Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-18T03:21:13.576Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Life Stressors and Cognitive Styles in Children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 April 2014

Sebastiano Santostefano
Affiliation:
The Institute for Child and Adolescent Development, Inc.
Mª Angeles Quiroga Estévez
Affiliation:
Complutense University of Madrid
Susan Rooney Santostefano*
Affiliation:
The Institute for Child and Adolescent Development, Inc.
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to: Sebastiano SantostefanoInstitute for Child and Adolescent Development, Inc. 169 Grove Street. Wellesley, MA 02482 (USA). Fax: +781 2376815. E-mail: icadss@worldnet.att.net

Abstract

To explore the way that children's cognitive functioning relates to stressors they report experiencing in every day life, this study used the approach of cognitive control theory, which defines cognition as a set of mobile functions that, in serving adaptation, shift in their organization. Children (N = 93), ranging in age from 56 to 115 months, were administered individually the Life Stressor Interview and several cognitive control tasks. Children who reported being exposed to arguments and threatening gestures among adults made more errors when focusing attention while distracted by stimuli concerning nurture. Children who reported being upset by shootings and fights had more difficulty remembering test information depicting two persons in a shoot-out. The results are discussed in terms of the potential value of an approach that integrates cognitive activity with personality.

En este estudio se ha utilizado el enfoque de la teoría de los controles cognitivos para explorar cómo se relaciona el funcionamiento cognitivo de los niños con los factores estresantes de los que informan. Este enfoque define la cognición como un conjunto de funciones móviles que cambian en su organización, al servicio de la adaptación. Se evaluó individualmente a 93 niños y niñas de edades comprendidas entre 56 y 115 meses, utilizando el Life Stressor Interview y diversas tareas de control cognitivo. Los resultados obtenidos muestran que los niños que informan de haberse sentido afectados por discusiones y gestos amenazantes entre adultos, cometen más errores mientras centran la atención ante estímulos relacionados con el nutrimento. Los niños que informan de haberse sentido afectados por disparos y peleas, tuvieron más dificultad en recordar la información del estímulo que muestra dos personas en un tiroteo. Los resultados obtenidos se discuten en relación al valor que posee un enfoque que integra la actividad cognitiva y la personalidad.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2001

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Armsworth, M.W., & Holaday, M. (1993). The effects of psychological trauma on children and adolescents. Journal of Counseling and Development, 72, 4956.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borenstein, M., & Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis: A computer program. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bruner, J.S. (1992). Another look at New Look I. American Psychologist, 47, 780783.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calicchia, J.A., Moncata, S.J., & Santostefano, S. (1993). Cognitive control differences in violent juvenile inpatients. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 49, 731740.3.0.CO;2-F>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, M.F. (1992). A developmental assessment of the cognitive principal of leveling-sharpening in two interpersonal-emotional contexts: Aggression and affiliation. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Boston College, Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Cotugno, A.J. (1981). Cognitive controls and reading disabilities revisited. Psychology in the Schools, 18, 455458.3.0.CO;2-1>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cotugno, A.J. (1987). Cognitive control functioning in hyperactive and nonhyperactive children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 20, 563567.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edgcumbe, R.S., & Gaushon, A. (1985). Clinical comparisons of traumatic events and reactions. Bulletin of the Anna Freud Center, 8, 321.Google Scholar
Engelbrecht, P. (1993). A South African perspective on cognitive control therapy for learning disabled children. School Psychology International, 14, 111117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farver, J.M., & Frosch, D.L. (1996). L.A. stories: Aggression in preschoolers' spontaneous narratives after the riots of 1992. Child Development, 67, 1932.Google ScholarPubMed
Foldi, N.S., Jutagir, R., Davidoff, D., & Gould, T. (1992). Selective attention skills in Alzheimer disease. Journal of Gerontology, 47, 147153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frenkl-Brunswick, E. (1951). Personality theory and perception. In Blake, R. R. & Ramsey, G. V. (Eds.), Perception: An approach to personality (pp. 356420). New York: Ronald Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freud, S. (1961). The ego and the id. In Strachey, J. (Ed. and Trans.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 19, pp. 366). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1923).Google Scholar
Galatzer-Levy, R.M. (1994). Children, bad happenings and meanings. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 42, 9971000.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garbarino, J., & Kostelny, K. (1996). The effects of political violence on Palestinian children's behavior problems. Child Development, 67, 3345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gruber, H.E., Hammond, K.R., & Jessor, R. (Eds.). (1957). Contemporary approaches to cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Guthrie, G.D. (1967) Changes in cognitive functioning under stress: A study of plasticity in cognitive controls. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Clark University, Worcester, MA.Google Scholar
Janoff-Bulman, R. (1995). Victims of violence. In Everly, G.S. & Lating, J.M. (Eds.), Psychotraumatology. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, J.H. (1986). Life events as stressors in childhood and adolescence. Newbury Park, NJ: Sage.Google Scholar
Klein, G.S. (1951). The personal world through perception. In Blake, R.R. & Ramsey, G.V. (Eds.), Perception: An approach to personality (pp. 328355). New York: Ronald Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, G.S. (1954). Need and regulation. In Jones, M.R. (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 2, pp. 224274). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Klein, G.S. (1970). Perception, motives and personality. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
Klein, G.S., & Schlesinger, H.J. (1949). Where is the perceiver in perceptual theory? Journal of Personality, 18, 3247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lutgendorf, S.K., Antoni, M.H., & Kumar, M. (1994). Changes in cognitive coping strategies predict EBV-antibody titer change following a stressor. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38, 6378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macksoud, M.S., & Aber, J.L. (1996). The war experience and psychological development of children in Lebanon. Child Development, 67, 7088.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pynoos, R.S., Steinberg, A.M., & Goenjian, A. (1996). Traumatic stress in childhood and adolescence: Recent developments and current controversies. In Van der Kolk, B.A., McFarlane, A.C., & Weisaeth, L. (Eds.), Traumatic stress: The effects of overwhelming experience on mind, body, and society (pp. 331358). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Quiroga, M.A., López, V., Gómez, M.E., Fernández de la Vega, S., & Pérez, F. (1998). Diferencias interindividuales en los perfiles de desarrollo del estilo cognitivo al comenzar la escolaridad. In López, M.P. Sánchez & Quiroga, M.A. (Eds.), Perspecivas actuales en la investigación psicológica de las diferencias individuales (pp. 434440). Madrid: Editorial Centro de Estudios Ramón Areces.Google Scholar
Quiroga, M.A., Rodríguez, J., Nieto, R., Orozco, M., Salamanca, M., & Murillo, M.J. (1998). Perfiles diferenciales de estilo cognitivo ante situaciones de cambio como explicación de las diferencias en rendimiento: el paso del Colegio al Instituto. In López, M.P. Sánchez & Quiroga, M.A. (Eds.), Perspecivas actuales en la investigación psicológica de las diferencias individuales (pp. 441447). Madrid: Editorial Centro de Estudios Ramón Areces.Google Scholar
Quiroga, M.A., & Santostefano, S. (1999) Changes in fantasies of children concerning aggression and affiliation as reflected by the balance maintained between cognition and emotion over time. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Rieder, C., & Cicchetti, D. (1989). Organizational perceptive on cognitive-control functioning and cognitive-affective balance in maltreated children. Developmental Psychology, 25, 382393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogosch, F.A., Cicchetti, D., & Aber, J.L. (1995). The role of child maltreatment in early deviations in cognitive and affective processing abilities and later peer relationship problems. Developmental and Psychopathology, 7, 591609.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Santostefano, S. (1978). A biodevelopmental approach to clinical child psychology: Cognitive controls and cognitive control therapy. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Santostefano, S. (1985). Cognitive control therapy with children and adolescents. Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Santostefano, S. (1986). Cognitive controls, metaphors and contexts: An approach to cognition and emotion. In Bearision, D. & Zimiles, H. (Eds.), Thought and emotion (pp. 175210). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Santostefano, S. (1988). The Cognitive Control Battery. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Santostefano, S. (1991). Cognitive style as process coordinating outer space with inner self: Lessons from the past. In Wapner, S. & Demick, J. (Eds.), Field dependence-independence: Cognitive style across the life span (pp. 269288). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Santostefano, S. (1992a). The Leveling-Sharpening Friends, Shoot-out, and Trauma Tests: Manual of instructions and scoring. Wellesley, MA: ICADGoogle Scholar
Santostefano, S. (1992b). Life Stressor Interview: Manual of instructions and scoring. Wellesley, MA: ICAD.Google Scholar
Santostefano, S. (1995). Embodied meanings, cognition, and emotion: Probing how three are one. In Cicchetti, D. & Toth, S.L. (Eds.), Rochester Symposium on developmental psychopathology. Vol. 6: Emotion, cognition and representation. (pp. 59132). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.Google Scholar
Santostefano, S. (1998). A handbook of integrative psychotherapies for children and adolescents. Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson.Google Scholar
Santostefano, S., & Moncata, S. (1989). A psychoanalytic view of cognition within personality: Cognitive dysfunction and educating troubled youth. Residential Treatment for Children and Youth, 6, 4162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Santostefano, S. & Paley, E. (1964) Development of cognitive controls in children. Child Development, 35, 939949.Google ScholarPubMed
Santostefano, S., & Rieder, C. (1984). Cognitive controls and aggression in children: The concept of cognitive-affective balance. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52, 4656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shapiro, I.F. (1972). Cognitive controls and adaptation in children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Boston College, Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Van der Kolk, B.A., & Ducey, C.P. (1989). The psychological processing of traumatic experience: Rorschach patterns in PTSD. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 2, 259274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wertlieb, D. L. (1979). Cognitive organization, regulations of aggression and learning disorders in boys. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Boston University, Boston, MA.Google Scholar
White, S.H. (1991). The child as agent: Issues of cognitive style and personal design in human development. In Wapner, S. & Demick, J. (Eds.), Field dependence-independence: Cognitive style across the life span (pp. 724). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Wolitsky, D.L., & Wachtel, P.L. (1973). Personality and perception. In Wolman, B.B. (Ed.), Handbook of general psychology (pp. 826857). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar