Social media summary: We identify clusters of cultural traits and differences in modes of transmission among traits in Austronesian cultures.
Introduction
The origin and evolution of patterns of variation in human cultures and non-human animal traditions have been subject to intensive study over the past six decades (Marler & Tamura, Reference Marler and Tamura1964; Slater & Ince, Reference Slater and Ince1979; Whiten et al., Reference Whiten, Spiteri, Horner, Bonnie, Lambeth, Schapiro and De Waal2007; Creanza et al., Reference Creanza, Ruhlen, Pemberton, Rosenberg, Feldman and Ramachandran2015; Garland & McGregor, Reference Garland and McGregor2020; Zandberg et al., Reference Zandberg, Lachlan, Lamoni and Garland2021; Turchin et al., Reference Turchin, Whitehouse, Gavrilets, Hoyer, François, Bennett and Benam2022; Skirgård et al., Reference Skirgård, Haynie, Blasi, Hammarström, Collins, Latarche and Gray2023; Whitehouse, Reference Whitehouse2023). This research has often been framed in terms of expected differences between patterns of genetic and cultural variation (Ammerman & Cavalli-Sforza, Reference Ammerman, Cavalli-Sforza and Renfrew1973; Cavalli-Sforza & Feldman, Reference Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman1973, Reference Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman1981; Feldman & Cavalli-Sforza, Reference Feldman and Cavalli-Sforza1976; Rindos et al., Reference Rindos, Carneiro, Cooper, Drechsel, Dunnell, Ellen and Boyd1985; Creanza et al., Reference Creanza, Ruhlen, Pemberton, Rosenberg, Feldman and Ramachandran2015). The distribution of cultural variation among human populations is not expected to be random, and an important historical and anthropological question is what forces shaped the patterns of cultural diversity within and between populations. While genetic inheritance is predominantly vertical, at the macro scale, cultural variants may be transmitted vertically or copied/adopted non-vertically via mechanisms such as learning, exchange, conquest and competition (Dyson-Hudson & Smith, Reference Dyson-Hudson and Smith1978; Marcus, Reference Marcus2008; Karin & Alon, Reference Karin and Alon2018; Turchin & Gavrilets, Reference Turchin and Gavrilets2021). Both geography and genealogy are likely to contribute to the variation observed in some cultural datasets. For example, decades ago Ammerman and Cavalli-Sforza (Reference Ammerman, Cavalli-Sforza and Renfrew1973) investigated whether the spread of farming from the Middle East to Europe was due to vertical processes such as the migration of farmers themselves, as reflected in genetic patterns, or the non-vertical transmission of the farming culture without the necessity of human migration. Similarly, Jeong et al. (Reference Jeong, Wilkin, Amgalantugs, Bouwman, Taylor, Hagan and Warinner2018) found that the spread of dairy pastoralism in the eastern steppes was likely to have occurred through adoption by local hunter–gatherers rather than population replacement. Patterns of cultural variation in practices, beliefs or social structures may be analysed statistically to estimate how cultural traits covary in terms of geographical proximity or other relationships among populations. For example, Turchin et al. (Reference Turchin, Currie, Whitehouse, François, Feeney, Mullins and Spencer2018) used logistic regression, principal components analysis (PCA) and metrics of different aspects of cultural complexity and showed a generally increasing trend in sociopolitical complexity through time and across geographic regions.
Cultural microevolution can be defined as a change in the frequency of cultural traits within a single population (Turchin & Gavrilets, Reference Turchin and Gavrilets2021). In this framework, vertical transmission is defined as individuals acquiring a trait from their parents. Processes of non-vertical transmission occur when individuals acquire traits from non-parental adults or peers of the same generation (Cavalli-Sforza & Feldman, Reference Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman1981; Creanza et al., Reference Creanza, Kolodny and Feldman2017). In contrast, cultural macroevolution is focused on change in cultural traits in meta-populations (Eldredge, Reference Eldredge2009; Turchin & Gavrilets, Reference Turchin and Gavrilets2021). From a macro-evolutionary perspective, the ‘tempo’ of evolution refers to evolutionary rates and their variation in time and space. The ‘mode’ of evolution relates to mechanisms of both vertical (e.g. migration) and non-vertical (e.g. competition, exchange, conquest) cultural change at the societal scale (Dyson-Hudson & Smith, Reference Dyson-Hudson and Smith1978; Marcus, Reference Marcus2008; Karin & Alon, Reference Karin and Alon2018; Turchin & Gavrilets, Reference Turchin and Gavrilets2021). At the population scale, vertical transmission can be defined as the flow of traits from ‘parent’ to ‘daughter’ cultures (Turchin & Gavrilets, Reference Turchin and Gavrilets2021).
Oceania is an informative region for studying human ecology and cultural evolution owing to its cultural and linguistic diversity, relatively recent contact with outside cultures (e.g. Europeans) and the availability of a large body of relevant ethnographic literature (Kirch, Reference Kirch2017; Sheehan et al., Reference Sheehan, Watts, Gray, Bulbulia, Claessens, Ringen and Atkinson2023). Austronesian-speaking people spread throughout Oceania after leaving Taiwan around 5000–3500 YBP (years before present), ultimately reaching the islands of Hawai'i (900 YBP), Aotearoa (New Zealand) and Rapa Nui (Easter Island) from 600 to 800 YBP (Chambers & Edinur, Reference Chambers and Edinur2021; Ioannidis et al., Reference Ioannidis, Blanco-Portillo, Sandoval, Hagelberg, Barberena-Jonas, Hill and Moreno-Estrada2021), and as far west as Madagascar (1500 YBP, Bellwood et al., Reference Bellwood, Chambers, Ross and Hung2011; Chang et al., Reference Chang, Liu, Moncada, Seelenfreund, Seelenfreund and Chung2015). It has been suggested that Austronesian maritime technologies allowed the exploration of new frontiers and settlement of small and large islands distant from neighbours and the mainland (Spriggs, Reference Spriggs1995; Bellwood et al., Reference Bellwood, Chambers, Ross and Hung2011; Kirch, Reference Kirch2017).
Many aspects of cultural variation have been analysed for Austronesian societies. Rogers and Ehrlich (Reference Rogers and Ehrlich2008) and Rogers et al. (Reference Rogers, Feldman and Ehrlich2009) investigated how structural and decorative variation in canoes used by Oceanic peoples (Haddon & Hornell, Reference Haddon and Hornell1975) may have been subject to selection as opposed to stochastic cultural phenomena analogous to genetic drift. The observed patterns suggested that canoe decorations may be more subject to random (undirected) change than structural features, which may have been subject to selection. However, the latter is difficult to separate from the influence of biases in cultural transmission. Indeed, neither structural nor decorative canoe variation fit a genealogical pattern (Gray et al., Reference Gray, Bryant and Greenhill2010). Karin and Alon (Reference Karin and Alon2018) took a different approach, analysing Austronesian cultural variation using Pareto task inference (Shoval et al., Reference Shoval, Sheftel, Shinar, Hart, Ramote, Mayo and Alon2012). They found three ‘ecotypes’, which they called resource competition, mobility/exchange and resource defence, aligned with three of the four clusters suggested by Dyson-Hudson and Smith (Reference Dyson-Hudson and Smith1978) in the context of economic-dependability theory. Gray et al. (Reference Gray, Drummond and Greenhill2009) constructed a linguistic phylogeny of over 400 Austronesian groups, implicitly assuming some degree of vertical transmission. Indeed, Gray et al. (Reference Gray, Bryant and Greenhill2010) found that the phylogeny of Austronesian languages in Papuasia and Polynesia had a ‘moderately tree-like’ structure, which can be interpreted as the result of cultural vertical transmission. Subsequent studies have used this phylogeny, together with other evolutionary frameworks, to investigate the tempo of co-evolution in cultural traits such as ritual human sacrifice (Watts et al., Reference Watts, Sheehan, Atkinson, Bulbulia and Gray2016), social stratification and agricultural intensification (Watts et al., Reference Watts, Sheehan, Atkinson, Bulbulia and Gray2016; Sheehan et al., Reference Sheehan, Watts, Gray and Atkinson2018), systems of political and religious authority (Sheehan et al., Reference Sheehan, Watts, Gray, Bulbulia, Claessens, Ringen and Atkinson2023), and belief in supernatural punishment and moralising high gods (Watts et al., Reference Watts, Greenhill, Atkinson, Currie, Bulbulia and Gray2015a) assuming mostly genetic-like vertical transmission across the phylogeny.
Different kinds of cultural traits may evolve in different ways. Guglielmino et al. (Reference Guglielmino, Viganotti, Hewlett and Cavalli-Sforza1995) considered models incorporating demic diffusion, environmental adaptation and cultural diffusion, and tested the fit of these models to the patterns of variation for different cultural traits – finding that each model best explained the variation in some cultural traits, and that traits related to kinship patterns tended to be conserved across generations. Similarly Currie and Mace (Reference Currie and Mace2014) found that while different cultural traits tend to evolve at similar rates in different geographic regions, there are differences in evolutionary rates between cultural traits related to external environmental conditions and those related to social structure.
Here, we study two cross-cultural datasets, Pulotu and the Ethnographic Atlas (Murdock, Reference Murdock1967; Watts et al., Reference Watts, Sheehan, Greenhill, Gomes-Ng, Atkinson, Bulbulia and Gray2015b; Kirby et al., Reference Kirby, Gray, Greenhill, Jordan, Gomes-Ng, Bibiko and Gavin2016). Teams of subject-matter experts encoded the cultural variables in these datasets based on previous ethnographies. Pulotu includes 137 Austronesian cultures and was compiled for studying Religion in Austronesian people (Watts et al., Reference Watts, Sheehan, Greenhill, Gomes-Ng, Atkinson, Bulbulia and Gray2015b; Kirby et al., Reference Kirby, Gray, Greenhill, Jordan, Gomes-Ng, Bibiko and Gavin2016). Pulotu also contains data on other aspects of culture and several geographical and historical features, such as island type and the presence or absence of pre-Austronesian populations. We focus on 36 variables from Pulotu that are informative on traditional/indigenous cultural traits and conflict within and between groups and are missing no more than 50% of their entries (Supplementary Data). The Ethnographic Atlas (EA) includes 1291 cultures from around the world (Murdock, Reference Murdock1967; Kirby et al., Reference Kirby, Gray, Greenhill, Jordan, Gomes-Ng, Bibiko and Gavin2016) and many cultural variables divided into subsistence, labour, community organisation and kinship practices, among others. We focus on 69 EA variables (Supplementary Data) and the 130 groups of people that speak Austronesian languages (Figure 1).
The variables in Pulotu and EA are either categorical or ordinal: an example from Pulotu is degree of external conflict, which takes values between 1 (frequent external conflict) and 4 (absence of external conflict). Multiple categories were assigned to each feature in the original data. Based upon these category assignments, we identify four classes of cultural variables from the resulting broad classifications (Table 1): (i) social organisation; (ii) subsistence (both from EA); (iii) religion; and (iv) cultural interaction (both from Pulotu). Previous studies of Austronesian cultural evolution have focused on covariation of two or three cultural features (cf. Watts et al., Reference Watts, Sheehan, Atkinson, Bulbulia and Gray2016; Sheehan et al., Reference Sheehan, Watts, Gray and Atkinson2018, Reference Sheehan, Watts, Gray, Bulbulia, Claessens, Ringen and Atkinson2023), aggregated all cultural features (cf. Karin & Alon, Reference Karin and Alon2018) or focused on linguistic variation as a proxy for total cultural variation (cf. Gray et al., Reference Gray, Drummond and Greenhill2009, Reference Gray, Bryant and Greenhill2010; Padilla-Iglesias et al., Reference Padilla-Iglesias, Gjesfjeld and Vinicius2020). Seeking a unified approach, we divided the data into cultural classes with three questions in mind: (i) did the modes of transmission differ among these cultural classes; (ii) were there distinct patterns of variation for each of these cultural classes; and (iii) is geography alone sufficient to explain (some) of these patterns of variation?
In analysing Pulotu and EA, we first deal with missing data. Cultural data may be incomplete owing, for example, to non-standardised data collection methods in ethnography and archaeology (Bernard & Gravlee, Reference Bernard and Gravlee2014; Hester et al., Reference Hester, Shafer and Feder2016) and the variable degree of preservation of material culture in the archaeological record. Therefore, we imputed missing values in the data. Furthermore, cultural traits may be transmitted in linked packages, which should be accounted for in the inference of cultural transmission (Yeh et al., Reference Yeh, Fogarty and Kandler2019). We analysed patterns of cultural covariation using archetypal analysis. This is a computational method for dimension reduction and soft-clustering. It represents each data point as a convex combination of representative points, i.e. ‘archetypes’ (Cutler & Breiman, Reference Cutler and Breiman1994; Mørup & Hansen, Reference Mørup and Hansen2012). Archetypal analysis and conceptually similar methods, such as ADMIXTURE and STRUCTURE, have been widely applied in population genetics and genomics (Pritchard et al., Reference Pritchard, Stephens and Donnelly2000; Li et al., Reference Li, Absher, Tang, Southwick, Casto, Ramachandran and Myers2008; Alexander et al., Reference Alexander, Novembre and Lange2009; Alexander & Lange, Reference Alexander and Lange2011; Gimbernat-Mayol et al., Reference Gimbernat-Mayol, Dominguez Mantes, Bustamante, Mas Montserrat and Ioannidis2022). After identifying cultural clusters, we assessed potential differences in their cultural transmission. We calculated the pairwise euclidean distance between cultures in the archetype space, which accounts for covariance in cultural traits in a way that distances calculated in the trait space do not, and for potential sources of over- or under-estimating similarity. Next, we constructed cultural phylogenetic networks (Bryant & Moulton, Reference Bryant and Moulton2004; Huson et al., Reference Huson, Kloepper and Bryant2008) from the pairwise distances and then assessed the ‘tree-likeness’ of these phylogenies – or the degree to which they were generated by a genealogical process with vertical transmission (Holland et al., Reference Holland, Huber, Dress and Moulton2002; Huson et al., Reference Huson, Kloepper and Bryant2008; Gray et al., Reference Gray, Bryant and Greenhill2010).
In the cultural macroevolution of Austronesia, we find correspondence between cultural and linguistic outliers and differing degrees of vertical transmission among the cultural classes, with social organisation and subsistence being relatively more vertically transmitted than religion. We also find a clear geographic explanation for variation in the cultural interaction cultural class. These results suggest that social organisation and subsistence are more determined by cultural continuity between subsequently settled islands than religion. However, none of the cultural classes show variation determined by vertical transmission. In the social organisation class, we find that lineal biases in residence patterns, kinship reckoning and inheritance rules are not always parallel. In the subsistence class, we find support for previously suggested pauses in migration and shifts in the mode of subsistence that appear to correspond with these pauses. We also find that grain agriculture is not a prerequisite for the rise of complex societies. Our results demonstrate that analyses inspired by population genetics can quantify and illuminate the complex processes of cultural transmission, ecology and geography that jointly generate observed patterns of cultural variation.
Methods
Imputation and dimension reduction
To address the problem of missing data, we first perform variational bayesian principal components analysis (VBPCA; Ilin & Raiko, Reference Ilin and Raiko2010b) previously implemented in Matlab (Ilin & Raiko, Reference Ilin and Raiko2010a). This probabilistic version of PCA, in contrast to traditional imputation approaches, such as multivariate iterative imputation (Van Buuren and Oudshoorn, Reference Van Buuren and Oudshoorn1999), reconstructs the entire dataset, estimates posterior distributions for both the data and principal components, filters noise in the data and performs dimension reduction (Figures S18–S21). We imputed features with values present for more than 50% of the cultures (rows) and removed features with values present for less than 50%.
We compared the performance of VBPCA to iterative imputation with regression implemented in Scikit-learn (Pedregosa et al., Reference Pedregosa, Varoquaux, Gramfort, Michel, Thirion, Grisel and Duchesnay2011). For the initialisation step, where a first estimate is obtained, missing values were replaced by the column median. We considered the following regression models: bayesian ridge regression, nearest-neighbours regression with five or 10 neighbours, decision tree regression and random forest regression with 100 trees. We found that the best iterative imputation model and VBPCA produced similar results (Figures S18 and S20). Therefore, we elected to use VBPCA because it provides explicit marginal posterior distributions for the data and the principal components and performs dimension reduction, similarly to standard PCA.
Cluster analysis
Archetypal analysis
Archetypal analysis represents samples in a dataset as a convex combination of extreme points or ‘corners’ (Cutler & Breiman, Reference Cutler and Breiman1994; Mørup & Hansen, Reference Mørup and Hansen2012; Gimbernat-Mayol et al., Reference Gimbernat-Mayol, Dominguez Mantes, Bustamante, Mas Montserrat and Ioannidis2022). Given an n × p dimensional data matrix X composed of n samples and p features, and k archetypes, archetypal analysis approximates
where α is a non-negative n × k matrix of archetype weights in which each row sums to 1, β is a sparse non-negative n × k matrix in which each column sums to 1, and S = X Tβ. is a p × k loadings matrix. Then, an optimisation algorithm is used to find the α and β that minimise the approximation error ||X − αβ TX|| (Mørup & Hansen, Reference Mørup and Hansen2012); we use the archetypal analysis algorithm previously implemented in the Python package pypcha (Aslak, Reference Aslak2016). Notably, α i,j can be interpreted as the contribution of archetype j to population i, or, for soft-clustering analysis, as the membership coefficient of population i in cluster j.
In our analysis, X is the mean-centred, imputed, and noise-filtered data matrix reconstructed using VBPCA. The following section explains how we determine the number of archetypes, k.
Number of archetypes
Patterson et al. (Reference Patterson, Price and Reich2006) have previously developed a method to determine the number of ‘large’ eigenvalues in a PCA decomposition, which they expect to equal k − 1. Their method makes several assumptions about the dataset (e.g. the number of rows is much lower than the number of columns, and data entries are identically and independently distributed) that may be unsuitable for cultural datasets. Recently, we have extended their method to apply to datasets with missing data without constraints on the number of rows vs. columns or assuming an identical distribution of data entries. Briefly, we apply the above-mentioned VBPCA approach and use the posterior distribution of the reconstructed data matrix to develop a significance test, similar to the TW test of Patterson et al. (Reference Patterson, Price and Reich2006), which does not assume a theoretical distribution for the eigenvalues (i.e. the TW distribution) but instead uses a resampling approach. In Macdonald et al. (Reference Macdonald, Blanco-Portillo, Feldman and Ram2024b), we provide full details of the approach, validate it using randomly generated datasets and demonstrate its application to several datasets.
Clustering in the archetype space
It is possible to have more clusters than archetypes in population genetics and genomics, for example, if individuals have similar mixtures of two or more archetypes (Behr et al., Reference Behr, Liu, Liu-Fang, Nakka and Ramachandran2016). Thus, we performed K-means clustering for each cultural class to determine if such mixtures exist.
We considered three different metrics to score the results of K-means clustering: the silhouette score (Rousseeuw, Reference Rousseeuw1987) measures how similar an average data point is to its cluster (higher is better); the variance ratio criterion (Caliński & Harabasz, Reference Caliński and Harabasz1974) is the ratio of the sum of between-cluster dispersion to within-cluster dispersion (higher is better); the Davies–Bouldin index (Davies & Bouldin, Reference Davies and Bouldin1979) is defined as the average similarity of each cluster to its most similar cluster – further apart and less dispersed clusters will result in a lower index value (lower is better).
For each cultural class, we considered any number of clusters between two less and two more than the number of archetypes, except for cultural interaction, where we considered between two and four clusters (as the number of archetypes is two). We set the number of clusters to be a value for which two or more of the above metrics were optimised; if no such value exists, we set it to the number of archetypes, k (Figure S12).
Outlier detection
We first test whether there are significant differences in regional archetypes (see Figure 1 for the geographic regions). We examine the archetype matrices for each pair of regions, in which each row records the archetype weights of a single culture in that region. We use maximum likelihood to fit two models, M 0 and M 1, to these archetype matrices. The null model, M 0, assumes both matrices are independent samples from the same Dirichlet distribution, a natural distribution for modelling archetype weights. Fitting this model requires estimating the parameters of a single distribution. The alternative model, M 1, assumes each matrix is a sample from a distinct Dirichlet distribution. Fitting this model requires estimating the parameters of two distributions, one for each matrix. We then perform a chi-square test on the likelihood ratio of the two models to determine if the null model can be rejected in favor of the alternative model, from which we can conclude that the two regions are significantly distinct. This process was previously implemented in the test function of the Python dirichlet package (Minka, Reference Minka2000).
Next, using the following steps, we test if Polynesia provides a better explanation for the outlier archetype weights than the actual geographic region for each cultural class and each known Polynesian outlier (see above) in a region significantly distinct from Polynesia. We first sample N datasets from the posterior distribution of the VBPCA data reconstruction and apply archetypal analysis to each sample. Then, for each geographic region, we fit a Dirichlet distribution to the region archetype matrix using maximum likelihood, as above (this is performed N times per region). Next, for each Polynesian outlier, we computed the log-likelihood of its archetype weights under the fitted Dirichlet distribution for both Polynesia and the actual region. For each Polynesian outlier, we have N log-likelihoods for Polynesia and N log-likelihoods for the actual region. We then perform the Mann–Whitney U-test on these two sets of log-likelihoods under the null hypothesis that the distributions of log-likelihoods for the two regions are the same, with the alternative hypothesis that the distribution of log-likelihoods for Polynesia is stochastically dominant over the distribution of log-likelihoods for the actual region. If the null hypothesis is rejected (p-value < 0.05), then we conclude that the Polynesian outlier is also a cultural outlier (Tables S8 and S9).
Finally, to examine differences in the intra-regional variance between cultures, we compared variation within regions in the archetype space using a normalised F st,. metric, which is strongly correlated with the Dirichlet distribution variance, using the R package fststruct (Morrison et al., Reference Morrison, Alcala and Rosenberg2022; Tables S2–S6, Figure S10).
Cultural distance, phylogenetic networks and mechanisms of cultural transmission
To assess the extent to which the observed archetype variation agrees with a process of vertical cultural transmission, a pairwise distance matrix for each cultural class is constructed by calculating the distance d(i, j) between cultures i and j as the Euclidean distance in the archetype space (Figure S13).
We then use the Neighbor-Net algorithm (Bryant & Moulton, Reference Bryant and Moulton2004) to construct a phylogenetic network from this pairwise distance matrix using the splitstree software package (Huson et al., Reference Huson, Kloepper and Bryant2008). The phylogenetic networks are then visualised using splitstree (Figures 2, S4 and S5). These networks may be more or less tree-like, and the number of reticulations provides a visual representation of the degree to which a network is generated by non-vertical transmission processes, with more reticulations indicating less verticality.
We also computed the δ-scores and Q-residuals for each distance matrix (Figure 2) to quantify the tree-likeness of each cultural class (Holland et al., Reference Holland, Huber, Dress and Moulton2002; Huson et al., Reference Huson, Kloepper and Bryant2008). These metrics measure the extent to which taxa in the phylogenetic network satisfy the four-point condition (Steel, Reference Steel2016), with a low score indicating a tree-like network mostly owing to vertical transmission and a high score indicating non-tree-like networks, probably owing to the action of parallel evolution or non-vertical (e.g. horisontal) transmission (Gray et al., Reference Gray, Bryant and Greenhill2010). In the extreme case, as with the binary Cultural Interaction archetypes, when all quartets of taxa in the network satisfy the four-point condition, the network reduces to the phylogenetic tree constructed by the neighbour-joining algorithm (Bryant & Moulton, Reference Bryant and Moulton2004) and the δ-score and Q-residual are zero.
Results
We impute missing data using VBPCA, perform dimension reduction and soft-clustering via archetypal analysis, and construct phylogenetic networks using the Neighbor-Net algorithm. The following summarises our results, with details in the supplementary information (SI).
Cultural transmission in Austronesia
Two pieces of evidence suggest that significant differences in modes of cultural transmission exist among the cultural classes. First, a clear geographic gradient exists in the cultural interaction archetypes (Figures S1–S3 and Table S1). Second, cultural amalgamation occurs when cultures combine their influences without one dominating another, as with genetic admixture (Yinger, Reference Yinger1981; Dussart, Reference Dussart2003; Saghar, Reference Saghar2023). There are apparent differences in cultural amalgamation in both the archetype and principal component plots of the religion archetypes relative to the social organisation and subsistence classes (Figures 2, 3 and S3–S6). Together, these results suggest that these cultural classes are determined by distance from the mainland and neighbouring islands and are more subject to macro non-vertical transmission processes (e.g. parallel evolution, exchange, competition; see Gray et al., Reference Gray, Bryant and Greenhill2010; Turchin & Gavrilets, Reference Turchin and Gavrilets2021) compared with the social organisation and subsistence classes.
Cultural and linguistic outliers
We define cultural outliers as societies whose geographic neighbours are culturally distinct from them, analogous to the definition of enclaves by Barbieri et al. (Reference Barbieri, Blasi, Arango-Isaza, Sotiropoulos, Hammarström, Wichmann and Shimizu2022). We hypothesised that such outliers can be detected in cultural classes with low degrees of amalgamation but not those with high degrees of amalgamation. To qualitatively test this hypothesis, we utilised known linguistic outliers. Polynesian outliers are cultures that speak Polynesian languages but are geographically outside of Polynesia proper, mainly in Micronesia, Papuasia and Outer Melanesia (see Pawley, Reference Pawley1967; Feinberg & Scaglion, Reference Feinberg and Scaglion2012; Wilson, Reference Wilson2018). Nine Polynesian outliers were represented in at least one of our datasets (Figures 3 and S3–S5). Rotuma is not considered a Polynesian outlier but is known to have been strongly influenced by Polynesian cultures, with many of its words borrowed from Polynesian languages (Biggs, Reference Biggs, Tent and Geraghty2003; Bromham et al., Reference Bromham, Hua, Fitzpatrick and Greenhill2015).
We first confirmed that the regions containing Polynesian outliers are distinct from Polynesia in their archetype distributions. For the cultural interaction class, the Polynesian archetype distribution is significantly different from that of Papuasia (but not from other regions; see Table S2). For the within-region variability of archetypes among those regions containing Polynesian outliers, Polynesia has the least and second-least intra-regional variance in social organisation, and subsistence, respectively (Figure S7 and Tables S3–S7). We searched for cultural outliers in these cultural classes by fitting archetype distributions to each region containing Polynesian outliers and then did the same for Polynesia. For a given outlier, we calculated the log-likelihood of its archetypes for both the Polynesian archetype distribution and the archetype distribution for the geographic region of the outlier (see the Methods section and supporting information, SI, for full details). We found that five and four, for the social organisation and subsistence respectively, out of the six Polynesian outliers in EA are also cultural outliers. For social organisation, Ellice, Kapingamarangi, Ontong Java, Rennel and Rotuma are also cultural outliers, but Tikopia is not. We observe that Tikopia is most strongly associated with the patrilineal clans archetype, the dominant archetype in Outer Melanesia (Figure S9). For subsistence, only Ellice and Kapingamarang are not outliers, but both are in Micronesia, which has broadly similar dominant subsistence archetypes to Polynesia. In contrast, none of the nine Polynesian outliers in Pulotu were also religion outliers, and only one of three Polynesian outliers in Papuasia (Ontong Java) was a cultural interaction outlier (Tables S8 and S9).
There is correspondence between cultural and linguistic outliers (Tables s3-s7) in some but not all cultural classes (figures 3 and s3–s5 insets). we hypothesised that the social organisation and subsistence cultural classes were relatively more vertically transmitted than the religion cultural class because these correspondences between cultural and linguistic outliers suggest that there was strong continuity in social organisation and subsistence traits between subsequently settled islands.
Cultural distances and phylogenies
To assess the degree of vertical transmission in each class, we defined a distance between cultures, used it to construct phylogenetic networks and measured the tree-likeness of these networks. We computed the euclidean distance between cultures in the archetype space to account for covariation between cultural traits. Using standard distance metrics such as Jaccard or Hamming directly in the trait space (rather than the archetype space) underestimates the degree of cultural variation (Figures S17–S19). The distribution of cultural distances in the social organisation cultural class shows a strong skew, and that of the subsistence class is bimodal. In contrast, distances in the religion cultural class are approximately normally distributed (Figure 2), resulting in a lower average distance between societies in the religion cultural class. These differences in the distributions of cultural distances suggest varying degrees of amalgamation in religion across the path of Austronesian expansion, unlike the social organisation and subsistence cultural classes, where most cultures have a few ‘close’ cultures and many that are more ‘distant’.
We constructed phylogenetic networks for each cultural class by applying the Neighbor-Net algorithm (Bryant & Moulton, Reference Bryant and Moulton2004) on the cultural pairwise distances (Figures 3, S4 and S5). The cultural interaction class was excluded from this analysis because, with only two archetypes, its network is reduced to a tree with two branches corresponding to the two archetypes (Figure S11). Qualitatively, we observe that clustering on the linguistic trees and phylogenetic networks is well described by the archetypes for both the social organisation and subsistence classes, with few outliers. In contrast, the religion class has many more outliers on both the linguistic tree and the cultural phylogenetic network (Figures 3, S4 and S5). We measured the tree-likeness of the cultural phylogenetic networks, i.e. the degree to which processes of vertical transmission are consistent with each network, using δ-scores (Holland et al., Reference Holland, Huber, Dress and Moulton2002), which indicate that the networks of the social organisation and subsistence classes are more tree-like than that of religion (Figure 2, Table S9), suggesting that the effect of vertical transmission is stronger in these cultural classes. We focus on the δ-score because it is a more accurate measure of reticulation and because, for linguistic data, the Q-residual, a related measure (Gray et al., Reference Gray, Bryant and Greenhill2010), is highly sensitive to lengths of terminal branches, i.e. the magnitude of pairwise distances in the underlying distance matrix (Holman et al., Reference Holman, Walker, Rama and Wichmann2011). The δ-score provides consistent results across the choice of pairwise distance (Figure S20).
Cultural covariation across the Austronesian expansion
Here, we focus on the covariation of cultural archetypes, geography and cultural traits within cultural classes. The SI comprehensively analyses how archetypes are named and their correlations with cultural traits.
Covariation between culture and geography
The Social organisation archetypes each have regions where they are geographically dominant. Most archetypes appear to have been present from the beginning of the Austronesian expansion, assuming that the snapshot in time represented by the data is reasonably representative of the more distant past (Chambers & Edinur, Reference Chambers and Edinur2021; Figures 3, 4 and S9). The duolateral communities archetype may have appeared in either Malesia or Papuasia. There is a distinct shift in the dominant archetype between Malesia and Papuasia, Outer Melanesia and Polynesia, which were settled subsequently. This shift is compatible with a pause in Malesia after the initial pulse out of Taiwan (Gray et al., Reference Gray, Drummond and Greenhill2009; Figure 4). The combination of matrilineal and patrilineal practices present in the duolateral communities archetype may be at least partially explained by geography: it is the dominant archetype in between geographic regions where the matrilineal (Micronesia) and patrilineal (Outer Melanesia and southeast Malesia) clans archetypes are most common (Figure 3).
For the subsistence archetypes, examination of the regional archetype distributions suggests that the grain & cattle archetype is present from the beginning of the Austronesian expansion (Figure 4), in agreement with the archaeological (cf. Stephens et al., Reference Stephens, Fuller, Boivin, Rick, Gauthier, Kay and Ellis2019) and anthropological literature (Murdock, Reference Murdock1967; Watts et al., Reference Watts, Sheehan, Greenhill, Gomes-Ng, Atkinson, Bulbulia and Gray2015b; Kirby et al., Reference Kirby, Gray, Greenhill, Jordan, Gomes-Ng, Bibiko and Gavin2016). This archetype seems to have been lost in the migration to Papuasia, where none of the cultures belong to this archetype (Figures 4, S4 and S10). The roots & pigs and the fish & fruit archetypes and cultures with a mixture of these archetypes appear in Papuasia. Moreover, Chamorro and Palau in Micronesia are thought to have been settled earlier, with the first Austronesian people arriving approximately 3500 YBP, and then a large wave of settlment from Papuasia around 2200 YBP (Chambers & Edinur, Reference Chambers and Edinur2021). This settlement pattern may explain the presence of the roots & pigs and agriculture (but not cattle & grain) archetypes on some Micronesian islands.
Among the religion archetypes, cultures with the Autonomous Communities archetype separate the Mana and Heroes archetypes, which have religious authority that extends beyond the individual community (Figures 4, S5 and S11). The Mana and Heroes archetypes are centred around cultural heroes and the concept of Mana in different forms; both archetypes focus on deified ancestors but with varying intensity. In contrast, the primary unifying features of the autonomous communities archetype are the absence of super-local religious authority, a focus on nature spirits and a lack of the importance of deified ancestors (Figure S11). Sheehan et al. (Reference Sheehan, Watts, Gray, Bulbulia, Claessens, Ringen and Atkinson2023) found evidence for the co-evolution of political and religious authority in Austronesian cultures. This may be due to the relatively loose social structure of cultures associated with the autonomous communities archetype, which is positively associated with the duolateral communities archetype, whose social structure is also associated with acephalous (‘lacking a head’) communities (Figures S11–S13).
Covariation between cultural traits: Social Organisation
It is often assumed that lineal biases in residence and kinship reckoning patterns align. Patrilineal and patrilocal practices are broadly present in the duolateral communities and patrilineal clans archetypes. The matrilineal clans archetype shows patterns opposite to the patrilineal clans archetype, with husbands relocating to the wife's kin group and inheritance of land and property by sisters’ sons. These archetypes have uniformly female- and male-biased kinship and inheritance rules, respectively. However, as Fortunato (Reference Fortunato2019) shows, such alignments of lineal bias do not always occur. Indeed, the Duolateral Communities archetype is associated with a mixture of lineal biases. This archetype has matrilineal inheritance of land and inheritance of moveable properties by sister's sons, but patrilocal residence patterns.
Further, our results suggest that kinship reckoning and broader community organisation have complex patterns of covariation (Figures 3 and S12). We find that clans are present with opposite lineal biases in two of the archetypes (matrilineal and patrilineal clans) and that cultures with various modes of (some) unbiased inheritance or kinship reckoning run the gamut from autonomous communities (the duolateral communities archetype) to loose neo-local cognatic kindreds (descent through any combination of male and female ancestors, the neo-local kindreds archetype), to stratified chiefdoms with an entrenched elite class (the ambilineal chiefdoms archetype). Possible explanations for variation in modes of social organisation include a ‘bottleneck’ linked to population size, with more stratified social structures developing in societies with larger populations, inheritance from mother cultures and conquest or influence by more stratified societies (Earle, Reference Earle1997; Feinman and Marcus, Reference Feinman and Marcus1998; Yoffee, Reference Yoffee2005; Marcus, Reference Marcus2008; Rácz et al., Reference Rácz, Passmore and Jordan2020; Turchin & Gavrilets, Reference Turchin and Gavrilets2021). For results on covariation between cultural traits in the other cultural classes, see the SI.
