Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-25wd4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T17:21:41.965Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Linguistic precocity and the development of reading: The role of extralinguistic factors

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Philip S. Dale*
Affiliation:
University of Washington
Catherine Crain-Thoreson
Affiliation:
Western Washington University
Nancy M. Robinson
Affiliation:
University of Washington
*
Philip S. Dale, Department of Psychology NI-25, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195

Abstract

The language and literacy skills of 21 children (aged 6;6), who were selected for linguistic precocity at age 1;8, are reported here. Verbal abilities remained high, and in contrast to the findings at 4;6 (reported in Crain-Thoreson & Dale, 1992), reading achievement is now at a superior level. Overall, the results are consistent with a two-phase model of reading development, in which the second phase is more closely related to language ability than the first. Phonological awareness, as indexed by a phoneme deletion task, appears to emerge as a consequence, rather than a cause, of early reading. There also appears to be a complex relationship among early interest in reading, instruction, and reading development. Differences in child interest in books and book reading may evoke variation in literacy-relevant experiences.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Baron, J. (1979). Orthographic and word-specific mechanisms in children's reading of words. Child Development, 50, 6072.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowey, J. A., & Francis, J. (1991). Phonological analysis as a function of age and exposure to reading instruction. Applied Psycholinguistics. 12, 91121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowey, J. A., & Patel, R. K. (1988). Metalinguistic ability and early reading achievement. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 367383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryant, P., MacLean, M., & Bradley, L. (1990). Rhyme, language, and children's reading. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 237252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chall, J. S. (1983). Stages of reading development. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Chall, J. S., Jacobs, V. A., & Baldwin, L. E. (1990). The reading crisis: Why poor children fall behind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clay, M. M. (1972). The early detection of reading difficulties: A diagnostic survey with recovery procedures. Auckland, New Zealand: Heinemann.Google Scholar
Crain-Thoreson, C., & Dale, P. S. (1992). Do early talkers become early readers? Linguistic precocity, preschool language, and early reading. Developmental Psychology, 28, 421429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, H. (1991). The unschooled mind. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Goswami, U., & Bryant, P. (1990). Phonological skills and learning to read. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hoffman, L. W. (1991). The influence of the family environment on personality: Accounting for sibling differences. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 187203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, N. E. (1992). Precocious reading of English: Origins, structure, and predictive significance. In Klein, P. & Tannenbaum, A. (Eds.), To be young and gifted (pp. 171203). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Kamhi, A., & Catts, H. W. (1989). Reading disabilities: A developmental language perspective. Boston: College Hill Publications.Google Scholar
Kirtley, C., Bryant, P., MacLean, M., & Bradley, L. (1989). Rhyme, rime, and the onset of reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 48, 224245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberman, I. Y., & Liberman, A. M. (1990). Language versus code emphasis: Underlying assumptions and their implications for reading instruction. Annals of Dyslexia, 40, 5166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mills, J. R., & Jackson, N. E. (1990). Predictive significance of early giftedness: The case of precocious reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 410419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morais, J., Cary, L., Alegria, J., & Bertelson, P. (1979). Does awareness of speech as a sequence of phones arise spontaneously? Cognition, 7, 323331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., Beck, I., Bell, L. C., & Hughes, C. (1988). Phonemic knowledge and learning to read are reciprocal: A longitudinal study of first grade children. In Stanovich, K. E. (Ed.), Children's reading and development of phonological awareness (pp. 3975). Detroit, MI: Wayne State University Press.Google Scholar
Plomin, R., & Daniels, D. (1987). Why are children within the family so different from each other? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 10, 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, N. M., Dale, P. S., & Landesman, S. (1990). Validity of Stanford–Binet IV with linguistically precocious toddlers. Intelligence, 14, 173186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosner, J., & Simon, D. P. (1971). The auditory analysis test: An initial report. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 4, 384392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scarborough, H. S., & Dobrich, W. (1994). On the efficacy of reading to preschoolers. Developmental Review, 14, 245302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. (1987). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals – Revised. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Shankweiler, D., & Crain, S. (1986). Language mechanisms and reader disorder: A modular approach. Cognition, 24, 139168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steiger, J. H. (1980). Tests for comparing elements of a correlation matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 87, 245251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, B. (1984). Early toy preferences of four-year-old readers and nonreaders. Child Development, 55, 424430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorndike, R. L., Hagen, E. P., & Sattler, J. M. (1986). Technical manual, Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scale (4th ed.). Chicago: Riverside Publishing.Google Scholar
Treiman, R. (1991). Phonological awareness and its roles in learning to read and spell. In Sawyer, D. J. & Fox, B. J. (Eds.), Phonological awareness in reading: The evolution of current perspectives (pp. 158189). New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Vellutino, F. (1987). Dyslexia. Scientific American, 256 (3), 3441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodcock, R. W., & Johnson, M. B. (1989). Woodcock-Johnson Test of Achievement, Revised. Allen, TX: DLM Teaching Research.Google Scholar