Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T15:30:11.458Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

14 - Is It Reasonable to Be Creative?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 January 2010

Jacques-Henri Guignard
Affiliation:
Institute de Psychologie, Laboratoire Cognition et Développement, Université René Descartes, Paris
Todd Lubart
Affiliation:
Laboratoire Cognition et Développement, UMR 8605, Université René Descartes, Paris
James C. Kaufman
Affiliation:
California State University, San Bernardino
John Baer
Affiliation:
Rider University, New Jersey
Get access

Summary

Reasoning refers, in general, to the act of thinking based on inductive and deductive processes. It is often associated, more specifically, with the process of developing and supporting assertions, claims, proposals, or conclusions (Reber, 1995). It provides, in many cases, the basis for action or decisions. The development of reasoning is often viewed as an important educational goal. In this chapter, we focus on the relation between reasoning and another aspect of cognition that has been gaining increasing attention, namely creativity.

Creativity can be defined as the capacity to produce novel, original work that fits within task constraints (Lubart, 1994). Work refers to all types of ideas and productions. This work must be novel in the sense that it goes beyond a replication or copy of that which exists. The extent to which the work produced is novel can vary from being original only for the person who completed the work (this is the notion of reinventing ideas known already in the larger social context) to being original for a limited social group to being original for all of humanity. The second component in the definition concerns the fit with constraints. We distinguish creative ideas from bizarre ideas, which are also novel, because creative ideas take into account the parameters of a situation, the constraints. Novel productions that are in some way “useful” or “reasonable” in a given context are, thus, creative.

We explore possible links between the development of reasoning and the development of creativity in children and adults.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Amabile, T. M. (1982). Social psychology of creativity: A consensual assessment technique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43(5), 997–1013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arieti, S. (1976). Creativity: The magic synthesis. Oxford, UK: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Arlin, P. K. (1990). Wisdom: The art of problem finding. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and development (pp. 230–243). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Basseches, M. (1984). Dialectical thinking and adult development. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Beckerman, M. B. (1990). Leos Janacek and “the late style” in music. The gerontologist, 30, 632–635.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benack, S., Basseches, M., & Swan, T. (1989). Perspectives on individual differences. In Ronning, R. R. & Glover, J. A. (Eds.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 199–208). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Chi, M. (1997). Creativity: Shifting across ontological categories flexibly. In Smith, S., & Ward, T. (Eds.) Creative thought: An investigation of conceptual structures and processes (pp. 209–234). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Cohen-Shalev, A. (1986). Artistic creativity across the adult life span: An alternative approach. Interchange, 17(4), 1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen-Shalev, A. (1989). Old age style: Developmental changes in creative production from a life-span perspective. Journal of Aging Studies, 3(1), 21–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dudek, S. Z., & Coté, R. (1994). Problem finding revisited. In Runco, M. A. (Ed.), Problem finding, problem solving, and creativity. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Duncker, K. (1945). On problem solving. Psychological Monographs, 58(5).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, H. (1993). Creating minds. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gardner, H., Phelps, E., & Wolf, D. P. (1990). The roots of adult creativity in children's symbolic products. In Langern, E. J. & Alexander, C. N. (Ed.). Higher tages of human development: Perspective on adult growth. (pp. 79–96). London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Georgsdottir, A. S., Ameel, E., & Lubart, T. I. (2002). Cognitive flexibility and logical reasoning in school aged children. Paper presented at the 17th Biennial Meeting of the International Society of Behavoir Development (ISSBD), Ottawa, Ontario, Canada.
Guignard, J. H. & Lubart, T. I. (2005). An empirical study of creative thinking skills in intellectually gifted students. Manuscript in preparation.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1994). Precis of beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17(4), 693–745.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koestler, A. (1964). The act of creation. Oxford, UK: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Kramer, D. A. (1990). Conceptualizing wisdom: the primacy of affect-cognition relations. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and development (pp. 279–313). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labouvie-Vief, G. (1985). Intelligence and cognition. In Schaie, K. W. & Birren, J. E. (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.Google Scholar
Lehman, H. C. (1953). Age and achievment. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Light, P., Blaye, A., Gilly, M., & Girotto, V. (1989). Pragmatic schemas and logical reasoning in 6- to 8-year-old children. Cognitive Development, 4(1), 49–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lubart, T. I. (1994). Creativity. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Thinking and problem solving (pp. 289–332). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Lubart, T. I., & Lautrey, J. (1996). Development of creativity in 9- to 10-year old children. Paper presented at the Growing Mind Congress, Genève, Suisse.
Lubart, T. I., Mouchiroud, C., Torjman, S., & Zenasni, F. (2004). Psychologie de la créativité [Psychology of creativity]. Paris: Armand Colin.Google Scholar
Lubart, T. I., & Sternberg, R. J. (1998). Life span creativity: An investment theory approach. In Adam-Price, C. (Ed.), Creativity and successful aging: Theoretical and empirical approach (pp. 21–41). New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Orwoll, L., & Perlmutter, M. (1990). The study of wise persons: Integrating a personality perspective. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and development (pp. 160–177). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pascual-Leone, J. (1990). An essay on wisdom: Toward organismic processes that make it possible. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and development (pp. 244–278). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reber, A. S. (1995). The Penguin dictionary of psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Ribot, T. (1926). Essai sur imagination créatrice [Essay on the creative imagination]. Paris: Alcan.Google Scholar
Rieben, L. (1982). Processus secondaire et créativité: Partie émegée de l'iceberg? In Nicolaïdis, N. & Schmid-Kitsikis, E. (Eds.), Créativité et/ou symptome (pp. 97–111). Paris: Clancier-Guenand.Google Scholar
Riegel, K. F. (1975). Toward a dialectic theory of development. Human Development, 18(1–2), 50–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenblatt, E., & Winner, E. (1988). The art of children's drawing. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 22(1), 3–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothenberg, A. (1990). Creativity in Adolescence. Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 13(3), 415–434.Google ScholarPubMed
Simonton, D. K. (1975). Interdisciplinary creativity over historical time: A correlational analysis of generational fluctuation. Social Behavior and Personality, 3(2), 181–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1989). The swan-song phenomenon: Last-works effects for 172 classical composers. Psychology and Aging, 4, 42–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torrance, E. P. (1968). A longitudinal examination of the fourth-grade slump in creativity. Gifted Child Quarterly, 12, 195–199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wyatt-Brown, A. M. (1998). Late style in the novels of Barbara Pym and Penelope Mortimer. The Gerontologist, 28, 835–839.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Is It Reasonable to Be Creative?
    • By Jacques-Henri Guignard, Institute de Psychologie, Laboratoire Cognition et Développement, Université René Descartes, Paris, Todd Lubart, Laboratoire Cognition et Développement, UMR 8605, Université René Descartes, Paris
  • Edited by James C. Kaufman, California State University, San Bernardino, John Baer, Rider University, New Jersey
  • Book: Creativity and Reason in Cognitive Development
  • Online publication: 19 January 2010
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511606915.016
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Is It Reasonable to Be Creative?
    • By Jacques-Henri Guignard, Institute de Psychologie, Laboratoire Cognition et Développement, Université René Descartes, Paris, Todd Lubart, Laboratoire Cognition et Développement, UMR 8605, Université René Descartes, Paris
  • Edited by James C. Kaufman, California State University, San Bernardino, John Baer, Rider University, New Jersey
  • Book: Creativity and Reason in Cognitive Development
  • Online publication: 19 January 2010
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511606915.016
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Is It Reasonable to Be Creative?
    • By Jacques-Henri Guignard, Institute de Psychologie, Laboratoire Cognition et Développement, Université René Descartes, Paris, Todd Lubart, Laboratoire Cognition et Développement, UMR 8605, Université René Descartes, Paris
  • Edited by James C. Kaufman, California State University, San Bernardino, John Baer, Rider University, New Jersey
  • Book: Creativity and Reason in Cognitive Development
  • Online publication: 19 January 2010
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511606915.016
Available formats
×