Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-17T18:11:39.578Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Perceptual perspective-taking and seriation abilities in high-functioning children with autism

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 October 2008

Nurit Yirmiya*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology School of Education, Hebrew University of Jerusalem
Marian Sigman
Affiliation:
Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles
Daphna Zacks
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Nurit Yirmiya, Department of Psychology, Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Mount Scopus, Jerusalem, Israel 91905.

Abstract

This study compared a group of 18 high-functioning children with autism (9–16 years of age) and a group of 14 normally developing children (9–14 years of age) on their abilities to take the perceptual perspective of someone else and to seriate objects based on length, size, weight, and color. Results indicated that the high-functioning children with autism were as able as the normally developing children to seriate the various objects. In contrast, significant differences emerged on the perceptual perspective-taking tasks with the high-functioning children with autism as a group performing less well than the normally developing children. However, the majority of autistic children showed good perspective-taking ability. The implications of these results to our understanding of autism are discussed.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1994

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association. (1981). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989a). The autistic child's “theory of mind”: A case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 285297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989b). Perceptual role taking and protodeclarative pointing in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 113127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1991). The development of a theory of mind in autism: Deviance and delay? Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 14, 3351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S. (1992). Debate and argument: On modality and development in autism: A reply to Burack. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 623629.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a ‘theory of mind’? Cognition, 21, 3746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1986). Mechanical, behavioral and intentional understanding of picture stories in autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 113125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Capps, L., Yirmiya, N., & Sigman, M. D. (1992). Understanding of simple and complex emotions in non-retarded children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 11691182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charman, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1992). Understanding drawings and beliefs: A further test of the metarepresentation theory of autism: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 11051112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawson, G., & Fernald, M. (1987). Perspective-taking ability and its relationship to the social behavior of autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmetal Disorders, 17, 487498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flavell, J. H., Everett, B. A., Croft, K., & Flavell, E. R. (1981). Young children's knowledge about visual perception: Further evidence for the Level 1 Level 2 distinction. Developmental Psychology, 17, 99108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frith, U. (1988). Autism and “theory of mind.” In Gilberg, C. (Ed.), Diagnosis and treatment of autism (pp. 3357). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. UK, Oxford: Billing & Sons.Google Scholar
Frith, U., Morton, J., & Leslie, A. (1991). The cognitive basis of a biological disorder: Autism. Trends in Neuroscience, 14, 433438.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, P. L. (1989). The autistic child's impaired conception of mental states. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 11911195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, R. P. (1984). Early childhood autism and the question of egocentrism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14, 85104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217250.Google Scholar
Kanner, L. (1949). Problems of nosology and psychodynamics in early childhood autism. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 19, 416426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kasari, C., Sigman, M., Baumgartener, P., & Stipek, D. (1993). Pride and mastery in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 353362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kasari, C., Sigman, M., Mundy, P., & Yirmiya, N. (1990). Affective sharing in the context of joint attention interactions of normal, autistic and mentally retarded children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 87100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kingma, J. (1984). The influence of task variations in seriation research: Adding irrelevant cues to the stimulus material. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 144, 241253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leekam, S., & Perner, J. (1991). Does the autistic child have a metarepresentation deficit? Cognition, 40, 203218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leslie, A., & Frith, U. (1988). Autistic children's understanding of seeing, knowing, and believing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 315324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leslie, A., & Thaiss, L. (1992). Domain specificity in conceptual development: Neuropsychological evidence from autism. Cognition, 43, 225251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1987). Metarepresentation and autism: How not to lose one's marbles. Cognition, 27, 291294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J. A., & Sherman, T. (1986). Defining the social deficits in autism: The contribution of non-verbal communication measures. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 657669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ozonoff, S. (in press). Executive functions in autism. In Schopler, E. & Mesibov, G. E. (Eds.), Learning and cognition in autism. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive function deficits in highfunctioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 10811105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perner, J., Frith, U., Leslie, A., & Leekam, S. (1989). Exploration of the autistic child's theory of mind: Knowledge, belief, and communication. Child Development, 60, 689700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reed, T., & Peterson, C. (1990). A comparative study of autistic subjects' performance at two levels of visual and cognitive perspective taking. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 555567.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russell, J., Mauthner, N., Sharpe, S., & Tidswell, T. (1991). The “windows task” as a measure of strategic deception in preschoolers and autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 331349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sigman, M., Mundy, P., Sherman, T., & Ungerer, J. A. (1986). Social interactions of autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children with their caregivers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 647656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sigman, M., & Ungerer, J. (1981). Sensorimotor skills and language comprehension in autistic children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 9, 149165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sigman, M., & Ungerer, J. (1984). Cognitive and language skills in autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Developmental Psychology, 20, 293302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., Mundy, P., & Sherman, T. (1987). Cognition in autistic children. In Cohen, D., Donnellan, A., & Paul, R. (Eds.), Handbook of autism and atypical developmental disorders (pp. 103120). Silver Springs, MD: V. H. Winston & Son.Google Scholar
Sigman, M. D., Kasari, C., Kwon, J. H., & Yirmiya, N. (1992). Responses to negative emotions of others: A comparison of autistic, mentally retarded and normal children. Child Development, 63, 796807.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tan, J., & Harris, P. L. (1991). Autistic children understanding seeing and wanting. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 163174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yirmiya, N., Sigman, M. D., Kasari, C., & Mundy, P. (1992). Empathy and cognition in high-functioning children with autism. Child Development, 63, 150160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed