Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T10:35:47.574Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Learning styles and perceptual patterns for English /i/ and /ɪ/ among Chinese college students

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 May 2015

XIAOHU YANG*
Affiliation:
Shanghai Jiao Tong University
FENG SHI
Affiliation:
Nankai University
XIANGJUN LIU
Affiliation:
Shanghai University of Finance and Economics
YONG ZHAO
Affiliation:
Shanghai Jiao Tong University
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Xiaohu Yang, School of Foreign Languages, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, 800 Dongchuan Road, Minhang District, Shanghai 200240, People's Republic of China. E-mail: sherwoodyxh@gmail.com

Abstract

This study examined whether learning styles, along with age of starting English learning and length of English learning, are related to perceptual patterns for English /i/–/ɪ/ among Chinese college students who learn English as a foreign language. A total of 83 Chinese college students with different learning styles as measured by Kolb's Learning Style Inventory (1985) and 16 native speakers of American English identified the vowels in a synthetic beat–bit continuum. The results revealed that the Chinese participants’ perceptual patterns for English /i/–/ɪ/ varied with their learning styles. The participants with Kolb's (1985) assimilative and divergent learning styles were more likely to exhibit perceptual patterns resembling those of the American participants than were the participants with convergent and accommodative learning styles. Furthermore, of Kolb's four learning modes, reflective observation had a facilitative effect on the participants’ perception, whereas active experimentation was more likely to cause difficulties; abstract conceptualization and concrete experience bore little relation to the perception of these two sounds. In addition, length of English learning played a critical part in the development of English /i/–/ɪ/ perception. However, age of starting English learning in foreign language conditions was not as crucial as suggested by earlier studies on speech perception in second language conditions.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Abrahamsson, N., & Hyltenstam, K. (2009). Age of onset and nativelikeness in a second language: Listener perception versus linguistic scrutiny. Language Learning, 59, 249306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andreou, E., Andreou, G., & Vlachos, F. (2008). Learning styles and performance in second language tasks. TESOL Quarterly, 42, 665674.Google Scholar
Baker, W. (2008). Social, experiential and psychological factors affecting L2 dialect acquisition. In Bowles, M., Foote, R., Perpinán, S., & Bhatt, R. (Eds.), Selected proceedings of the 2007 Second Language Research Forum (pp. 187198). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Baker, W., Trofimovich, P., Mack, M., & Flege, J. E. (2002). The effect of perceived phonetic similarity on non-native sound learning by children and adults. In Skarabela, B., Fish, S., & Do, A. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 26th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 3647). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Best, C. T., & Strange, W. (1992). Effects of phonological and phonetic factors on cross-language perception of approximants. Journal of Phonetics, 20, 305330.Google Scholar
Best, C. T., & Tyler, M. D. (2007). Nonnative and second-language speech perception: Commonalities and complementarities. In Bohn, O. S. & Munro, M. (Eds.), Language experience in second language speech learning: In honor of James Emil Flege (pp. 1334). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bion, R. A. H., Escudero, P., Rauber, A. S., & Baptista, B. O. (2006). Category formation and the role of spectral quality in the perception and production of English front vowels. Paper presented at Interspeech 2006 ICSLP.Google Scholar
Birdsong, D. (2007). Nativelike pronunciation among late learners of French as a second language. In Bohn, O. S. & Munro, M. (Eds.), Language experience in second language speech learning: In honor of James Emil Flege (pp. 99116). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bohn, O.-S. (1995). Cross language speech production in adults: First language transfer doesn't tell it all. In Strange, W. (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: Issues in cross-language research (pp. 279304). Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Bohn, O.-S., & Best, C. T. (2012). Native-language phonetic and phonological influences on perception of American English approximants by Danish and German listeners. Journal of Phonetics, 40, 109128.Google Scholar
