Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-18T01:14:56.996Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

13 - Different Mechanisms of Cognitive Flexibility Within the Prefrontal Cortex

from Section B - Cognitive and Social Neuroscience

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 August 2016

Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Cornell University, New York
Susan T. Fiske
Affiliation:
Princeton University, New Jersey
Donald J. Foss
Affiliation:
University of Houston
Get access

Summary

My significant contribution arose from a simple and perhaps naïve question about localization of function within the primate prefrontal cortex (PFC). Ever since Brenda Milner's seminal discovery about the problems of patients with frontal lobe damage in performing a “test of cognitive flexibility” (Grant and Berg's Wisconsin Card-Sorting Test; WCST), my colleagues and I had debated WCST's precise neural basis. Following analysis of effects of variable lesions suffered by frontal patients, Milner thought that the dorsolateral PFC was the key zone, whereas lots of extant theory about the role of the PFC in behavioral inhibition favored a more ventral region: the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC).

We can classify the WCST either as a test of concept formation or as a trial-and-error rule-learning task. A clinical neuropsychologist tests patients with a pack of 128 cards, each of which varies with respect to three perceptual dimensions: color, shape, and number (of shapes). The neuropsychologist provides spoken feedback for card-sorting according to three possible “rules”: color, shape, or number. Once a patient has learned the rule, the rule changes without warning, and they have to suppress the just-learned “rule” and learn another instead. Patients with frontal lobe damage perseverate with the former rule, even though they are apparently aware that it is wrong.

To address the issue of neural localization we modified the task for non-human primates, as the WCST essentially exemplified a paradigm well known in animal learning theory and developmental psychology. For complex (“compound”) stimuli comprising multiple dimensions, we make a distinction between learning to generalize a rule across different exemplars of a dimension, such as circles to squares (intra-dimensional shift [ids]; also known as “learning set”), and extra-dimensional shifts (eds) where the subject learns to respond to an alternative perceptual dimension, e.g., color, after attending to shape – similar to the shift in WCST rules. This distinction was important to learning theory in the 1970s, developed by Mackintosh and Sutherland, because a comparison of learning these two shifts predicts that the former, within-dimensional shift, will be easier according to their theory of attentional learning.

However, our non-human primate, the New World marmoset, could not be expected to sort cards (or indeed reliably perceive colors or numbers), so we adapted the test for use on a touch-sensitive screen with computer-controlled stimuli, using perceptual dimensions of lines and shapes.

Type
Chapter
Information
Scientists Making a Difference
One Hundred Eminent Behavioral and Brain Scientists Talk about Their Most Important Contributions
, pp. 62 - 65
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Milner, B. (1963). Effects of different brain lesions on card-sorting. Archives of Neurology, 9, 90–100.Google Scholar
Dias, R., Robbins, T. W., & Roberts, A. C. (1996). Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of affective and attentional shifts. Nature, 380, 69–72.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×