References
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 05 June 2012
General editor
Summary
A summary is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
- Type
- Chapter
- Information
- The Handbook of East Asian Psycholinguistics , pp. 547 - 620Publisher: Cambridge University PressPrint publication year: 2009
References
1983. Lexical categories and reading tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 675–99.Google Scholar
and 1987. The English noun phrase in its sentential aspect. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, MIT.
1973. Toward a theory of natural classification. Paper from the ninth regional meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, 1–10.
and 1998. Speech perception abilities in children with specific reading difficulties (dyslexia). Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51, 153–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1998. Eight-and-a-half-month-old infants' reasoning about containment events. Child Development, 16, 636–53.Google Scholar
and 1991. Light verbs, VP-movement, negation and clausal architecture of Korean and English. Doctoral dissertation, University of Wisconsin, Madison.
1989. Functional categories in Korean. In Harvard Studies in Korean Linguistics III (pp. 79–88). Department of Linguistics, Harvard University.Google Scholar
and 1997. Is manually coded Korean language? [in Korean]. Communication Disorders, 20, 49–65.Google Scholar
1990. Korean quantification and Universal Grammar. Doctoral dissertation, University of Connecticut.
2002. Production of grammatical morphemes in adults with Broca's aphasia [in Korean]. In Proceedings of the Annual Conference of Korean Academy of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology in 2002 (pp. 162–9).
, , and 1997. Dissociating brain responses to syntactic and semantic anomalies: evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 112–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, and 2003. Differential object marking: iconicity vs. economy. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 21, 435–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1995. Japanese/Korean Linguistics 4. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.
(ed.) 1998. Japanese/Korean Linguistics 7. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.
, , , and (eds.) 1996. Japanese/Korean Linguistics 5. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.
, and (eds.) 2002. Japanese/Korean Linguistics 10. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.
, and (eds.) 1999. Acquiring basic word order: Evidence for data-driven learning of syntactic structure. Journal of Child Language, 26, 339–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
1988. The Acquisition of Aspect and Modality: the Case of Past Reference in Turkish. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1985. The acquisition of Turkish. In (ed.), The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition (vol. II: The Data). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
and 1986. A psychological account of the development and use of evidentials in Turkish. In and (eds.) Evidentiality: the Linguistic Coding of Epistemology. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
and 2003. Preferred argument structure in early Inuktitut spontaneous speech data. In and (eds.) Preferred Argument Structure: Grammar as Architecture for function.
. and 1971. On the role of deep structure in semantic interpretation. Foundations of Language, 6, 197–219.Google Scholar
1970. Classification of hand preference by association analysis. British Journal of Psychology, 61, 303–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
1976. How children talk about what happened. Journal of Child Language, 3, 167–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and and
2005. Intonational analysis and prosodic annotation of Greek spoken corpora. In (ed.), Prosodic Typology: the Phonology of Intonation and Phrasing. London/New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
and 1962. The recovery of forgotten language knowledge through hypnotic age regression: a case report. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 5, 24–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
1969. The optimal age to learn a foreign language. Modern Language Journal, 53, 334–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 2001. Temporal processing ability in above average and average readers. Perception and Psychophysics, 63, 148–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and 2007. Salvaging heritage languages. In and (eds.), Heritage Language Acquisition: a New Field Emerging (pp. 337–51). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
1994. Input vs. constraints: early word acquisition in Korean and English. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 567–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, and 2002. Overhearing a language during childhood. Psychological Science, 13, 238–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, , and 1997. Does childhood language experience help adult learners? In (ed.), The Cognitive Processing of Chinese and Related Asian Languages (pp. 417–41). Hong Kong: Chinese University Press.Google Scholar
and 1992. Introduction: John Gumperz' approach to contextualization. In and (eds.), The Contextualization of Language (pp. 1–38). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1998. Code-switching in Conversation: Language, Interaction and Identity. London/New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
(ed.) 1996. Speech production units among bilinguals. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25 (3), 397–416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1997. Negation and object shift in early child Korean. In The Interpretive Tract, MIT Working Papers in Linguistics (vol. 25, pp. 73–86). Cambridge, MA: MITWPL.Google Scholar
2000. Acquisition of negation in Korean: object constructions in the optional infinitive stage. Language Research, 36, 181–203.Google Scholar
2000. The role of the unique checking constraint in the syntax and acquisition of Korean negation. Unpublished manuscript.
and 2001. Integrative ASL-English language arts: bridging paths to literacy. Sign Language Studies, 1, 147–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1995. A model of physical reasoning in infancy. In and (eds.), Advances in Infancy Research (vol. 9, pp. 305–71). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
1990. Is the top object adequately supported by the bottom object? Young infants' understanding of support relations. Cognitive Development, 5, 29–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1991. Visual word recognition: evidence for strategic control of lexical and nonlexical routines in oral reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 644–52.Google Scholar
and 1996. Emotion talk(s): the role of perspective in the construction of emotions. In and (eds.), The Language of Emotions (pp. 209–25). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
and
1990. The contextual effects on reference resolution during text comprehension [in Korean]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Ewha Woman's University, Korea.
1984. In support of bilingual/bicultural education for deaf children. American Annals of the Deaf, 129, 404–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2004. Passives in German children with Williams Syndrome. In and (eds.), Williams Syndrome Across Languages (pp. 345–70). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1992. Epistemic modality in French children's discourse: “to be sure” or “not to be sure”? Journal of Child Language, 19, 389–414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, and 1996. The production of finite and nonfinite verb forms in agrammatism. Brain and Language, 55, 1–11.Google Scholar
, , and 1994. Individual differences and their implications for theories of language development. In and (eds.), Handbook of Child Language (pp. 96–151). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
, and 2001. Psycholinguistics: a cross-language perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 369–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, and 1987a. Grammatical morphology in aphasia: evidence from three languages. Cortex, 23, 545–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, and 1987b. Comprehension in aphasia: a crosslinguistic study. Brain and Language, 32, 19–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, and 1982. Functionalist approaches to grammar. In and (eds.), Language Acquisition: the State of Art. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
and 1987. Competition, variation, and language learning. In (ed.), Mechanisms of Language Acquisition (pp. 157–93). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
and 1989. Functionalism and the competition model. In and (eds.), The Crosslinguistic Study of Sentence Processing (pp. 3–73). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
and 1994. Developmental and stylistic variation in the composition of early vocabulary. Journal of Child Language, 21 (1), 85–124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, , , , , , and 2002. Event-related fMRI reveals cortical sites involved in contextual sentence integration. Neuroimage, 16, 736–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, and 1987. Performance of disabled and normal readers on the continuous performance test. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 15, 229–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, , and 1997. Guidelines for ToBI Labelling. Online MS and accompanying files. Available at www.ling.ohio-state.edu/research/phonetics/E_ToBI/.
. and and
1986. A final brief in the case against agrammatism: the role of theory in the selection data. Cognition, 24, 277–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1994. Code-switching and X-bar theory: the functional head constraint. Linguistic Inquiry, 25 (2), 221–37.Google Scholar
, and 1979. Some relationships between articulation and perception. Phonetica, 36 (6), 373–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, , and 1971. Simplification in children's language. In and (eds.), Language Acquisition: Models and Methods. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
1990. The late acquisition of preferred argument structure in Venezuelan spoken Spanish. In (ed.), Proceedings of the XVIth International Congress of Linguists. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
1999. Recruiting comparative crosslinguistic evidence to address competing accounts of agrammatic aphasia. Brain and Language, 67, 149–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, , and 2001. The effects of scrambling on Spanish and Korean agrammatic interpretation: why linear models fail and structural models survive. Brain and Language, 79, 407–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, , , , and 1986. Linguistic constraints on intrasentential code-switching: a study of Spanish/Hebrew bilingualism. Language in Society, 15, 313–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1969. Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
and 1994. Developmental perspectives on transitivity: a confluence of cues. In (ed.), Other Children, Other Languages: Issues in the Theory of Language Acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
1999. How regular is sentence comprehension in Broca's aphasia? It depends on how you select the patients. Brain and Language, 67, 242–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and 1996. Comprehension of reversible sentences in “agrammatism”: a meta-analysis. Cognition, 58, 289–308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, and 1997. Verb retrieval in aphasia: 1. characterizing single word impairments. Brain and Language, 56, 68–106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, , and 1997. Patterns of sentence comprehension in aphasia: a consideration of three hypotheses. Brain and Language, 60, 197–221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, and 1970. The cognitive basis for linguistic structures. In Hayes, (ed.), Cognition and the Development of Language (pp. 274–353). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
1974. The ascent of the specious, or there's a lot we don't know about mirrors. In . (ed.), Explaining Linguistic Phenomena (pp. 173–200). Washington: Hemisphere.Google Scholar
1997. The structure of age: in search of barriers to second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 13, 116–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1994. In Other Words: the Science and Psychology of Second-Language Acquisition. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
and 1999. Confounded age: linguistic and cognitive factors in age differences for second language acquisition. In (ed.), Second Language Acquisition and the Critical Period Hypothesis (pp. 161–81). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
and 1999. The problem of age in second-language acquisition: influence from language, structure, and task. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2 (2), 127–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1989. The child, the bioprogram and the input data: a commentary on Cziko. First Language, 9 (25), 33–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
2000. Development of sign language translation system for the education of hearing impaired people [in Korean]. In A Report of Korea Advanced Institute of Science and Technology (KAIST).
, , , and 1967. Some semantic universals of German adjectivals. Foundations of Language, 5, 153–84.Google Scholar
1995. The effect of focus on memory for words in the sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 232–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
. and 1999. Second Language Acquisition and the Critical Period Hypothesis. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
(ed.) 1999. Auditory temporal processing impairment: neither necessary nor sufficient for causing language impairment in children. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 42, 1295–1310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, , and 1996. Continuing influences of to-be-forgotten information. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 5, 176–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
. and 1992. A new theory of disuse and an old theory of stimulus fluctuation. In , and (eds.), From Learning Processes to Cognitive Processes: Essays in Honor of William K. Estes (vol. II, pp. 35–67). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
and 1990. The logical problem of foreign language learning. Linguistic Analysis, 20 (1–2), 3–49.Google Scholar
1996. Conservative pattern accumulation in foreign language learning. Paper presented at the EUROSLA 6 Conference.
1997. Features and patterns in foreign language learning. Paper presented at Second Language Research Forum, Michigan State University.
1988. The accessibility of Universal Grammar in adult language learning. Second Language Research, 4 (1), 1–32.Google Scholar
, and 2002. Acquisition of the constraints on wanna-contraction by advanced second language learners: Universal Grammar and imperfect knowledge. (www.sls.hawaii.edu/bley-vroman/).
and 1970. Language Development: Form and Function in Emerging Grammars. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
1975. Structure and variation in child language. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 40 (Serial No. 160).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, and 1987. Language-mixing in children's speech: Dutch language use in Turkish discourse. Language Learning, 37, 191–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1981. Consonance, dissonance, and grammaticality: the case of wanna. Language and Communication, 1, 189–206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1997. Age and ultimate attainment in the pronunciation of a foreign language. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19, 447–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, , and 1987. The maturation of syntax. In and (eds.), Parameter Setting (pp. 123–72). Dordrecht: Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1992. Bi-unique relations and the maturation of grammatical principles. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 10, 147–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1996. The presence of magnocellular dyslexia depends on the type of dyslexia. Vision Research, 36, 1047–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, , , , and 1997. Grammatical deficits in Italian-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 41, 1185–92.Google Scholar
, and 1973. Early Syntactic Development: a Crosslinguistic Study, With Special Reference to Finnish. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
1982. Reorganizational processes in lexical and syntactical development. In and (eds.), Language Acquisition: the State of the Art (pp. 319–46). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
1990. Mapping thematic roles onto syntactic functions: are children helped by innate linking rules? Linguistics, 28, 1253–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1996. Learning how to structure space for language: a crosslinguistic perspective. In , , and (eds.), Language and Space (pp. 385–486). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
1994. Linguistic and nonlinguistic determinants of spatial semantic development: a crosslinguistic study of English, Korean, and Dutch. Presented at the Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston, MA.
and 2001. Shaping meanings for language: universal and language-specific in the acquisition of spatial semantic categories. In and (eds.), Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development (pp. 475–511). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 2003. Space under construction: language-specific spatial categorization in first language acquisition. In and (eds.), Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Thought (pp. 387–428). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
and 1992. Crosslinguistic perspectives on topological spatial relationships. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Anthropological Association, San Francisco, CA.
and 1984. Discernible differences in the babbling of infants according to target language. Journal of Child Language, 11, 1–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, and 1980. Lexical representation of derivational relation. In and (eds.), Juncture (pp. 37–55). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
1976. Children's first word combinations. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 41 (1, Serial No. 164).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1990. Can children use a verb without exposure to its argument structure? Journal of Child Language, 17, 313–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, , , and 1995. Verb argument structure and the problem of avoiding an overgeneral grammar. In and (eds.), Beyond Names for Things: Young Children's Acquisition of Verbs (pp. 331–52). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
. and 1998. Was ist Asverstivitaet? aber oder und? [in German]. Deutsche Sprache, 26, 138–59. Quoted in Ewald Lang 2000.Google Scholar
1990. Auditory Scene Analysis: the Perceptual Organization of Sounds. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
1997. The Locative Alternation in German: Its Structure and Acquisition (Language Acquisition and Language Disorder 15). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamin.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1996. What do children know about the universal quantifiers all and each? Cognition, 60, 235–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
. and 1999. How children constrain their argument structure constructions. Language, 75, 720–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
. and 2002. Does preemption help children learn verb transitivity? Journal of Child Language, 29, 759–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
. and 1991. Parameter setting in second language phonology and syntax. Second Language Research, 7, 35–59.Google Scholar
and 1971. Children's comprehension of relativized English sentences. Child Development, 42, 1923–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1994. The INs and ONs of Tzeltal locative expressions: the semantics of static descriptions of location. Linguistics, 32, 743–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1998. Early Tzeltal verbs: Argument structure and argument representation. In (ed.), The Proceedings of the Twenty-ninth Annual Child Language Research Forum (pp. 129–40). Stanford University: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
1973. A First Language: the Early Stage. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1964. Three processes in the child's acquisition of syntax. In (ed.), New Directions in the Study of Language (pp. 131–61). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
and 1963. The acquisition of syntax. In and (eds.), Verbal Behavior and Learning: Problems and Processes (pp. 158–97). New York: McGraw-Hill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1978. The interpretation of universal affirmative propositions: a developmental study. Cognition, 6, 55–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
2001. Input variation and the development of argument structure: an examination of Hindi-speaking caregiver-child discourse. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Minneapolis, MA.