Bohn, O.-S., & Flege, J. E. (1990a). Perception and production of a new vowel category by adult second language learners. In Leather, J. & James, A. (Eds.), New sounds 90: Proceedings of the 1990 Amsterdam Symposium on the Acquisition of Second-Language Speech (pp. 3756). Amsterdam: University of Amsterdam Press.Google Scholar
Bohn, O.-S., & Flege, J. E. (1990b). Interlingual identification and the role of foreign language experience in L2 vowel perception. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 303328.Google Scholar
Boud, D., Keogh, R., & Walker, D. (Eds.). (1985). Reflection: Turning experience into learning. New York: Nichols.Google Scholar
Castro, O., & Peck, V. (2005). Learning styles and foreign language learning difficulties. Foreign Language Annals, 38, 401409.Google Scholar
Chang, C.-C., Tseng, K.-H., & Tseng, J.-S. (2011). Is single or dual channel with different English proficiencies better for English listening comprehension, cognitive load and attitude in ubiquitous learning environment? Computers & Education, 57, 23132321.Google Scholar
Chermahini, S. A., Ghanbari, A., & Ghanbari Talab, M. (2013). Learning styles and academic performance of students in English as a second-Language class in Iran. Bulgarian Journal of Science and Education Policy, 7, 322333.Google Scholar
De Angelis, G. (2007). Third or additional language acquisition. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1933). How we think: A restatement of the relation of reflective thinking to the educative process. Boston: Heath.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z. (2005). The psychology of the language learner: Individual differences in second language acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Driscoll, M. E. (1994). Psychology of learning for instruction. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Ehrman, M. E. (1996). Understanding second language difficulties. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Elliott, R. E. (1995). Field independence/dependence, hemispheric specialization, and attitude in relation to pronunciation accuracy in Spanish as a foreign language. Modern Language Journal, 79, 356371.Google Scholar
Ertmer, P. A., & Nweby, T. J. (1996). The expert learner: Strategic, self-regulated, and reflective. Instructional Science, 24, 124.Google Scholar
Escudero, P. (2000). Developmental patterns in the adult L2 acquisition of new contrasts: The acoustic cue weighting in the perception of Scottish tense/lax vowels by Spanish speakers. Unpublished master's thesis, University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
Escudero, P. (2001). The role of the input in the development of L1 and L2 sound contrasts: Language-specific cue weighting for vowels. In Do, A. H.-J., Dominguez, L., & Johansen, A. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 25th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 50261). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Escudero, P. (2006). The phonological and phonetic development of new vowel contrasts in Spanish learners of English. In Baptista, B. O. & Watkins, M. A. (Eds.), English with a Latin beat: Studies in Portuguese/Spanish–English interphonology (pp. 149161). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Escudero, P., Benders, T., & Lipski, S. C. (2009). Native, non-native and L2 perceptual cue weighting for Dutch vowels: The case of Dutch, German, and Spanish listeners. Journal of Phonetics, 37, 452465.Google Scholar
Escudero, P., & Boersma, P. (2004). Bridging the gap between L2 speech perception research and phonological theory. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26, 551585.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E. (1995). Second-language speech learning: Theory, findings, and problems. In Strange, W. (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: Theoretical and methodological issues (pp. 229273). Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E., Bohn, O.-S., & Jang, S. (1997). Effects of experience on non-native speakers’ production and perception. Journal of Phonetics, 25, 437470.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E., & MacKay, I. R. A. (2004). Perceiving vowels in a second language. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26, 134.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E., MacKay, I. R. A., & Meador, D. (1999). Native Italian speakers’ perception and production of English vowels. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 106, 29732987.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E., Munro, M. J., & MacKay, I. R. A. (1995). Factors affecting strength of perceived foreign accent in a second language. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97, 31253134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fullana, N. (2006). The development of English (FL) perception and production skills: Starting age and exposure effects. In Muñoz, C. (Ed.), Age and the rate of foreign language learning (pp. 4164). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Furnham, A. (1992). Personality and learning style: A study of three instruments. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 429438.Google Scholar
Gottfried, T., & Beddor, P. (1988). Perception of temporal and spectral information in French vowels. Language and Speech, 31, 5775.Google Scholar
Healey, M., & Jenkins, A. (2000). Kolb's experiential learning theory and its application in geography in higher education. Journal of Geography, 99, 185195.Google Scholar
Hillenbrand, J., Clark, M. J., & Houde, R. A. (2000). Some effects of duration on vowel recognition. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 108, 30133022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hillenbrand, J., & Gayvert, R. (2005). Open source software for experiment design and control. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 4560.Google Scholar
Honey, P., & Mumford, A. (1982). The manual of learning styles. Maidenhead: Honey Press.Google Scholar
Howie, J. (1976). Acoustical studies of Mandarin vowels and tones. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ingvalson, E. M., Ettlinger, M., & Wong, P. C. M. (2014). Bilingual speech perception and learning: A review of recent trends. International Journal of Bilingualism, 18, 3547.Google Scholar
Jia, J., Strange, W., Wu, Y., Collado, J., & Guan, Q. (2006). Perception and production of English vowels by Mandarin speakers: Age-related differences vary with amount of L2 exposure. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119, 11181130.Google Scholar
Joy, S., & Kolb, D. (2009). Are there cultural differences in learning style? International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 33, 6985.Google Scholar
Kim, S., & Kim, M. (2012). Kolb's learning styles and educational outcome: Using Digital Mind Map as a study tool in Elementary English class. International Journal for Educational Media and Technology, 6, 413.Google Scholar
Klatt, D. H. (1980). Software for a cascade/parallel formant synthesizer. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 67, 971995.Google Scholar
Kolb, D. A. (1981). Learning styles and disciplinary differences. In Chickering, A. W. & Associates (Eds.), The modern American college (pp. 232255). San Francisco, CA: Jossey–Bass.Google Scholar
Kolb, D. A. (1984). Experiential learning: Experience as the source of learning and development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice–Hall.Google Scholar
Kolb, D. A. (1985). Learning style inventory and technical manual. Boston: McBer & Company.Google Scholar
Kolb, A. Y., & Kolb, D. A. (2008). The learning way: Meta-cognitive aspects of experiential learning. Simulation & Gaming, 40, 297327.Google Scholar
Kolb, D. A., Boyatzis, R. E., & Mainemelis, C. (2001). Experiential learning theory: Previous research and new directions. In Sternberg, R. J. & Zhang, L.-F. (Eds.), Perspectives on thinking, learning, and cognitive styles (pp. 227247). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kondaurova, M. V., & Francis, A. L. (2008). The relationship between native allophonic experience with vowel duration and perception of the English tense/lax vowel contrast by Spanish and Russian listeners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 124, 39593971.Google Scholar
Ladefoged, P. (2001). A course in phonetics (4th ed.). New York: Harcourt College.Google Scholar
Lin, H.-B., & Repp, B. (1989). Cues to the perception of Taiwanese tones. Language and Speech, 32, 2544.Google Scholar
MacKay, I. R. A., Meador, D., & Flege, J. E. (2001). The identification of English consonants by native speakers of Italian. Phonetica, 58, 103125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maddox, W. T., & Chandrasekaran, B. (2014). Tests of a dual-system model of speech category learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 709728.Google Scholar
Marinova-Todd, S. H., Marshall, D. B., & Snow, C. E. (2000). Three misconceptions about age and L2 learning. TESOL Quarterly, 34, 934.Google Scholar
Metallidou, P., & Platisidou, M. (2008). Kolb's learning style Inventory—1985: Validity issues and relations with metacognitive knowledge about problem-solving strategies. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 114119.Google Scholar
Morrison, G. S. (2005). An appropriate metric for cue weighting in L2 speech perception: Response to Escudero & Boersma, 2004. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 597606.Google Scholar
