Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-01T21:30:19.416Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

11 - Behavioral Precursors of Developmental Dyslexia

from Part II - Cross-Linguistic Perspectives on Developmental Dyslexia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 September 2019

Ludo Verhoeven
Affiliation:
Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen
Charles Perfetti
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh
Kenneth Pugh
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

Learning to read requires mapping the units of a particular writing system onto its corresponding spoken language units. While such a mapping process is universal to all orthographies, there is considerable variation in (1) the visual characteristics of writing systems (e.g., Chinese, Arabic, Latin-based), (2) the grain size of the spoken language units that are represented by a writing system (e.g., words/morphemes in Chinese vs. phonemes in Western alphabets), and (3) the consistency and regularity of the correspondences between spoken and written language units, which is often called orthographic depth (Katz & Frost, 1992). Shallow orthographies like Finnish represent the sound structure of the spoken language in a highly consistent and transparent way while deep orthographies like English represent deeper linguistic structures (i.e. morphology) rather than the phonological surface structure of words, which makes them rather opaque to the developing reader.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Araújo, S. & Faísca, L. (2919). A meta-analytic review of naming speed deficits in developmental dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading. doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2019.1572758 epub ahead of print.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255, 556559. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1736359.Google Scholar
Boets, B., Wouters, J., Van Wieringen, A., & Ghesquière, P. (2006). Auditory temporal information processing in preschool children at family risk for dyslexia: Relations with phonological abilities and developing literacy skills. Brain and Language, 97, 6479. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2005.07.026.Google Scholar
Borgwaldt, S. R., Hellwig, F. M., & de Groot, A. M. B. (2005). Onset entropy matters: Letter-to-phoneme mappings in seven languages. Reading and Writing, 18, 211229. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-3001-9.Google Scholar
Bowey, J. A., McGuigan, M., & Ruschena, A. (2005). On the association between serial naming speed for letters and digits and word-reading skill: Towards a developmental account. Journal of Research in Reading, 28, 400422. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2005.00278.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brady, S. A., & Shankweiler, D. (1991). Phonological processes in literacy: A tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman. Hillsdale, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Breznitz, Z. (1997). Effects of accelerated reading rate on memory for text among dyslexic readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 289297. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.89.2.289.Google Scholar
Bruck, M. (1992). Persistence of dyslexics’ phonological awareness deficits. Developmental Psychology, 28, 874886. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.28.5.874.Google Scholar
Bus, A. G., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1999). Phonological awareness and early reading: A meta-analysis of experimental training studies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 403414. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.403.Google Scholar
Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2004). Children’s reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 3142. doi: http://dx.doi.org/0.1037/0022-0663.96.1.31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caravolas, M., & Landerl, K. (2010). The influences of syllable structure and reading ability on the development of phoneme awareness: A longitudinal, cross-linguistic study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 14, 464484. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10888430903034804.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caravolas, M., Lervåg, A., Defior, S., Seidlová Málková, G., & Hulme, C. (2013). Different patterns, but equivalent predictors, of growth in reading in consistent and inconsistent orthographies. Psychological Science, 24, 13981407. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797612473122.Google Scholar
Caravolas, M., Lervåg, A., Mousikou, P. et al. (2012). Common patterns of prediction of literacy development in different alphabetic orthographies. Psychological Science, 23, 678686. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797611434536.Google Scholar
Caravolas, M., Volín, J., & Hulme, C. (2005). Phoneme awareness is a key component of alphabetic literacy skills in consistent and inconsistent orthographies: Evidence from Czech and English children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 107139. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2005.04.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Jong, P. F., & van der Leij, A. (2002). Effects of phonological abilities and linguistic comprehension on the development of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6, 51-77. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S1532799XSSR0601_03.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehri, L. C. (2014). Orthographic mapping in the acquisition of sight word reading, spelling memory, and vocabulary learning. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18, 521. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2013.819356.Google Scholar
Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Stahl, S. A., & Willows, D. M. (2001). Systematic phonics instruction helps students learn to read: Evidence from the national reading panel’s meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 71, 393447. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/00346543071003393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elbeheri, G., & Everatt, J. (2007). Literacy ability and phonological processing skills amongst dyslexic and non-dyslexic speakers of Arabic. Reading and Writing, 20, 273294. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-006-9031-0.Google Scholar
Facoetti, A. (2012). Spatial attention disorder in developmental dyslexia: Towards the prevention of reading acquisition deficits. In Stein, J. & Kapoula, Z. (Eds.), Visual aspects of dyslexia (pp. 123136). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199589814.003.0008.Google Scholar
Frith, U., Wimmer, H., & Landerl, K. (1998). Differences in phonological recoding in German- and English-speaking children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 2, 3154. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0201_2.Google Scholar
Furnes, B., & Samuelsson, S. (2010). Predicting reading and spelling difficulties in transparent and opaque orthographies: A comparison between Scandinavian and US/Australian children. Dyslexia, 16, 119142. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dys.401.Google Scholar
Furnes, B., Elwèr, A., Samuelsson, S., Olson, R.K., & Byrne, B. (2019). Investigating the double-deficit hypothesis in more and less transparent orthographies: A longitudinal study from preschool to Grade 2. Scientific Studies of Reading, doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2019.1610410 epub ahead of print.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furnes, B., & Samuelsson, S. (2011). Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming predicting early development in reading and spelling: Results from a cross-linguistic longitudinal study. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 8595. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2010.10.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galuschka, K., Ise, E., Krick, K., & Schulte-Körne, G. (2014). Effectiveness of treatment approaches for children and adolescents with reading disabilities: A meta-analysis of randomized control trials. PLoS One, 9(2), e89900. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0089900.Google Scholar
Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2008). Predictors of word decoding and reading fluency across languages varying in orthographic consistency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 566580. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.100.3.566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Georgiou, G. K., Torppa, M., Manolitsis, G., Lyytinen, H., & Parrila, R. (2012). Longitudinal predictors of reading and spelling across languages varying in orthographic consistency. Reading and Writing, 25, 321346. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9271-x.Google Scholar
Hamilton, L. G., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2016). The home literacy environment as a predictor of the early literacy development of children at family-risk of dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading, 20, 401419. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2016.1213266.Google Scholar
Hari, R., & Renvall, H. (2001). Impaired processing of rapid stimulus sequences in dyslexia. Trends in Cognitive Science, 5, 525532. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01801-5.Google Scholar
Hatcher, P. J., Hulme, C., & Ellis, A. W. (1994). Ameliorating early reading failure by integrating the teaching of reading and phonological skills: The phonological linkage hypothesis. Child Development, 65, 4157. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00733.x.Google Scholar
Heikkilä, R., Aro, M., Närhi, V., Westerholm, J., & Ahonen, T. (2013). Does training in syllable recognition improve reading speed? A computer-based trial with poor readers from second and third grade. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17, 398414. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2012.753452.Google Scholar
Ho, C. S.-H., Chan, D. W.-O., Lee, S.-H., Tsang, S.-M., & Luan, V. H. (2004). Cognitive profiling and preliminary subtyping in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 91, 4375. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00163-X.Google Scholar
Ho, C. S.-H., Leung, M.-T., & Cheung, H. (2011). Early difficulties of Chinese preschoolers at familial risk for dyslexia: deficits in oral language, phonological processing skills, and print-related skills. Dyslexia, 17, 143164. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dys.429.Google Scholar
Katz, L., & Frost, R. (1992). The reading process is different for different orthographies: The orthographic depth hypothesis. In Frost, R. & Katz, L. (Eds.), Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning (pp. 6784). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.Google Scholar
Kirby, J. R., Georgiou, G. K., Martinussen, R., & Parrila, R. (2010). Naming speed and reading: From prediction to instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 45, 341362. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.45.3.4.Google Scholar
Landerl, K. (2017). Reading acquisition in German language. In Perfetti, C. & Verhoeven, L (Eds.), Learning to read across languages and writing systems (pp. 299323). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Landerl, K., Freudenthaler, H.H., Heene, M., et al. (2019). Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming as longitudinal predictors of reading in five alphabetic orthographies with varying degrees of consistency. Scientific Studies of Reading, 23, 220–234. doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2018.1510936.Google Scholar
Landerl, K., Ramus, F., Moll, K. et al. (2013). Predictors of developmental dyslexia in European orthographies with varying complexity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 686694. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12029.Google Scholar
Landerl, K., Wimmer, H., & Frith, U. (1997). The impact of orthographic consistency on dyslexia: A German-English comparison. Cognition, 63, 315334. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(97)00005-X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyytinen, H., Aro, M., Eklund, K. et al. (2004). The development of children at familial risk for dyslexia: Birth to early school age. Annals of Dyslexia, 54, 184220. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11881-004-0010-3.Google Scholar
Mann, V., & Wimmer, H. (2002). Phoneme awareness and pathways into literacy: A comparison of German and American children. Reading and Writing, 15, 653682. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1020984704781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marx, P., & Lenhard, W. (2010). Diagnostische Merkmale von Screeningverfahren. In Hasselhorn, M. & Schneider, W. (Eds.), Frühprognose schulischer Kompetenzen. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Meng, X., Cheng-Lai, A., Zeng, B., Stein, J. F., Zhou, X. (2011). Dynamic visual perception and reading development in Chinese school children. Annals of Dyslexia, 61, 161176. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11881-010-0049-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mol, S. E., & Bus, A. G. (2011). To read or not to read. A meta-analysis of print exposure from infancy to early adulthood. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 267296. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021890.Google Scholar
Moll, K., & Landerl, K. (2009). Double dissociation between reading and spelling deficits. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13, 359382. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10888430903162878.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moll, K., Loff, A., & Snowling, M. J. (2013). Cognitive endophenotypes of dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17, 385397. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2012.736439.Google Scholar
Moll, K., Ramus, F., Bartling, J. et al. (2014). Cognitive mechanisms underlying reading and spelling development in five European orthographies: Is English an outlier orthography? Learning and Instruction, 29, 6577. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.09.003.Google Scholar
Nash, H. M., Hulme, C., Gooch, D., & Snowling, M. J. (2013). Preschool language profiles of children at family risk of dyslexia: Continuities with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 958968. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12091.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (2011). Dyslexia, dysgraphia, procedural learning and the cerebellum. Cortex, 47, 117127. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2009.08.016.Google Scholar
Norton, E. S., & Wolf, M. (2012). Rapid Automatized Naming (RAN) and reading fluency: Implications for understanding and treatment of reading disabilities. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 427452. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100431.Google Scholar
Ojanen, E., Ronimus, M., Ahonen, T. et al. (2015). GraphoGame – a catalyst for multi-level promotion of literacy in diverse contexts. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, e671. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00671.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A. (2003). The universal grammar of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 324. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S1532799XSSR0701_02.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A. & Harris, L. N. (2013). Universal reading processes are modulated by language and writing system. Language Learning and Development, 9, 296316. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2013.813828.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., & Verhoeven, L. (2017). Learning to read across languages and writing systems. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Petersen, D. K. & Elbro, C. (1999). Pre-school prediction and prevention of dyslexia: A longitudinal study with children of dyslexic parents. In Nunes, T. (Ed.), Learning to read: An integrated view from research and practice (pp. 133154). Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, R. L., Arnett, A. B., Pennington, B. F., et al. (2018). Literacy acquisition influences children’s rapid automatized naming. Developmental Science, 21, e12589. doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, R. L., & Pennington, B. F. (2012). Developmental dyslexia. Lancet, 379, 19972007. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60198-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rack, J. P., Snowling, M. J., & Olson, R. K. (1992). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia: A review. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 2853. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/747832.Google Scholar
Ramus, F. (2003). Developmental dyslexia: specific phonological deficit or general sensorimotor dysfunction? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13, 212218. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4388(03)00035-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramus, F. (2004). Neurobiology of dyslexia: A reinterpretation of the data. Trends in Neurosciences, 27, 720726. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2004.10.004.Google Scholar
Rau, A. K., Moll, K., Moeller, K. et al. (2016). Same same, but different: Word and sentence reading in German and English. Scientific Studies of Reading, 20, 203219. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2015.1136913.Google Scholar
Rau, A. K., Moll, K., Snowling, M. J., & Landerl, K. (2015). Effects of orthographic consistency on eye movement behavior: German and English children and adults process the same words differently. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 130, 92105. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.09.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scarborough, H. S. (1989). Prediction of reading disability from familial and individual differences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 101108. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-0663.81.1.101.Google Scholar
Schmalz, X., Marinus, E., Coltheart, M., & Castles, A. (2015). Getting to the bottom of orthographic depth. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22, 16141629. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0835-2.Google Scholar
Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J.-A. (2002). Parental involvement in the development of children’s reading skill: A five-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 73, 445460. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00417.Google Scholar
Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143174. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/000712603321661859.Google Scholar
Share, D. L. (2008). On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: the perils of over-reliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 584615. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.4.584.Google Scholar
Share, D. L. (2014). Alphabetism in reading science. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, e752. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00752.Google Scholar
Snowling, M. J., Gallagher, A., & Frith, U. (2003). Family risk of dyslexia is continuous: Individual differences in the precursors of reading skill. Child Development, 74, 358373. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.7402003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stein, J. (2012). Visual contributions to reading difficulties: The magnocellular theory. In Stein, J. & Kapoula, Z. (Eds.), Visual aspects of dyslexia (pp. 171197). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stein, J., & Walsh, V. (1997). To see but not to read: The magnocellular theory of dyslexia. Trends in Neuroscience, 20, 147152. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(96)01005-3.Google Scholar
Suggate, S. P. (2016). A meta-analysis of the long-term effects of phonemic awareness, phonics, fluency, and reading comprehension interventions. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 49, 7796. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022219414528540.Google Scholar
Tallal, P. (2004). Improving language and literacy is a matter of time. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 721728. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn1499.Google Scholar
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Burgess, S., & Hecht, S. (1997). Contributions of phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming ability to growth of word-reading skills in second- to fifth- grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1, 161185. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0102_4.Google Scholar
Vaessen, A., Bertrand, D., Tóth, D. et al. (2010). Cognitive development of fluent word reading does not qualitatively differ between transparent and opaque orthographies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 827842. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaessen, A., Gerretsen, P., & Blomert, L. (2009). Naming problems do not reflect a second independent core deficit in dyslexia: Double deficits explored. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 202221. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2008.12.004.Google Scholar
Valdois, S., Bosse, M.-L., & Tainturier, M.-J. (2004). The cognitive deficits responsible for developmental dyslexia: Review of evidence for a selective visual attentional disorder. Dyslexia, 10, 339363. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dys.284.Google Scholar
van Bergen, E., de Jong, P. F., Regtvoort, A. et al. (2011). Dutch children at family risk of dyslexia: precursors, reading development, and parental effects. Dyslexia, 17, 218. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dys.423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 240. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.0021-9630.2003.00305.x.Google Scholar
Wimmer, H. (1993). Characteristics of developmental dyslexia in a regular writing system. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 133. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400010122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wimmer, H., Mayringer, H., & Landerl, K. (2000). The double-deficit hypothesis and difficulties in learning to read a regular orthography. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 668680. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.92.4.668.Google Scholar
Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415-438. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.415.Google Scholar
Wolf, M., & Katzir-Cohen, T. (2001). Reading fluency and its intervention. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 211239. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S1532799XSSR0503_2.Google Scholar
World Health Organization. (2008). International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems - Tenth revision (2nd ed.). Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., Bertrand, D., Tóth, D. et al. (2010). Orthographic depth and its impact on universal predictors of reading: A cross-language investigation. Psychological Science, 21, 551559. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797610363406.Google Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., Ma-Wyatt, A., Ladner, D., & Schulte-Körne, G. (2003). Developmental dyslexia in different languages: Language-specific or universal? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 169193. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00139-5.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×