Discussion
Four cultural classes were identified: social organisation, subsistence, religion and cultural interaction. There are significant differences in the verticality of transmission between the cultural classes; different patterns of variation in kinship residence, inheritance rules and descent reckoning (Figures 3 and S12); support for pulses and pauses in the Austronesian expansion (Figure 4) previously inferred from linguistics (Gray et al., Reference Gray, Drummond and Greenhill2009) and genetics (Chambers & Edinur, Reference Chambers and Edinur2021; Ioannidis et al., Reference Ioannidis, Blanco-Portillo, Sandoval, Hagelberg, Barberena-Jonas, Hill and Moreno-Estrada2021); a general west-to-east gradient in the degree of isolation, but also culturally significant exceptions to this gradient (Figures S1–S3, Table S1); and correspondence between linguistic outliers and cultural outliers (Tables S8 and S9).
Cultural and linguistic outliers
We found that five of six Polynesian outliers – cultures outside of the Polynesian Triangle that speak Polynesian languages – are more similar to Polynesian cultures than to their neighbouring cultures in their profiles of social organisation archetypes, and four of six subsistence archetypes (Table S8 and S9). Indeed, the ambilineal chiefdoms archetype, common in Polynesia, shows stratified community organisation with high levels of authority (Sheehan et al., Reference Sheehan, Watts, Gray, Bulbulia, Claessens, Ringen and Atkinson2023), which in turn could have resulted in the transmission of this archetype and associated cultural traits to newly settled communities.
In contrast, in the religion cultural class, none of the nine Polynesian outliers in Pulotu were cultural outliers; similarly, in the cultural interaction class, only one of three Polynesian outliers in Papuasia (Ontong Java) is an archetype outlier (Table S9). For the religion cultural class, this may be explained by greater cultural amalgamation relative to the social organisation and subsistence cultural classes (Figure 2). In the cultural interaction cultural class, it may be explained by geography: the distance between islands in Polynesia and the regions containing Polynesian outliers is relatively greater than in other regions of Oceania (Figures S1–S3).
The path of Austronesian expansion
Lydekker's line and Wallace's line are proposed biogeographic barriers separating the Asian and Australian biospheres (Ali & Heaney, Reference Ali and Heaney2021, and Figures 3 and S3–S5). The channel between the islands of Borneo and Celebes delineates Wallace's line. Lydekker's line corresponds to the western end of the Australian continental shelf. In the subsistence cultural class, we found a striking shift in the archetype distribution away from the grain & cattle archetype (Figure S4), which seems to agree with the previously inferred pause in the Austronesian expansion between Malesia and Papuasia (Gray et al., Reference Gray, Drummond and Greenhill2009). At the last glacial maximum, when much of Malesia, but not Australia, was connected to mainland Asia, the Banteng (Bos javanicus, a species of wild bovine) reached the eastern end of the Asian continental shelf (Ju et al., Reference Ju, Wang and Jiang2007; Gardner et al., Reference Gardner, Pudyatmoko, Bhumpakphan, Yindee, Ambu and Goossens2014). Genetic evidence also indicates that Indonesian varieties of cattle were domesticated locally (Chapter 8 of Bellwood, Reference Bellwood1995), which may explain why Lydekker's line (but not Wallace's line) appears to delineate the presence and absence of cattle. Some islands probably could not support cattle (e.g. small islands off the coast of Papua New Guinea). However, there are islands in Polynesia that, in the post-colonial context, are associated with large-scale animal husbandry: Aotearoa (New Zealand) famously has more sheep than people (Morris, Reference Morris2009), and most of the modern agricultural land use in Hawai'i is dedicated to livestock (Rehkamp et al., Reference Rehkamp, Roberts and MacDonald2021). However, in the pre-colonial context, Polynesia is associated with the roots & pigs and fish & fruit archetypes. The lack of widespread animal husbandry in Polynesia may be attributed to the loss of the grain & cattle and agriculture archetypes during the Austronesian expansion before the arrival to Polynesia. This lack of pre-colonial large-scale husbandry, combined with evidence for local domestication and the distribution of wild bovines, suggests that cattle were not transported with voyagers on the path of expansion. The accidental loss of valuable plants and animals during sea voyages (Whistler, Reference Whistler2009; Wilme et al., Reference Wilme, Waeber and Ganzhorn2016) may also have played a role in shifting away from grain & cattle.
Patterns of variation in the four religion archetypes are consistent with previous analyses of the Pulotu dataset that found evidence for the co-evolution of religious and political authority (Sheehan et al., Reference Sheehan, Watts, Gray, Bulbulia, Claessens, Ringen and Atkinson2023). Notably, Polynesia is associated with super-local religious authority and dual stratification into a hereditary aristocracy and a lower class (Figures S11 and S12). Furthermore, the presence of all religion archetypes in Malesia and regions further east (Figures S5 and S15) and the finding that Polynesian outliers are not religion outliers (Table S9) suggest that the evolution of the religion cultural class was strongly affected by non-vertical mechanisms such as parallel evolution or cultural exchange resulting in religious syncretism.
We found a west-to-east gradient of increasing Isolation (Figures S1–S3 and Table S1), with notable exceptions. In Polynesia, Tonga is in the contact archetype (Figure S3), which may be due to its period of expansion during the Tu'i Tonga Empire (Mageo, Reference Mageo2002). Similarly, Rapa Nui, Samoa, Mahoi and Tokelau have some degree of contact (although less than isolation, Figure S2). Among these, Samoa and Tokelau are known to have fallen within the Tongan sphere of influence (Gunson, Reference Gunson1990; Geraghty, Reference Geraghty, Dutton and Tryon1994) and Rapa Nui has been suggested to have been in contact with South American cultures (Scaglion, Reference Scaglion, Ballard, Brown, Bourke and Harwood2005; Chambers & Edinur, Reference Chambers and Edinur2021; Ioannidis et al., Reference Ioannidis, Blanco-Portillo, Sandoval, Hagelberg, Barberena-Jonas, Hill and Moreno-Estrada2021). In Micronesia, Ulithi is primarily associated with the contact archetype, which, together with the presence of pre-Austronesian people, may be explained by its position in western Micronesia (Watts et al., Reference Watts, Sheehan, Greenhill, Gomes-Ng, Atkinson, Bulbulia and Gray2015b). In Papuasia, three cultures are more associated with isolation than contact: Ontong Java, Rennell and Bellona, all of which have Polynesian languages.
Covariation of cultural traits
We found five archetypes in the social organisation cultural class (Figure 3). The matrilineal and patrilineal clans archetypes appear to have a predominantly female (matrilineal) or male (patrilineal) bias in residence, inheritance, and descent reckoning. In contrast, the neo-local kindreds archetype shows little, if any, lineal bias (Figure S12). Furthermore, the duolateral communities archetype shows a female bias in inheritance, a male bias in residence, and unbiased (duolateral) descent reckoning. This mixture of kinship practices may be explained by the geographic distribution of this archetype, located between regions in which patrilineal practices are common to the west and east and matrilineal practices to the north (Figure 3c). In addition, the ambilineal chiefdoms archetype shows unbiased (ambilineal) descent reckoning and male-biased land inheritance, which further support a ‘shift in focus – from viewing matrilineal and patrilineal kinship as unitary phenomena to consideration of the different aspects of the social system featuring a bias towards lineally related kin’ (Fortunato, Reference Fortunato2019), and a more general shift away from ‘main sequence theory’, as suggested by Opie et al. (Reference Opie, Shultz, Atkinson, Currie and Mace2014). Genetic evidence and cultural phylogenetic analyses indicate that early Austronesian societies were matrilocal (Jordan et al., Reference Jordan, Gray, Greenhill and Mace2009; Fortunato & Jordan, Reference Fortunato and Jordan2010). Thus, the persistence of matrilocal practice in Micronesia may be (partially) explained by the early settlement of Chamorro and Palau early in the Austronesian expansion and its relative isolation (Figure 4).
It is possible to have more clusters than archetypes, for example, in population genetics and genomics, when multiple individuals have similar levels of admixture (Behr et al., Reference Behr, Liu, Liu-Fang, Nakka and Ramachandran2016). Similarly, some cultural groups may have similar archetype mixtures. In the subsistence cultural class, we found four archetypes but five clusters. The fifth cluster is a mixture of the roots & pigs and fish & fruit archetypes. Geographically, fish & fruit, roots & pigs and their mixture dominate in all regions except for Island Southeast Asia and Madagascar, where grain & cattle dominates (Figures S4 and S10).
In the anthropological and archeological literature, primarily based on Eurasian cultures, it has often been assumed that grain-based agriculture (e.g. maize in the Americas, wheat in the Levant and rice in East Asia) is a pre-requisite for the rise of a complex society (complex meaning chiefdoms and states; see Moseley & Feldman, Reference Moseley, Feldman, Bailey and Parkington1988; Earle, Reference Earle1997; Feinman & Marcus, Reference Feinman and Marcus1998; Yoffee, Reference Yoffee2005; Marcus, Reference Marcus2008; Beresford-Jones et al., Reference Beresford-Jones, Pullen, Chauca, Cadwallader, García, Salvatierra and French2018). The Austronesian chiefdoms represent a challenge to the necessity of grain-based agriculture as the primary mode of subsistence for the rise of complex societies owing to (i) the absence of correlation between the Ambilineal Chiefdoms archetype and grain agriculture, (ii) these chiefdoms’ lack of association with metalworking and pottery production and (iii) their association with the fish & fruit and roots & pigs archetypes (Figures S11 and S12). These findings are further backed by evidence for widespread sweet potato cultivation in the archaeological records of Polynesia, but no evidence for grain cultivation (Kirch et al., Reference Kirch, Hartshorn, Chadwick, Vitousek, Sherrod, Coil and Sharp2004; Kirch, Reference Kirch2017).
Additional evidence from the Americas suggests that only a sufficiently stable and stationary food source and, sometimes, appropriate local trade partners are required for complex societies to rise. For example, complex societies in the modern Pacific Northwest are thought to have relied on socio-ecological systems and the rich local biodiversity for subsistence (Trosper, Reference Trosper2003). The most appropriate analogy for the Polynesian chiefdoms in challenging the necessity of grain-based agriculture may be the complex societies on the northwest coast of South America. Andean archaeologists have developed the Maritime Foundations of Andean Civilisation hypothesis, which directly challenges the notion that grain-based agriculture is necessary for the rise of complex societies (Moseley, Reference Moseley1975, Reference Moseley1992, Reference Moseley and Trillo2005; Moseley & Feldman, Reference Moseley, Feldman, Bailey and Parkington1988; but see Beresford-Jones et al., Reference Beresford-Jones, Pullen, Chauca, Cadwallader, García, Salvatierra and French2018). On the arid coastal plane of Peru, which has the world's richest fishery immediately off-shore, the rise of chiefdoms and states (e.g. Caral–Supe) occurred only in the north, where there were inland trade partners with the plant fibres necessary to make the nets needed to catch large amounts of fish (Beresford-Jones et al., Reference Beresford-Jones, Pullen, Chauca, Cadwallader, García, Salvatierra and French2018). Thus, both the Polynesian chiefdoms and the complex societies of the northern Peruvian coast represent an alternative model in which it is not necessary for the materials needed for the acquisition and storage of food to be locally produced, and where grain-based agriculture is not required if suitable trade partners and sufficiently rich sources of subsistence are available.
We note that Sheehan et al. (Reference Sheehan, Watts, Gray and Atkinson2018) found support for coevolution between landesque capital intensive agriculture and political complexity, which appears to contrast with our findings. This difference is probably due to the different operationalisation of variables, with Sheehan et al. (Reference Sheehan, Watts, Gray and Atkinson2018) focussed on intensive agriculture and a high level of political complexity rather than just the presence of agriculture and hereditary chiefs.
The high degree of religious and political authority, together with the relative geographic isolation of each culture in Polynesia, may explain why it has the most within-region variation in religion archetypes, despite its position at the terminal end of the Austronesian expansion (Figures S7). Specifically, the relatively greater geographic distance between neighbours and the high degree of authority may combine to preserve differences among individual cultures. When paired with occasional contact with other cultures, this greater consolidation of power may result in greater variation owing to fewer amalgamation events from trade and other contact.
Modes of cultural transmission
We inferred that the social organisation and subsistence cultural classes are relatively more vertically transmitted than the religion cultural class (Figure 2 and Table S10). This is supported by measurements of the tree-likeness of the phylogenies of each cultural class, the distributions of pairwise archetype distances (Figure 2), and the degree of separation and mixing between individual cultures and archetypes in principal components spaces, linguistic trees and cultural phylogenetic networks (Figures 3, S4, S5 and S8). These results are relative, and none of the metrics considered are absolute measures of vertical transmission (Holland et al., Reference Holland, Huber, Dress and Moulton2002; Gray et al., Reference Gray, Bryant and Greenhill2010).
At the macroevolutionary scale one might a priori expect religion to be relatively more vertically transmitted then social organisation or subsistence. Cultural group selection (Smith, Reference Smith2020) may drive the adoption and spread of religious beliefs, and these beliefs may be maintained by mechanisms such as costly sacrifice (Atran & Henrich, Reference Atran and Henrich2010; Norenzayan, Reference Norenzayan2013; Norenzayan et al., Reference Norenzayan, Shariff, Gervais, Willard, McNamara, Slingerland and Henrich2016). However, the relatively lesser degree of verticality in transmission of religious traits may be explained by the interplay of both intra- and intercultural factors. Vlerick (Reference Vlerick2020) argues that many features of religious practice are not generated by top-down control, but rather from the bottom up. Thus one possible explanation for the relatively lower verticality of the religion class is the combination of various modes of cultural exchange and tension between top-down religious authority (i.e. super-local religious structures; Sheehan et al., Reference Sheehan, Watts, Gray, Bulbulia, Claessens, Ringen and Atkinson2023) and bottom-up adoption of different beliefs and practices.
However, examination of the individual traits within the religion class suggests that some other factors are at play in the transmission of religious traits. When we checked for phylogenetic signal of individual traits over the linguistic phylogeny of Gray et al. (Reference Gray, Drummond and Greenhill2009), we found that while some traits had strong phylogenic signal (i.e. stronger verticality, e.g. headhunting with λ = 0.52, p = 4 × 10−5.) and others had none (e.g. cultural heroes with λ = 0.034 p = 0.7; see Table S13 for a complete list), the majority were somewhere in between.
Both vertical and non-vertical transmission, as well as other factors such as adaptation, probably played a role in cultural evolution in Austronesia, as even the relatively more tree-like cultural classes are only moderately tree-like (Holland et al., Reference Holland, Huber, Dress and Moulton2002; Gray et al., Reference Gray, Bryant and Greenhill2010). In agreement with previous studies (Gray et al., Reference Gray, Bryant and Greenhill2010), our results suggest strong non-vertical cultural transmission in Polynesia and throughout the Austronesian expansion. A possible explanation for this non-vertical transmission is the advanced maritime technology of Austronesian societies (Spriggs, Reference Spriggs1995; Bellwood et al., Reference Bellwood, Chambers, Ross and Hung2011; Kirch, Reference Kirch2017). Societies in the contact archetype are associated with frequent contact (Figure S16). We also detect a geographic gradient in isolation (Figures 4 and S1–S3), which suggests that the degree of contact vs. isolation drives cultural evolutionary processes rather than being an outcome of such processes. Essentially, the degree of isolation, while potentially mitigated by the maritime technologies that allowed Austronesians to colonise islands previously uninhabited by humans, appears to be primarily explained by geography and ecology (Kirch, Reference Kirch2017). For example, the ability of Rapa Nuians to maintain their canoes upon arrival may have been hampered by the eventual loss of trees on the island (see Hunt & Lipo, Reference Hunt, Lipo and Larrue2013), as well as their relative geographic isolation. Similarly, different ‘canoe plants’ and ‘canoe animals’, taken with voyagers settling new islands, were lost at various stages of the Austronesian expansion (Whistler, Reference Whistler2009; Wilme et al., Reference Wilme, Waeber and Ganzhorn2016), which may be explained by some plants and animals or the knowledge required to sustain them not surviving subsequent voyages. These results align with those of Padilla-Iglesias et al. (Reference Padilla-Iglesias, Gjesfjeld and Vinicius2020), who found that geographic and cultural isolation are drivers of linguistic evolution in Austronesian cultures.
Many studies have applied models of character evolution over phylogenetic trees to study the evolution and co-evolution of cultural traits over linguistic phylogenetic trees (Holden & Mace, Reference Holden and Mace2003; Mace & Holden, Reference Mace and Holden2005; Jordan et al., Reference Jordan, Gray, Greenhill and Mace2009; Fortunato & Jordan, Reference Fortunato and Jordan2010; Opie et al., Reference Opie, Shultz, Atkinson, Currie and Mace2014; Watts et al., Reference Watts, Greenhill, Atkinson, Currie, Bulbulia and Gray2015a, Reference Watts, Sheehan, Atkinson, Bulbulia and Gray2016; Sheehan et al., Reference Sheehan, Watts, Gray and Atkinson2018, Reference Sheehan, Watts, Gray, Bulbulia, Claessens, Ringen and Atkinson2023; Surowiec et al., Reference Surowiec, Snyder and Creanza2019). However, concerns have been raised about the appropriate application of such approaches to cultural evolution (Evans et al., Reference Evans, Greenhill, Watts, List, Botero, Gray and Kirby2021). Indeed, non-vertical transmission via macro-cultural processes such as innovation, conquest, exchange and competition may result in lower degrees of cultural diversity owing to cultural amalgamation and assimilation (Dyson-Hudson & Smith, Reference Dyson-Hudson and Smith1978; Marcus, Reference Marcus2008; Karin & Alon, Reference Karin and Alon2018; Yeh et al., Reference Yeh, Fogarty and Kandler2019; Turchin & Gavrilets, Reference Turchin and Gavrilets2021; and Figures S5 and S15). For Austronesian societies in particular, we observe that cultural amalgamation results in lower average cultural distances in the Religion class (Figure 2). Further, using traditional pairwise distances in the trait space without accounting for linkage may underestimate cultural variation (Figures S17–S19). Finally, failure to account for linkages between cultural traits and degree of vertical transmission may also introduce biases and errors into such analyses (Yeh et al., Reference Yeh, Fogarty and Kandler2019; Evans et al., Reference Evans, Greenhill, Watts, List, Botero, Gray and Kirby2021), similar to the biases introduced by genetic admixture to inferences of population history from genetic data (Royal et al., Reference Royal, Novembre, Fullerton, Goldstein, Long, Bamshad and Clark2010). Consistent with the findings of Holman et al. (Reference Holman, Walker, Rama and Wichmann2011) for linguistic data, we also find that relative values of the Q-residual (Gray et al., Reference Gray, Bryant and Greenhill2010) are sensitive to the choice of pairwise distance for cultural trait data. Thus, our results call for further research on non-vertical cultural transmission and the application of statistical methods used in population-genetic analyses to cultural evolution.
Supplementary material
The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2024.45.
Acknowledgements
We thank Laura Fortunato, Sven Kasser, Daniel Weissman, Uri Obolski, Alex Ioannidis and Tal Simon for their discussions and comments. The icons used in Figure 4 are from the noun project: the social organisation icon by user sun; the subsistence icon by user DinosoftLabs; the religion icon by user sofi; and the cultural interaction icon by user Ayan Sabbir.
Author contributions
JCM: software, formal analysis, data and software curation, wrote the original draft. JB-P: formal analysis and software. MWF: conceptualisation, supervision, formal analysis, funding acquisition. YR: conceptualisation, supervision, formal analysis, funding acquisition. All authors reviewed and edited the manuscript prior to submission.
Financial support
This research was supported in part by the John Templeton Foundation (MWF, YR), the Morrison Institute for Population and Resources Studies at Stanford University (MWF), the Minerva Stiftung Center for Lab Evolution (YR) and the Zuckerman STEM Leadership Program (JCM).
Conflict of interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest
Transparency and reproducibility
The authors have made the code used to produce these results available in the Zenodo repository (Macdonald et al., Reference Macdonald, Blanco-Portillo, Feldman and Ram2024a).
Data availability
The raw input data from EA and Pulotu is available through D-place (Kirby et al., Reference Kirby, Gray, Greenhill, Jordan, Gomes-Ng, Bibiko and Gavin2016, https://d-place.org). All output data produced by this work are available in Zenodo along with the software files (Macdonald et al., Reference Macdonald, Blanco-Portillo, Feldman and Ram2024a).