Morrison, G. S. (2006). Methodological issues in L2 perception research and vowel spectral cues in Spanish listeners’ perception of word-final /t/ and /d/ in Spanish. In Díaz-Campos, M. (Ed.), Selected proceedings of the 2nd Conference on Laboratory Approaches to Spanish Phonetics and Phonology (pp. 3547). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Morrison, G. S. (2008). L1-Spanish speakers’ acquisition of the English /i/-/ɪ/ contrast: Duration-based perception is not the initial development stage. Language & Speech, 51, 285315.Google Scholar
Moyer, A. (2004). Age, accent and experience in second language acquisition: An integrated approach to critical period inquiry. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Moyer, A. (2007). Do language attitudes determine accent? A study of bilinguals in the USA. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 28, 502518.Google Scholar
Muñoz, C. (2006). The effects of age on foreign language learning: The BAF project. In Muñoz, C. (Ed.), Age and the rate of foreign language learning (pp. 140). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Muñoz, C. (2008). Symmetries and asymmetries of age effects in naturalistic and instructed L2 learning. Applied Linguistics, 29, 578596.Google Scholar
Passarelli, A. M., & Kolb, D. A. (2011). The learning way: Learning from experience as the path to lifelong learning and development. In London, M. (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of lifelong learning (pp. 7090). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Passarelli, A. M., & Kolb, D. A. (2012). Using experiential learning theory to promote student learning and development in programs of education abroad. In Vande Berg, M., Paige, R. M., & Lou, K. H. (Eds.), Student learning abroad: What our students are learning, what they’re not, and what we can do about it (pp. 137161). Sterling, VA: Stylus.Google Scholar
Patkowski, M. (2003). Laterality effects in multilinguals during speech production under the concurrent task paradigm: Another test of the age of acquisition. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 41, 175200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piske, T., MacKay, I. R. A., & Flege, J. E. (2001). Factors affecting degree of foreign accent in an L2: A review. Journal of Phonetics, 29, 191215.Google Scholar
Reid, J. M. (1995). Learning styles in the ESL/EFL classroom. Boston: Heinle & Heinle.Google Scholar
Salazar Noguera, J., & Wageman, J. (2011). Spanish EFL undergraduate students’ perceptions of learning styles. Nordic Journal of English Studies, 10, 77101.Google Scholar
Skehan, P. (1991). Individual difference in second language learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 13, 275298.Google Scholar
Slevc, L. R., & Miyake, A. (2006). Individual differences in second-language proficiency: Does musical ability matter? Psychological Science, 17, 675681.Google Scholar
Smith, B. L., & Hayes-Harb, R. (2011). Individual differences in the perception of final consonant voicing among native and non-native speakers of English. Journal of Phonetics, 39, 115120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, D. M., & Kolb, D. A. (1986). User's guide for the Learning Style Inventory. Boston: McBer & Company.Google Scholar
Smith, R. M. (1991). How people become effective learners. Adult Learning, 2, 1113.Google Scholar
Thompson, I. (1991). Foreign accents revisited: The English pronunciation of Russian immigrants. Language Learning, 41, 177204.Google Scholar
Tsukada, K., Birdsong, D., Bialystok, E., Mack, M., Sung, H., & Flege, J. E. (2005). A developmental study of English vowel production and perception by native Korean adults and children. Journal of Phonetics, 33, 263290.Google Scholar
Wang, X. (1998). Effects of first language on native Mandarin speakers’ perception of English vowels. Paper presented at the 14th Northwestern Linguistics Conference, March 7–8, Simon Fraser University.Google Scholar
Wang, X. (2002). Training Mandarin and Cantonese speakers to identify English vowel contrasts: Long-term retention and effects on production. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Simon Fraser University.Google Scholar
Wang, D., & Dai, W. D. (2013). The correlation of learning styles and performance in second language tasks: An experimental study based on Kolb's learning style theory. Foreign Language World, 154, 2835.Google Scholar
Yaha, I. (1998). Willcoxson and Prosser's factor analysis on Kolb's (1985) LSI data: Reflections and re-analysis. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 68, 281286.Google Scholar
Zhang, J.-S. (2002). A comparative study of English and Chinese vowels and its pedagogical significance. Journal of PLA University of Foreign Languages, 25, 5659.Google Scholar