and 1994. Selective suppression of the magnocellular visual pathway during saccadic eye movements. Nature, 371, 511–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, and 1983. Lexical Representation. In (ed.), Language Production (vol. II, pp. 257–94). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
1983. A note on the “word-order problem” in agrammatism. Brain and Language, 20, 155–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1985. Syntactic and semantic structures in agrammatism. In (ed.), Agrammatism (pp. 125–52). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1991. Agrammatism is a theoretically coherent aphasic category. Brain and Language, 40, 274–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
1995. Issues arising in contemporary studies of disorders of syntactic processing in sentence comprehension in agrammatic patients. Brain and Language, 50, 325–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
1985. Syntactic determinants of sentence comprehension in aphasia. Cognition, 21, 117–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, and 1986. Assignment of thematic roles to nouns in sentence comprehension by an agrammatic patient. Brain and Language, 27, 117–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and and
1999. Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 77–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and 1988. Lexical access and inflectional morphology. Cognition, 28, 207–332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, and 1976. Dissociation of algorithmic and heuristic processes in language comprehension: evidence from aphasia. Brain and Language, 3, 572–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and 1978. The child as word learner. In , and (eds.), Linguistic Theory and Psychological Reality (pp. 264–93). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
1994. Does learning a language require the child to reconceptualize the world? Lingua, 92, 143–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1987. How children learn to classify nouns in Thai. Doctoral dissertation, Stanford University.
1991. Later rather than sooner: extra-linguistic categories in the acquisition of Thai Classifiers. Journal of Child Language, 18, 93–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1993. Syllable frequency and visual word recognition in Spanish. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 766–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, and 1995. The advantage of first-mention in Spanish. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 2, 124–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, and 2002. Infant categorization of containment, support and tight-fit spatial relationships. Developmental Science, 5, 247–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1995. A crosslinguistic study of early lexical development. Cognitive Development, 10, 159–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, , , , , and 1999. Neighbourhood effects in masked form-priming in children and adults. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14, 201–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, and 1968. The acquisition of noun and verb inflections. Child Development, 39, 433–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
1987. Cognitive constraints on information flow. In (ed.), Coherence and Grounding in Discourse. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
1994. Discourse, Consciousness, and Time: the Flow and Displacement of Conscious Experience in Speaking and Writing. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
2000. Language Acquisition By Eye. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
, and (eds.), 1980. Active memory process in visual sentence comprehension: clause effects and pronominal reference. Memory and Cognition, 8, 58–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1996. Korean (London Oriental and African Language Library vol. 4). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
1977. Korean reflexive pronoun caki and its referential NP's point of view [in Korean]. Language Research, 13 (1), 35–48.Google Scholar
1997. Early word acquisition in Korean: evidence for constraints View (I) [in Korean]. Korean Journal of Human Development, 4 (1), 76–86.Google Scholar
2004. Early lexical development of Korean infants: 8~17 months [in Korean]. Korean Journal of Psychology, 23 (1), 77–99.Google Scholar
and
1995. An examination of similarity neighbourhoods in young children's receptive vocabularies. Journal of Child Language, 22, 727–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and 2007. The neural basis of processing anomalous information. Neuroreport, 18, 747–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, , , , , , and 1995. Orthographic and phonological activation in recognizing Chinese characters. Psychological Research, 58, 144–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, , and 1981. Context-specificity and generalization in the acquisition of pronominal distinctions. Journal of Child Language, 8, 75–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and
1987. A comparison between Chinese-speaking and English-speaking children's acquisition of reflexives and pronouns. Paper presented at the 12th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development.
and 2005. Jiwon corpus accessible in zip format at http://childes.psy.cmu.edu/data/EastAsian/Korean/.
1975. The scope of negation in Korean. In The Korean Language: Its Structure and Social Projection. The Center for Korean Studies, University of Hawaii.Google Scholar
1994. Functional projections and verb movement. In (ed.), Theoretical Issues in Korean Linguistics (pp. 233–54). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
1998. Phonology and orthography in recognizing Hanja in skilled and less-skilled readers [in Korean]. Korean Journal of Experimental and Cognitive Psychology, 10, 17–35.Google Scholar
and 1999. Orthographic and phonological activation in the semantic processing of Korean Hanja and Hangul. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14 (5/6), 481–502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 2005. Semantic and phonological processing in reading Korean Hangul and Hanja words. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 34, 401–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and 1992. The processing characteristics of contrast information embedded in different story structures and in different abstraction level [in Korean]. Korean Journal of Experimental and Cognitive Psychology, 4, 76–92.Google Scholar
and 1996. The effect of syllable type and familiarity on Hangul recognition processing [in Korean]. In Proceedings of Spring Conference of the Korean Society for Cognitive Science (pp. 179–89).Google Scholar
and 1991. The effect of length and visual angle in reading rotated Korean words [in Korean]. Korean Journal of Experimental and Cognitive Psychology, 3, 63–75.Google Scholar
and 1999. The acquisition of relative clauses: experimental studies on Korean. Doctoral dissertation, University of Hawaii at Manoa.
1981. The acquisition of word order in Korean. Master's thesis, University of Calgary.
1985. Issues in the structure and acquisition of Korean anaphora. An unpublished doctoral dissertation submitted to the University of Alberta. Edmonton, Alberta.
1989. Parameter, subset principle, and the acquisition of the Korean reflexive pronoun. In Proceedings for the Cognitive Science Conference (pp. 296–301). Seoul, Korea.Google Scholar
1991. Null subjects in complements and adjuncts in Korean. In and (eds.), Korean Syntax and Semantics: ‘91 LSA Linguistic Institute Workshop. Seoul, Korea: Thae-Hak-Sa.Google Scholar
1992. The syntax and acquisition of ‘kyay’ (‘s/he’) and ‘caki’(‘self’) [in Korean]. Studies in Generative Grammar, 2, 361–92.Google Scholar
1994. Null arguments and mood categories. In (ed.), Theoretical Issues in Korean Linguistics (pp. 443–62). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
1996. Caki (‘self’) as a nominal anaphor and a free pronoun in Korean. Paper presented at the annual convention organized by the Linguistic Society of America (LSA – 1996).
