, 96, 207220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anagnostopoulou, E. (1999). Conditions on clitic doubling. In van Riemsdijk, R. H. (Ed.), Clitics in the languages of Europe (Vol. 3, pp. 759798). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Anagnostopoulou, E. (2003). The syntax of ditransitives: Evidence from clitics. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Arvaniti, A. (1991). The phonetics of Greek rhythm and its phonological implications. PhD dissertation, University of Cambridge.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G. (2008). Lexical access in bilinguals: Effects of vocabulary size and executive control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21, 522538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M., Carlyon, R. P., Deeks, J. M., & Bishop, S. J. (1999). Auditory temporal processing impairment: Neither necessary nor sufficient for causing language impairment in children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 12951310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blom, E., & Vasić, N. (2011). The production and processing of determiner–noun agreement in child L2 Dutch. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1, 265290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bošković, Ž. (2008). What will you have, DP or NP? Proceedings of NELS, 37, 101114.Google Scholar
Bottari, P., Cipriani, P., Chilosi, A. M., & Pfanner, L. (1998). The determiner system in a group of Italian children with SLI. Language Acquisition, 7, 285315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1995). The minimalist program, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chondrogianni, V. (2008a). The acquisition of determiners and clitic pronouns by child and adult L2 learners of Greek. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Cambridge.Google Scholar
Chondrogianni, V. (2008b). Comparing child and adult L2 acquisition of the Greek DP: Effects of age and construction. In Haznedar, B. & Gavruseva, E. (Eds.), Current trends in child L2 acquisition: Generative approaches (pp. 97142). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chondrogianni, V., & Marinis, T. (2012). Production and processing asymmetries in the acquisition of tense morphology by sequential bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 119.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H. (2008). Behavioral methods for investigating morphological and syntactic processing in children. In Sekerina, I. A., Fernández, E. M., & Clahsen, H. (Eds.), Developmental psycholinguistics: On-line methods in children's language processing (pp. 127). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamanti, M. (2000). Aples morphosyntaktikes domes sto logo pediou me eidiki glwssiki diataraxi. Paper presented at the 8th Symposium of the Panhellenic Association of Logopedists, Athens.Google Scholar
Enç, M. (1991). The semantics of specificity. Linguistic Inquiry, 22, 125.Google Scholar
Genesee, F., Paradis, J., & Crago, M. (2005). Domain-general versus domain-specific accounts of specific language impairment: Evidence from bilingual children's acquisition of object pronouns. Language Acquisition, 13, 3362.Google Scholar
Goad, H., & White, L. (2006). Ultimate attainment in interlanguage grammars: A prosodic approach. Second Language Research, 22, 243268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grüter, T. (2005). Comprehension and production of French object clitics by child second language learners and children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 363391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grüter, T., & Crago, M. (2012). Object clitics and their omission in child L2 French: The contributions of processing limitations and L1 transfer. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 531549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Håkansson, G., & Nettelbladt, U. (1996). Similarities between SLI and L2 children: Evidence from the acquisition of Swedish word order. In Johnson, C. E. & Gilbert, J. H. V. (Eds.), Children's language (pp. 135151). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Haznedar, B., & Schwartz, B. D. (1997). Are there optional infinitives in child L2 acquisition? In Hughes, E., Hughes, M., & Greenhill, A. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 21st Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 257268). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Institute for Language and Speech Processing. (2001). Basic vocabulary. Athens: Author. Retrieved from http://www.ilsp.gr/ippotis/greeklang.htmlGoogle Scholar
Ionin, T., & Wexler, K. (2002). Why is “is” easier than “-s”? Acquisition of tense/agreement morphology by child second language learners of English. Second Language Research, 18, 95136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jakubowicz, C., Nash, L., Rigaut, C., & Gerard, C.-L. (1998). Determiners and clitic pronouns in French-speaking children with SLI. Language Acquisition, 7, 113160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ketrez, N. (2005). Children's scope of indefinite objects. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Kornfilt, J. (1997). Turkish. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Kowaluk, A. (2001). The development of pronouns and determiners in the second language: A study of Polish learners of English. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Cambridge.Google Scholar
Lardiere, D. (2005). On morphological competence. In Dekydtspotter, L., Sprouse, R. A., & Liljestrand, A. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 7th Generative Approaches to Second Language Acquisition Conference (pp. 178192). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Lardiere, D. (2009). Some thoughts on the contrastive analysis of features in second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 25, 173227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L. B. (1989). Language learnability and specific language impairment in children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 10, 179202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L. B. (2009). Cross-linguistic studies of child language disorders. In Schwartz, R. (Ed.), Handbook of child language disorders (pp. 308324). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Leonard, L. B., Eyer, J. A., Bedore, L. M., & Grela, B. G. (1997). Three accounts of the grammatical morpheme difficulties of English-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 741753.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, L. B., Miller, C., & Finneran, D. (2009). Grammatical morpheme effects on sentence processing by school-aged adolescents with specific language impairment. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24, 450478.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Longobardi, G. (1994). Reference and proper names: A theory of N-movement in syntax and logical form. Linguistic Inquiry, 25, 609665.Google Scholar
Manika, S., Varlokosta, S., & Wexler, K. (2011). The lack of omission of clitics in Greek children with SLI: An experimental study. In Danis, N., Mesh, K., & Sung, H. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 429437). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Marinis, T. (2000). The acquisition of clitic objects in Modern Greek: Single clitics, clitic doubling, clitic left dislocation. In Alexiadou, A., Fuhrop, N., Kleinhenz, U., & Law, P. (Eds.), ZAS working papers 15 (pp. 260283). Berlin: Humboldt University.Google Scholar
Marinis, T. (2003). The acquisition of DP in Modern Greek. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marinis, T. (2008). Syntactic processing in developmental and acquired language disorders. In Ball, M. J., Perkins, M., Mueller, N., & Howard, S. (Eds.), The handbook of clinical linguistics (pp. 198211). Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marinis, T. (2010). Using on-line processing methods in language acquisition research. In Blom, E. & Unsworth, S. (Eds.), Experimental methods in language acquisition research (pp. 139162). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marinis, T., & Chondrogianni, V. (2011). Comprehension of reflexives and pronouns in sequential bilingual children: Do they pattern similarly to L1 children, L2 adults, or children with specific language impairment? Journal of Neurolinguistics, 24, 202212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marinis, T., & Saddy, D. (2013). Parsing the passive: Comparing children with Specific Language Impairment to sequential bilingual children. Language Acquisition 20, 155179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marinis, T., & van der Lely, H. (2007). Processing of wh-questions in children with G-SLI and typically developing children. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 42, 557582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mavrogiorgos, M. (2010). Clitics in Greek. A minimalist account of proclisis and enclisis. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mastropavlou, M. (2006). The effect of phonological saliency and LF-interpretability in the grammar of Greek normally developing and language impaired children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki.Google Scholar
Meisel, J. (2008). Child second language acquisition or successive first language acquisition? In Haznedar, B. & Gavruseva, E. (Eds.), Current trends in child second language acquisition (pp. 5580). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, J. W., & Leonard, L. B. (1998). Real-time inflectional processing by children with specific language impairment: Effects of phonetic substance. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 14321443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montgomery, J. W., & Leonard, L. B. (2006). Effects of acoustic manipulation on the real-time inflectional processing of children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 12381256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Öztürk, B. (2005). Case, referentiality and phrase structure in Turkish. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J. (2004). The relevance of specific language impairment in understanding the role of transfer in second language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 6776.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J. (2005). Grammatical morphology in children learning English as a second language: Implications of similarities with specific language impairment. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in the Schools, 36, 172187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J. (2010a). Bilingual children's acquisition of English verb morphology: Effects of language dominance, structure difficulty, and task type. Language Learning, 60, 651680.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J. (2010b). The interface between bilingual development and specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., & Crago, M. (2000). Tense and temporality: A comparison between children learning a second language and children with SLI. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 834847.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paradis, J., Rice, M. L., Crago, M., & Marquis, J. (2008). The acquisition of tense in English: Distinguishing child second language from first language and specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29, 689722.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paradis, M., & Gopnik, M. (1997). Compensatory strategies in genetic dysphasia: Declarative memory. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 10, 173185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prévost, P., & White, L. (2000). Missing surface inflection or impairment in second language acquisition? Evidence from tense and agreement. Second Language Research, 16, 103133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raven, J. C. (1998). The Coloured Progressive Matrices. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Schaeffer, J. (2000). The acquisition of direct object scrambling and clitic placement: Syntax and pragmatics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, W., Eschmann, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime user's guide. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B. D. (2003). Child L2 acquisition: Paving the way. In Beachley, B., Brown, A., & F. Conlin (Eds.), Proceedings of the 27th Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 2650). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Segalowitz, N., & Hulstijn, J. (2005). Automaticity in bilingualism and second language learning. In Kroll, J. & de Groot, A. M. B. (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism. psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 371388). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Seymour, H., Roeper, T., de Villiers, J., & de Villiers, P. (2005). Diagnostic Evaluation of Language Variation (DELV™): Norm-referenced. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.Google Scholar
Sioupi, A. (2002). On the semantic nature of bare NP's in Greek. Paper presented at the 5th International Conference on Greek Linguistics, Sorbonne, Paris.Google Scholar
Smith, N. (2008). Morphosyntactic and phonological short-term memory skills of Greek pre-school children with specific language impairment. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Reading.Google Scholar
Snedeker, J. (2009). Children's sentence processing. In Bavin, E. (Ed.), The handbook of child language (pp. 331338). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Stavrakaki, S. (2001). SLI in Greek: aspects of syntactic production and comprehension. Unpublished PhD dissertation, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki.Google Scholar
Stavrakaki, S., & Tsimpli, I. (2000). Diagnostic verbal IQ test for Greek preschool and school age children: Standardization, statistical analysis, psychometric properties. Paper presented at the 8th Symposium of the Panhellenic Association of Logopedists, Athens, Greece.Google Scholar
Stavrakaki, S., & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2010). Production and comprehension of pronouns by Greek children with specific language impairment. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28, 189216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trueswell, J. C., Sekerina, I. A., Hill, N., & Logrip, M. (1999). The kindergarten path effect: Studying on-line sentence processing in young children. Cognition, 73, 89134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsakali, V., & Wexler, K. (2003). Why do children omit clitics in some languages but not in others? In Kampen, J. v. & Baauw, S. (Eds.), Proceedings of the Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition Conference, 2003 (pp. 493504). Utrecht: LOT Publications.Google Scholar
Tsimpli, I. M. (2001). LF-interpretability and language development: A study of verbal and nominal features in normally developing and SLI Greek children. Brain and Language, 77, 432448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsimpli, I. M. (2003). Clitics and articles in L2 Greek. In Liceras, J. M., Zobl, H., & Goodluck, H. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 6th Generative Approaches to Second Language Acquisition Conference (pp. 331339). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Tsimpli, I. M. (2005). Peripheral positions in early Greek. In Stavrou, M. & Terzi, A. (Eds.), Advances in Greek generative syntax (pp. 179216). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsimpli, I. M., & Mastropavlou, M. (2008). Feature interpretability in L2 acquisition and SLI: Greek clitics and determiners. In Goodluck, H., Liceras, J., & Zobl, H. (Eds.), The role of formal features in second language acquisition (pp. 143183). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Tsimpli, I. M., & Papadopoulou, D. (2006). Aspect and argument realization: A study on antecedentless null objects in Greek. Lingua, 116, 15951615.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsimpli, I. M., & Stavrakaki, S. (1999). The effects of a morphosyntactic deficit in the determiner system: The case of a Greek SLI child. Lingua, 108, 3185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ullman, M. T., & Gopnik, M. (1999). Inflectional morphology in a family with inherited specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 51117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van der Lely, H. K. J., & Battell, J. (2003). Wh-movement in children with grammatical SLI: A test of the RDDR hypothesis. Language, 79, 153181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
VanPatten, B., Keathing, G., & Leeser, M. (2012). Missing verbal inflections as a representational problem. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 2, 109140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Varlokosta, S. (2002). Functional categories in Greek specific language impairment. In Clairis, C. (Ed.), Recherches en linguistique grecque (pp. 303306). Paris: L'Harmattan.Google Scholar
Vasić, N., & Blom, E. (2011). Production and processing of determiners in Turkish–Dutch child L2 learners. In Danis, N., Mesh, K., & Sung, H. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 616627). Somervillle, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Vasić, N., Chondrogianni, V., Marinis, T., & Blom, E. (2012). Processing of gender in Turkish–Dutch and Turkish–Greek L2 children. In Biller, A. K., Chung, E. Y., & Kimball, A. E. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 36th Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 646659). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Windsor, J., & Kohnert, K. (2004). The search for common ground: Part I. lexical performance by linguistically diverse learners. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 47, 877890.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zdorenko, T., & Paradis, J. (2011). Articles in child L2 English: When L1 and L2 acquisition meet at the interface. First Language, 21, 125.Google Scholar