1999. Caki as a Jekyll and Hyde. In Harvard Studies in Korean Linguistics VIII (pp. 252–65).
2001. Subjectless sentences spoken by young children in Korean. Qualifying paper. Harvard University.
2002. Lexical and semantic idiosyncracies of the predicates and caki (‘self’) in Korean. Ene-wa Cengpo Sahoy (Language and Information Society), 1, 1–20.Google Scholar
2004. Syntax, Pragmatics, and Acquisition of Korean Reflexive Anaphora. Seoul, Korea: Sogang University Press.Google Scholar
1996. Vowel correlates to consonant phonation: an acoustic-perceptual study of Korean obstruents. M.A. thesis, University of Texas at Arlington.
2001. Articulatory strengthening at the onset of prosodic domains in Korean. Journal of Phonetics, 28, 155–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1987. Phrasal phonology of Korean. In , , , and (eds.), Harvard Studies in Korean Linguistics III. Seoul, Korea: Hanshin.Google Scholar
1988. Evidence for the VP constituent from child Korean. In Papers and Reports on Child Language Development 27. Stanford, CA: Department of Linguistics, Stanford University.Google Scholar
and 1988. Restructuring parameters and complex predicates: a transformational approach. Doctoral dissertation, MIT.
1997. Viewpoint shifting in Korean and Bulgarian: the use of kinship terms. Pragmatics, 7 (3), 389–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1991. Korean-English code switching: switch-alpha and linguistic constraints. Linguistics, 29, 877–902.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
2001. Ambiguity of relative clause in Korean. Unpublished Master's thesis, Ajou University.
2002. Ambiguity of relative clause attachment in Korean. The Korean Journal of Experimental and Cognitive Psychology, 14, 187–204.Google Scholar
and 2003. Analysis of relationship between Korean Sign Language ability and reading ability [in Korean]. Journal of Special Education, 10, 151–68.Google Scholar
and 1988. Caregiver input in English and Korean: use of nouns and verbs in book-reading and toy-play contexts. Journal of Child Language, 27, 69–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1991. Early acquisition of epistemic meanings in Korean: a study of sentence-ending suffixes in the spontaneous speech of three children. First Language, 11, 93–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
(ed.)
1995. The development of epistemic sentence-ending modal forms and functions in Korean children. In and (eds.), Modality in Grammar and Discourse. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: Benjamins.Google Scholar
1997. Language-specific input and early semantic development: evidence from children learning Korean. In (ed.), The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition (vol. 5: Expanding the Contexts, pp. 41–133). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
1998a. Acquisition of Korean. In (ed.), Language Acquisition in North America: Cross Cultural and Crosslinguistic perspectives (pp. 281–336). San Diego, CA: Singular.Google Scholar
1998b. Verbs in early lexical and syntactic development in Korean. Linguistics, 36, 755–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1999. Early development of verb structures and caregiver input in Korean: two case studies. International Journal of Bilingualism, 3, 241–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
2000. Caregiver input in English and Korean: use of nouns and verbs in book-reading and toy-play contexts. Journal of Child Language, 27, 69–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2002. Language-specific influences on early semantic and cognitive development. presented at the International Congress for the Study of Child Language (IASCL), Madison, WI.
2006. Acquisition of modality. In (ed.), The Expression of Modality. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
1991. Learning to express motion events in English and Korean: the influence of language-specific lexicalization patterns. Cognition, 41, 83–121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and 1995. Early acquisition of verbs in Korean: a crosslinguistic study. Journal of Child Language, 22, 497–529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1999. Early sensitivity to language-specific spatial categories in English and Korean. Cognitive Development, 14, 241–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, , and 1986. The influence of the number of syllables on recognition of Korean words [in Korean]. Master's thesis, Pusan National University.
2003. Preschool aged children's use of prosody in sentence processing: the case of Korean children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Duke University.
2001. Are prosodic units universally important to children in their early language acquisition? Korean children's detection and use of language-specific prosodic units. Journal of Cognitive Science, 2 (2), 171–93.Google Scholar
and 2003. Young children's use of prosody in sentence parsing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32, 197–217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and 1999. Negation, its scope and NPI licensing in Korean. In and (eds.), Proceedings of ESCOL (pp. 25–36). Cornell University.Google Scholar
1977. On Wh-Movement. In , and (eds.), Formal Syntax (pp. 71–132). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
and
and
1993. A multicultural approach to education of children who are deaf. In and (eds.), Multicultural Issues in Deafness (pp. 17–27). White Plains, NY: Longman.Google Scholar
2000. Emerging literacy in bilingual/multicultural education of children who are deaf: a communication-based perspective. In (ed.), Deaf Plus: a Multicultural Perspective (pp. 41–58). San Diego, CA: Dawn Sign Press.Google Scholar
1998. The study for development of literal vocabulary ability in deaf students [in Korean]. M.A. thesis, Kong-Ju National University.
1997. NPIs outside of negation scope. In and (eds.), Japanese/Korean Linguistics 6 (pp. 415–35). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
and 1994. Acquisition of Case marking in Korean. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Texas, Austin.
1999. Adverbs and Functional Heads: a Crosslinguistic Perspective. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
1980. Referential choice in English and Japanese narrative discourse. In (ed.), The Pear Stories: Cognitive, Cultural, and Linguistic Aspects of Narrative Production (pp. 127–202). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
1984. The acquisition of subjects in Korean. Paper presented at the Ninth Annual University Conference on Language Development. Boston, USA.
1985. The acquisition of Japanese. In (ed.), The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition (vol. I: The Data). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(ed.)
1993. Preferred argument structure in Korean acquisition. In (ed.), The Proceedings of the Twenty-fifth Annual Child Language Research Forum (pp. 307–14). Stanford University: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
1995. Subject and object in Korean acquisition: surface expression and casemarking. In , , , , and (eds.), Harvard Studies in Korean Linguistics VI (pp. 3–17). Seoul, Korea: Hanshin Publishing Co.Google Scholar
1996. Referential strategies and the co-construction of argument structure in Korean acquisition. In (ed.), Studies in Anaphora (pp. 33–68). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1997. Discourse motivations for referential choice in Korean acquisition. In and (eds.), Japanese/Korean Linguistics 6 (pp. 639–59). Stanford University: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
2000. Transitivity in Korean acquisition: discourse-functional foundations. In (ed.), The Proceedings of the 13th Annual Child Language Research Forum. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
(ed.)
2003. The lexicon in interaction: developmental origins of preferred argument structure in Korean. In , and (eds.), Preferred Argument Structure: Grammar as Architecture for Function (pp. 81–108). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1986. Processing strategies in the acquisition of relative clauses: universal principles and language-specific realizations. Cognition, 24, 225–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, and 1973a. What's in a word? On the child's acquisition of semantics in his first language. In (ed.), Cognitive Development and the Acquisition of Language (pp. 65–110). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1973b. Non-linguistic strategies and the acquisition of word meanings. Cognition, 2, 161–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1977. Universal categories: on the semantics of classifiers and children's early word meaning. In (ed.), Linguistic Studies Offered to Joseph Greenberg (pp. 449–62). Sarratoga, CA: Anma Libri and Co.Google Scholar
1987. The principle of contrast: a constraint on language acquisition. In (ed.), Mechanisms of Language Acquisition: the 20th Annual Carnegie Symposium on Cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
1965. Some structural properties of simple active and passive clauses. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 4, 365–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1973. Space, time, semantics, and the child. In (ed.), Cognitive Development and the Acquisition of Language (pp. 27–63). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
2000. Evaluating models of human sentence processing. In , and (eds.), Architecture and Mechanisms for Language Processing (pp. 31–55). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
2003. The use of thematic role information in parsing: syntactic processing autonomy revisited. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 317–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Jr., , , , and 1980. The acquisition of tone. In , and (eds.), Child Phonology (vol. I: Production). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
1988. The acquisition of the meanings of modality in children aged eight and twelve. Journal of Child Language, 15, 425–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
1997. Words and morphemes as units for lexical access. Journal of Memory and Language, 37, 312–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, and 1990. Principles and parameters of long-distance reflexives. Linguistic Inquiry, 21, 1–22.Google Scholar
, and and
1980. Reading phonological recoding and deep dyslexia. In , and (eds.), Deep Dyslexia (pp. 197–226). London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
2001. DRC: a dual route cascade model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, , , and 1984. Form and function in explaining language universals. In , , and (eds.), Explanations for Language Universals (pp. 87–103). The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
1994. Working memory and retrieval: a resource-dependent inhibition model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 354–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and 2003. Brain activity during automatic semantic priming revealed by event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage, 20, 302–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, , , , and 1983. Pronoun disambiguation: accessing potential antecedent. Memory and Cognition, 16, 283–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1997. Effects of association and imageability on phonological mapping. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 4, 226–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, and 1976. The role of letter recognition in word recognition. Memory and Cognition, 4, 204–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
1988. Awareness of phonological segments and reading ability in Italian children, Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, , , and 1986. Jacaltec noun classifiers: a study in language and culture. In (ed.), Noun Classes and Categorization: Proceedings of a Symposium on Categorization and Noun Classification (pp. 264–94). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1985. The acquisition of structural restrictions on anaphora. In Proceedings of NELS 16.Google Scholar
and 1998. Investigations in Universal Grammar: a Guide to Research in Acquisition of Syntax and Semantics. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
and 1996. Quantification without qualification. Language Acquisition, 5 (2), 83–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, , , , and 1979. Prosodic development. In and (eds.), Language Acquisition. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
1992. Rhythmic cues to speech segmentation: evidence from juncture misperception. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 218–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1986. The syllable's differing role in the segmentation of French and English. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 385–400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, , and 1980. Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1986. Individual differences in comprehending and producing words in context. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 1–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and . 1967. The logical form of action sentences. In (ed.), The Logic of Decision and Action. University of Pittsburgh Press.Google Scholar
2001. Lapses of concentration and dyslexic performance on the Ternus Task. Cognition, 81, B21–B31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, , and 1987. From agrammatism to paragrammatism: German aphasiological traditions and grammatical disturbances. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 4, 187–256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
2003. Acquisition and loss of nouns and verbs: parallel or divergent patterns? Journal of Neurolinguistics, 16, 213–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1994. Fetal reactions to recurrent maternal speech. Infant Behavior and Development, 17 (2), 159–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, and 1986. Prenatal maternal speech influences newborn's perception of speech sounds. Infant Behavior and Development, 9, 133–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 2003. Flexible use of lexical and phonological information in word recognition. Journal of Research in Reading, 26, 280–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, , and , Jr. 2000. The robustness of critical period effects in second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 22, 499–534.Google Scholar
1998 Psychophysical evidence for a magnocellular pathway deficit in dyslexia. Vision Research, 38, 1555–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, , and 1992. Accessing functional categories in Sesotho: interactions at the morpho-syntax interface. In (ed.), The Acquisition of Verb Placement: Functional Categories and V2 Phenomena in Language Acquisition (pp. 83–107). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1994. On the “underspecification” of functional categories in early grammars. In , and (eds.), Syntactic Theory and First Language Acquisition: Crosslinguistic Perspectives (vol. I: Heads, Projections and Learnability, pp. 119–34). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
1996. Prosodic structure of early words. In and (eds.), Signal to Syntax: Bootstrapping from Speech to Grammar in Early Acquisition (pp. 171–84). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
1999. Verbs in Tzotzil (Mayan) early syntactic development. International Journal of Bilingualism, 3, 219–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1991. Adverbs of quantification: a generalized quantifier approach. Doctoral dissertation, University of Groningen.
1998. Verbs and nouns are organized and accessed differently in the mental lexicon: evidence from Hebrew. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 1238–55.Google Scholar
, and 1979. Children's comprehension of relative clauses. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 8, 499–518.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, , and 2000. Crosslinguistic psycholinguistics. In , , and (eds.), Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing (pp. 282–300). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
2001. Language deficits, localization, and grammar: evidence for a distributive model of language breakdown in aphasic patients and neurologically intact individuals. Psychological Review, 108, 759–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, , , , and 1989. The Theory of Functional Grammar, Part I: the Structure of the Clause. Functional Grammar Series 9. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
, and
1992. Integration process of consonants and vowels in Korean letter perception [in Korean]. Korean Journal of Experimental and Cognitive Psychology, 4, 1–15.Google Scholar
1994. Representation of causal information in reading: causality, causal field, and multiple cause [in Korean]. Korean Journal of Experimental and Cognitive Psychology, 6, 164–77.Google Scholar
1980. Children's use of phonological encoding when reading for meaning. Memory and Cognition, 8, 195–209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and , and
1974. Sentences and situations: children's judgments of match and mismatch. In (ed.), Current Problems in Psycholinguistics. Paris: Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique.Google Scholar
and 1998. Children's weak interpretations of universally quantified questions. In and (eds.), Conceptual Development and Language Acquisition. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
1992. Event quantification in preschoolers' comprehension of negation. In (ed.), Child Language Research Forum (vol. 24). Stanford, CA: CLRF.Google Scholar
and 2002. Discourse and grammar. In , (ed.), The New Psychology of Language: Cognitive and Functional Approaches to Language (vol. II). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
2003. Argument structure: grammar in use. In and (eds.), Preferred Argument Structure: Grammar as Architecture for Function (pp. 12–60).
2003. Preferred Argument Structure: Grammar as Architecture for Function. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRef
, and (eds.) 1988. Lexical ambiguity and fixation times in reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 429–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, and 2000. A small-to-large unit progression in metaphonological awareness and reading?The Quarterly Journal Of Experimental Psychology, 53a, 1081–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, and 1994. From Grammar to Politics: Linguistic Anthropology in a Western Samoan Village. Los Angeles and Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
2006. Retrieval interference in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 157–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and 1964. Memory: a Contribution to Experimental Psychology ( & , translators; original work published 1885), New York: Dover.Google Scholar
1996. A role for stress in early speech segmentation. In and (eds.), Signal to Syntax: Bootstrapping from Speech to Grammar in Early Acquisition (pp. 151–70). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
1997. The perception of rhythmic units in speech by infants and adults. Journal of Memory and Language, 36, 202–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, and 1985. Clause and the semantic representation of words. Memory and Cognition, 13, 371–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1996. Abnormal processing of visual motion in dyslexia revealed by functional brain imaging. Nature, 382, 66–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, , , , and , , and
2004. On the structure of resultative participles in English. Linguistic Inquiry, 35, 355–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1981. Pre-school children's use of the articles in definite and indefinite referring expressions. Journal of Child Language, 8, 313–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and 1996. Second language acquisition: theoretical and experimental issues in contemporary research. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19, 677–758.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, and 1984. Scissors, paper, stone: perceptual foundations of noun classifier system. In Papers and Reports on Child Language Development (vol. 23, pp. 41–9).
1986. Taking stock: the development of Chinese noun classifiers historically and in young children. In (ed.), Typological Studies in Language: Noun Classes and Categorization (vol. 7, pp. 399–436). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1995. Shifting face from Asia to Europe. In and (eds.), Essays in Semantics and Pragmatics: In Honor of Charles J. Fillmore (pp. 43–72). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
, and . 1993/1994. On the transfer of parametric values in L2 development. Language Acquisition, 3, 183–208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1985. An experiment of the use of classifier by 4–6 year olds. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 17 (4), 384–92.Google Scholar
1956. On the predictability of formant levels and spectrum envelopes from formant frequencies. In , and (eds.), For Roman Jakobson (pp. 109–20). The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
1968. Analysis and synthesis of speech processes. In (eds.), Manual of Phonetics (pp. 243–53). Amsterdam: North Holland.Google Scholar
1975. Vocal tract wall effects, losses, and resonance bandwidths. STL-QPSR, 2 (3), 1–19.Google Scholar
1995. The evidence for a temporal processing deficit linked to dyslexia: a review. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 2, 460–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and 1975. The relationship between age and second language learning ability. Language Learning, 25, 245–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1984. EPAM-like models of recognition and learning. Cognitive Science, 8, 305–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1985. Four-month-old infants prefer to listen to motherese. Infant Behavior and Development, 8, 181–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1988. The mechanisms of “Construction Grammar.”Berkeley Linguistics Society, 17, 35–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
2000. The loss of family languages by immigrant children: should educators be concerned?Theory into Practice, 39, 203–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1991. Binding parameters in second language acquisition. In (ed.), Point Counterpoint: Universal Grammar in Second Language Acquisition (pp. 351–73). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1986. Second language acquisition of reflexive-binding. In , and (eds.), Proceedings of the 16th Meeting of the North East Linguistic Society (pp. 154–68). Amherst, MA: University Of Massachusetts, Graduate Linguistics Student Association.Google Scholar
and 1987. A critical period for learning to pronounce foreign languages?Applied Linguistics, 8, 162–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1999. Age of learning and second language speech. In (ed.), Second Language Acquisition and the Critical Period Hypothesis (pp. 101–32). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
1997. Effects of experience on non-native speakers' production and perception of English vowels. Journal of Phonetics, 25, 437–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, and 2001. Effect of experience in L2 acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 23, 527–52.Google Scholar
and 1995. Factors affecting strength of perceived foreign accent in a second language. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97 (5), 3125–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, and 1999. Age constraints on second-language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 78–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, and 1983. A study of the effects of principal branching direction in second language acquisition: the generalization of a parameter of universal grammar from first to second language acquisition. Doctoral dissertation, Cornell University.
2004. On the developmental primacy of free relatives. In , and (eds.), Plato's Problem: Papers on Language Acquisition, MIT Working Papers in Linguistics 48 (pp. 59–69). Cambridge, MA: Department of Linguistics, MIT.Google Scholar
and 1982. Referential and quantificational indefinites. Linguistics and Philosophy, 5, 355–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 2000. The treatment of contrasts in interaction. In and (eds.). Cause-Condition-Concession-Contrast: Cognitive and Discourse Perspective. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
1976. Accessing the mental lexicon. In and (eds.), New Approaches to Language Mechanisms (pp. 257–87). Amsterdam: North Holland.Google Scholar
1973. Lexical access and naming time. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 627–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1997. Articulatory strengthening at edges of prosodic domains. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 101, 3728–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and 1990. A discourse explanation of the grammar of relative clauses in English conversation. Language, 66, 297–316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1998. Children's passive: a view from the by-phrase. Linguistic Inquiry, 29, 311–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1982. Individual variation in the perception of vowels: implications for a perception-production link. Phonetica, 39, 1–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
1979. On Comprehending Sentences: Syntactic Parsing Strategies. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Linguistics Club.Google Scholar
1987. Syntactic processing: evidence from Dutch. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 5, 519–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1982. Making and correcting errors during sentence comprehension: eye movements in the analysis of structurally ambiguous sentences. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 178–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1981. Sources of process interaction in reading. In and (eds.), Interactive Processes in Reading (pp. 361–86). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
1982. All the cars-which cars? from word meaning to discourse analysis. In (ed.), Children Thinking Through Language. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
, and 1986. How children deal with natural language quantification. In , and (eds.), Knowledge and Language. North Holland: Elsevier.Google Scholar
and 1995. The time course of syntactic activation during language processing: a model based on neuropsychological and neurophysiological data. Brain and Language, 50, 259–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2002. Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 78–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
1992. Thematic analysis in agrammatic comprehension: syntactic structures and task demands. Brain and Language, 42, 1–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and 1987. Processing passive sentences in aphasia: deficits and strategies. Brain and Language, 30, 93–105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and 1996. Syntactic parsing as revealed by brain responses: first-pass and second-pass parsing processes. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25, 157–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and 1999. Lexical integration: sequential effects of syntactic and semantic information. Memory and Cognition, 27, 438–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, and 1994. Morphology in agrammatism: a dissociation between tense and agreement. Master's Thesis, Tel Aviv University.
1997. Tense and agreement in agrammatic production: pruning the syntactic tree. Brain and Language, 56, 397–425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and 2003. Agrammatic comprehension of simple active sentences with moved constituents: Hebrew OSV and OVS structures. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 288–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and 1999. Paradoxes in the definition of dyslexia. Dyslexia, 5, 192–214.3.0.CO;2-N>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1970. Age regression with unexpected reappearance of a repressed childhood language. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 18, 79–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
1987. Strategies for visual word recognition and orthographical depth: a multilingual comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 104–15.Google ScholarPubMed
, and , and
1985. Verb Classes in English and Japanese: a Case Study in the Interaction of Syntax, Morphology and Semantics (Lexicon Project Working Papers vol. 3). Cambridge, MA: MIT Center for Cognitive Science.Google Scholar
, and , , , and
2001. Age-of-acquisition, word frequency, and neighborhood density effects on spoken word recognition by children and adults. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 468–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, and 1978. Selective attention to attributes and to stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 107, 287–308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2002. Are inferences from stereotyped role name to characters' gender made elaboratively?Memory and Cognition, 30, 439–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, and 1990. Referential processes in reading: focusing on roles and individuals. In , and (eds.), Comprehension Processes in Reading (pp. 465–86). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
and 1985. On the use of ‘will’ and ‘gonna’: toward a description of activity-types for child language. Discourse Processes, 8, 143–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1988. Neuropsychology and acquisition of second language learning. In (ed.), Issues in Second Language Acquisition (pp. 81–112). Cambridge, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
, , and
1978. On relational meaning: the acquisition of verb meaning. Child Development, 49, 988–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1982. Why are nouns learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In II (ed.), Language Development (vol. II: Language, Thought and Culture). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
1991. The meanings and use of modals hafta, needta and wanna in children's speech. Journal of Pragmatics, 16, 531–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1991. The metrical basis for children's subjectless sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 431–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1996. Prosodic structure in young children's language production. Language, 72 (4), 683–712.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1990. Function morphemes in young children's speech perception and production. Developmental Psychology, 26 (2), 204–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, and 1989. Mechanisms that improve referential access. Cognition, 32, 99–156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
1990. Language comprehension as structure building. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1997. Two decades of structure building. Discourse Processes, 23, 265–304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
1988. Accessing sentence participants: the advantage of first mention. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 699–717.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and 1989. Building and accessing clausal representations: the advantage of first mention versus the advantage of clause recency. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 735–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, and 1990. Investigating differences in general comprehension skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 430–45.Google ScholarPubMed
, and 1998. Linguistic complexity: locality of syntactic dependence. Cognition, 68, 1–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and
2000. Effects of prime word frequency and cumulative root frequency in masked morphological priming. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15, 421–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1997. The categorial status of determiners. In (ed.), The New Comparative Syntax (pp. 95–123). London: Longman.Google Scholar
1986. The Pragmatics of Word Order: Predictability, Importance, and Attention. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
1967. Language identification in the limit. Information and Control, 10, 447–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1995. Constructions: a Construction Grammar Approach to Argument Structure. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
1998. Patterns of experience in patterns of language. In (ed.), The New Psychology of Language: Cognitive and Functional Approaches to Language Structure (pp. 203–19). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
1987. The eyes have it: lexical and syntactic comprehension in a new paradigm. Journal of Child Language, 14, 23–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, , and 1984. Morphological complexity, ASL, and the acquisition of sign language in deaf children. Doctoral dissertation, Boston University.
2001. Theories, language, and culture: Whorf without wincing. In and (eds.), Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
1990. Do linguistic differences lead to cognitive differences?: a crosslinguistic study of semantic and cognitive development. First Language, 10, 199–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1995. Names, relational words, and cognitive development in English and Korean speakers: nouns are not always learned before verbs. In and (eds.), Beyond Names for Things: Young Children's Acquisition of Verbs (pp. 63–80). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
and 1982. The acquisition of syntactic categories: the case of the count/mass distinction. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA.
1985. Evaluating the semantic categories hypothesis: the case of the count/mass distinction. Cognition, 29, 31–54.Google Scholar
1988. Count/mass category acquisition: distributional distinction in children's speech. Journal of Child Language, 15, 109–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1990. Verb-based versus class-based accounts of actionality effects in children's comprehension of passives. Cognition, 36, 227–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and 2005. Relativization, ergativity, and corpus frequency. Linguistic Inquiry, 36, 456–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 2001. Memory interference during language processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 27, 1411–23.Google ScholarPubMed
, and 2004. Effects of noun phrase type on sentence complexity. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 97–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, and 2006. Similarity-based interference during language comprehension: evidence from eye tracking during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 32, 1304–21.Google ScholarPubMed
, , and 2002. Memory-load interference in syntactic processing. Psychological Science, 13, 425–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, and 2002. Aspects of the acquisition of (past) tense and (telic) aspect in Turkish and Korean. Language Research, 38, 1317–47.Google Scholar
and 1994. Constructing inferences during narrative text comprehension. Psychological Review, 101, 371–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, and 1995. Transcription of German intonation using ToBI-tones – the Saarbrücken system. Phonus, 1, 33–51.Google Scholar
and 1981. Form, function, and the language acquisition device. In and (eds.), The Logical Problem of Language Acquisition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
1986. Language deficits and the theory of syntax. Brain and Language, 27, 135–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
1989. Agrammatic comprehension of relative clauses. Brain and Language, 37, 480–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
1995a. A restrictive theory of trace deletion in agrammatism. Brain and Language, 50, 27–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1995b. Trace deletion, theta-roles, and cognitive strategies. Brain and Language, 51, 469–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2000. The neurology of syntax: language use without Broca's area. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 1–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
1999. The critical role of group studies in neuropsychology: comprehension regularities in Broca's aphasia. Brain and Language, 67, 134–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, , and 1982. Life with Two Languages: An Introduction to Bilingualism. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
1988. Exploring the recognition of guest words in bilingual speech. Language and Cognitive Processes, 3, 233–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1997. Processing mixed language: issues, findings, and models. In and (eds.), Tutorials in Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Perspectives (pp. 225–54). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
1986. Processing mixed language: some preliminary findings. In (ed.), Language Processing in Bilinguals: Psycholinguistic and Neuropsychological Perspective (pp. 123–43). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
and 1991a. Syntax and semantics in the acquisition of locative verbs. Journal of Child Language, 18, 115–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, , and 1991b. Affectedness and direct object: the role of lexical semantics in the acquisition of verb argument structure. Cognition, 41, 153–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, , and 1992. Verb syntax in Italian child grammar. In Geneva Generative Papers (vol. 1, pp. 145–62).Google Scholar
1992. Functional categories and language acquisition. Canadian Journal of Linguistics, 37, 241–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1982a. Conversational code-switching. In (ed.), Discourse Strategies (pp. 59–99). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1992. Contextualization revisited. In and (eds.), The contextualization of language (pp. 39–53). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1975. Cognitive aspects of bilingual communication. In , and (eds.), El lenguaje de los Chicanos. Arlington, VA: Center for Applied Linguistics.Google Scholar
and 2000. The development of comprehension and production of causative and passive sentences of 4–9 year old normal children [in Korean]. Master's thesis, Ewha Womens University, Korea.
1991a. A computer model of the temporal course of agrammatic sentence understanding: the effects of variation in severity and sentence complexity. Cognitive Science, 15, 49–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1991b. Syntactic priming in Broca's aphasics: evidence for slow activation. Aphasiology, 5, 247–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1994. On-line sensitivity to subject-verb agreement violations in Broca's aphasics: the role of syntactic complexity and time. Brain and Language, 46, 493–516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1990. Integration and buffering of new information. In and (eds.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (vol. 25, pp. 71–87). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
and 1978. Dichotic auditory fusion levels in children with learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 12, 349–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1995. The breakdown of functional categories and the economy of derivation. Brain and Language, 50, 92–116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
1993. The syntactic positive shift (SPS) as an ERP measure of syntactic processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 439–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, and 2000. Phrasal movement in Korean negation. In Proceedings of SCIL 9 (pp. 127–42). Cambridge: MITWPL.Google Scholar
2002. Implications of child error for the syntax of negation in Korean. Journal of East Asian Linguistics, 11, 211–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1981. The development of negation in one Korean child. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Hawaii, Manoa.
1999. Electrophysiological evidence for two steps in syntactic analysis: early automatic and late controlled processes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11, 194–205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and 2002. Differential task effects on semantic and syntactic processes as revealed by ERPs. Cognitive Brain Research, 13, 339–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and 1994. Distinguishing among proficiency, choice, and attitudes in questions about language for bilinguals. In and (eds.), Puerto Rican Women and Children: Issues in Health, Growth, and Development (pp. 191–209). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1992. Some properties of bilingual maintenance and loss in Mexican background high-school students. Applied Linguistics, 13, 72–99.Google Scholar
and and
1998. Asymmetry in the interpretation of –(n)un in Korean. In , , , and (eds.), Japanese/Korean Linguistics 7 (pp. 1–15). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
2007. In search of evidence for the placement of the verb in Korean and Japanese. In (ed.), Japanese/Korean Linguistics 15. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
2007. On negative imperatives in Korean. Linguistic Inquiry, 38 (2).
and 2007. V-raising and grammar competition in Korean: evidence from negation and quantifier scope. Linguistic Inquiry, 38, 1–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, and 1997. Development of functional categories in child Korean. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of South Carolina, Columbia.
1994. The syntax of negation in Korean and its development in child language. In Proceedings of ESCOL ‘94 (pp. 152–62).Google Scholar
and 1976. On the development of perceptual strategies in children: a case study on the Japanese child's comprehension of the relative clause constructions. Annual Bulletin of the Research Institute Logopedics Phoniatrics, 10, 199–224. University of Tokyo.
, , and 1997. The critical period hypothesis: where are we now? In and (eds.) Tutorials in Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Perspectives (pp. 19–61). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
and 1997. The partial availability of Universal Grammar in second language acquisition: the failed functional features hypothesis. Second Language Research, 13 (3), 187–226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 2000. Intelligence writing assistance. In , and (eds.), A Handbook of Natural Language Processing: Techniques and Applications for the Processing of Language as Text (pp. 181–207). New York: Marcel Dekker.Google Scholar
1976. The nature of comprehension errors in Broca's, conduction and Wernicke's aphasics. Cortex, 12, 258–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and 1991. Articles and definiteness, published in German as “Artikel und Definitheit.” In v. and (eds.), Semantics: An International Handbook of Contemporary Research. Berlin: de Gruyter.Google Scholar
1999. Auditory stream segregation in dyslexic adults. Brain, 122, 907–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, and 1988. Codeswitching: Anthropological and Sociolinguistic Perspectives. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRef
(ed.) 1984. Morphemic structure and lexical access. In and . (eds.), Attention and Performance X (pp. 211–24). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
, and 1992. Agrammatic Comprehension and the Trace-Deletion Hypothesis. MIT Occasional Paper 45.
1995. Comprehension of wh-questions by two agrammatic Broca's aphasics. Brain and Language, 51, 10–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1998. From Alexander to Wilhelm Von Humboldt: a crosslinguistic perspective. In (ed.), Syntax and Semantics (vol. 31, pp. 1–31.). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
1999. Involuntary language loss among immigrants: Asian American linguistic autobiographies. ERIC Digest (www.eric.ed.gov).Google Scholar
1992. An investigation of the recognition process for jukugo by use of priming paradigms [in Japanese]. Japanese Journal of Psychology, 63, 303–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
2004. Children's passives and their resulting interpretation. Paper presented at Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition North America (GALANA). University of Hawaii at Manoa. December, 2004.
and 1987. Clauses are perceptual units for young infants. Cognition, 26, 269–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, , , , and 1982. Acquisition of epistemic and deontic meaning of modals. Journal of Child Language, 9, 659–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
and 1994. From adjunct to head. In and (eds.), Language Acquisition Studies in Generative Grammar (pp. 119–49). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 2000. A piece of the puzzle: ASL and reading comprehension in deaf children. In Chamberlain, Morford and Mayberry, Language Acquisition By Eye (pp. 131–41).
1975. Lexical ambiguity and sentence comprehension. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 265–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
and 1987. Consciousness condition on the Korean reflexive. In Proceedings of the Nineteenth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (pp. 161–69). University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
2001. An empirical survey on animal classifiers in Korean Sign Language. A paper presented at Conference on Sign Linguistics Deaf Education and Deaf Culture in Asia, Hong Kong, 17th–19th December 2001.
1985. A quantitative analysis of KA/I and (N)UN as subject markers in spoken and written Korean. In Proceedings of the 1985 Harvard Workshop on Korean Linguistics (pp. 145–53).
1978. The development of the full passive. Journal of Child Language, 5, 65–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, and
and
2006. Contextual and syntactic cues for head-final relative clauses in Chinese. In Proceedings of CUNY 2006 Conference.
, and 1993. The acquisition of Chinese classifiers by young Mandarin speaking children. Doctoral dissertation, Boston University.
2008. Japanese/Korean Linguistics 13. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.
, and (eds.) 1975. A child and the English question formation rule. Journal of Child Language, 2, 299–301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
1994. The syntactic structure and development of the sign language by Korean deaf students [in Korean]. Doctoral dissertation, Dankook University, Korea.
1990. “Deference” versus “politeness” in Korean speech. International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 82, 41–55.Google Scholar
2002. Sentence comprehension of Korean-speaking adults with Broca's aphasia [in Korean]. Korean Journal of Communication Disorders, 7, 64–84.Google Scholar
2003. The production of grammatical morphemes of Korean children with developmental language impairment [in Korean]. Korean Journal of Speech Sciences, 10, 47–64.Google Scholar
2004. Comprehension of relative clauses in Korean-speaking adults with Broca's aphasia. Poster presented at the annual convention of the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, Philadelphia, PA.
and 2000. Difference between deixis and anaphora shown in aphasics [in Korean]. Master's thesis, Korea University, Seoul, Korea.
2003. The structure and processing of the Korean functional category in Aphasics [in Korean]. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 12, 197–216.Google Scholar
, and 2002. A model of Korean morphological passive processing [in Korean]. In Proceedings of the 16th KSCS Annual Spring Conference (pp. 137–41).
, , and 2000. Characteristics of processing semifinal verbal inflections in Korean [in Korean]. In Proceedings of the Winter Conference of the Korean Society of Experimental and Cognitive Psychology (pp. 29–40).
, , and 2000. Characteristics of processing functional categories in Korean [in Korean]. In Proceedings of the Spring Conference on Korean Cognitive Science (pp. 273–78).
, , and 2000. The structure and processing of the Korean functional category [in Korean]. In Proceedings of the Conference of the Korean Society of Communication Disorders (pp. 137–42).
, and 2001. The research of deictic pronoun and anaphoric pronoun in aphasic case [in Korean]. Korean Journal of Communication Disorders, 6 (1), 131–59.Google Scholar
, and 1992. The genesis of clausal structure. In (ed.), The Acquisition of Verb Placement: Functional Categories and V2 Phenomena in Language Acquisition (pp. 371–400). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar