Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-02T00:54:44.780Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Moving Forward (and Beyond) the Modularity Debate: A Network Perspective

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2022

Abstract

Modularity is one of the most important concepts used to articulate a theory of cognitive architecture. Over the last 30 years, the debate in many areas of the cognitive sciences and in philosophy of psychology about what modules are, and to what extent our cognitive architecture is modular, has made little progress. After providing a diagnosis of this lack of progress, this article suggests a remedy. It argues that the theoretical framework of network science can be brought to bear on the traditional modularity debate, facilitating our progress in articulating a good theory of the human cognitive architecture.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Philosophy of Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

I thank Filippo Rossi, Andy Clark, and Tim Bates for discussion of some of the ideas that motivated this article. Charles Rathkopf, Liz Irvine, Carlos Zednik, and Jan Sprenger read and commented on earlier drafts, for which I am sincerely grateful. Rhiannon Bolos was so kind to assist me with copyediting. A special thank you goes to Clark Barrett and an anonymous reviewer for detailed comments and suggestions. This work was supported by a grant from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) as part of the priority program New Frameworks of Rationality (SPP 1516).

References

Atkinson, Anthony P., and Wheeler, Michael. 2004. “The Grain of Domains: The Evolutionary-Psychological Case against Domain-General Cognition.” Mind and Language 19:147–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, H. Clark. 2005. “Enzymatic Computation and Cognitive Modularity.” Mind and Language 20:259–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, H. Clark 2009. “Where There Is an Adaptation, There Is a Domain: The Form-Function Fit in Information Processing.” In Foundations in Evolutionary Cognitive Neuroscience, ed. Platek, Steven M. and Shackelford, Todd K., 97116. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shackelford, Todd K. 2012. “A Hierarchical Model of the Evolution of Human Brain Specializations.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 109:10733–40.Google Scholar
Barrett, H. Clark, and Kurzban, Robert. 2006. “Modularity in Cognition: Framing the Debate.” Psychological Review 113:628–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bassett, Danielle S., Meyer-Lindenberg, Andreas, Achard, Sophie, Duke, Thomas, and Bullmore, Edward. 2006. “Adaptive Reconfiguration of Fractal Small-World Human Brain Functional Networks.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 103:19518–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechtel, William, and Abrahamsen, Adele. 2005. “Explanation: A Mechanist Alternative.” Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 36:421–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bilder, Robert M., Sabb, Fred W., Parker, D. Stott, Kalar, Donald, Chu, Wesley W., Fox, Jared, Freimer, Nelson B., and Poldrack, Russell A.. 2009. “Cognitive Ontologies for Neuropsychiatric Phenomics Research.” Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 14:419–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Börner, Katy, Sanyal, Soma, and Vespignani, Alessandro. 2007. “Network Science.” Annual Review of Information Science and Technology 41:537607.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buller, David J. 2005. Adapting Minds: Evolutionary Psychology and the Persistent Quest for Human Nature. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bullmore, Edward, and Sporns, Olaf. 2009. “Complex Brain Networks: Graph-Theoretical Analysis of Structural and Functional Systems.” Nature Reviews Neuroscience 10:186–98.Google ScholarPubMed
Bullmore, Edward, and Sporns, Olaf 2012. “The Economy of Brain Network Organization.” Nature Reviews Neuroscience 13:336–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carruthers, Peter. 2006. The Architecture of the Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, Zhang J., He, Yong, Rosa-Neto, Pedro, Germann, Jurgen, and Evans, Alan C.. 2008. “Revealing Modular Architecture of Human Brain Structural Networks by Using Cortical Thickness from MRI.” Cerebral Cortex 18:2374–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Churchland, Patricia S. 1986. Neurophilosophy: Toward a Unified Science of the Mind-Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cohen, Alexander L., Fair, Damien A., Dosenbach, Nico U. F., Miezin, Francis M., Dierker, Donna, Van Essen, David C., Schlaggar, Bradley L., and Petersen, Steven E.. 2008. “Defining Functional Areas in Individual Human Brains Using Resting State Functional Connectivity MRI.” Neuroimage 41:4557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, Max. 1999. “Modularity and Cognition.” Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3:115–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cosmides, Leda, and Tooby, John. 1995. “From Function to Structure: The Role of Evolutionary Biology and Computational Theories in Cognitive Neuroscience.” In The Cognitive Neurosciences, ed. Gazzaniga, Michael S., 11991210. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, Michael S. 2008a. “Can a General Deontic Logic Capture the Facts of Human Moral Reasoning? How the Mind Interprets Social Exchange Rules and Detects Cheaters.” In Moral Psychology, Vol. 1, The Evolution of Morality: Adaptations and Innateness, ed. Sinnott-Armstrong, Walter, 53119. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Sinnott-Armstrong, Walter 2008b. “When Falsification Strikes: A Reply to Fodor.” In Moral Psychology, Vol. 1, The Evolution of Morality: Adaptations and Innateness, ed. Sinnott-Armstrong, Walter, 143–64. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Eliasmith, Chris, Stewart, Terrence C., Choo, Xuan, Bekolay, Trevor, DeWolf, Travis, Tang, Yichuan, and Rasmussen, Daniel. 2012. “A Large-Scale Model of the Functioning Brain.” Science 338:1202–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ermer, Elsa, Cosmides, Leda, and Tooby, John. 2007. “Functional Specialization and the Adaptationist Program.” In The Evolution of Mind: Fundamental Questions and Controversies, ed. Gangestad, Steven W. and Simpson, Jeffry A., 153–59. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Fodor, Jerry A. 1983. The Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, Jerry A. 2000. The Mind Does Not Work That Way. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, Jerry A. 2008. “Comment on Cosmides and Tooby.” In Moral Psychology, Vol. 1, The Evolution of Morality: Adaptations and Innateness, ed. Sinnott-Armstrong, Walter, 137–41. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fortunato, Santo. 2010. “Community Detection in Graphs.” Physics Reports 486:75174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friston, Karl J. 1994. “Functional and Effective Connectivity in Neuroimaging: A Synthesis.” Human Brain Mapping 2:5678.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friston, Karl J. 2011. “Functional and Effective Connectivity: A Review.” Brain Connectivity 1:1336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friston, Karl J., and Price, Cathy J.. 2001. “Dynamic Representations and Generative Models of Brain Function.” Brain Research Bulletin 54:275–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friston, Karl J., and Price, Cathy J. 2011. “Modules and Brain Mapping.” Cognitive Neuropsychology 28:241–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallos, Lazaros K., Makse, Hernán A., and Sigman, Mariano. 2012. “A Small World of Weak Ties Provides Optimal Global Integration of Self-Similar Modules in Functional Brain Networks.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 109:2825–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golland, Yulia, Golland, Polina, Bentin, Shlomo, and Malach, Rafael. 2008. “Data-Driven Clustering Reveals a Fundamental Subdivision of the Human Cortex into Two Global Systems.” Neuropsychologia 46:540–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hagmann, Patric, Cammoun, Leila, Gigandet, Xavier, Meuli, Reto, Honey, Christopher J., Wedeen, Van J., and Sporns, Olaf. 2008. “Mapping the Structural Core of Human Cerebral Cortex.” PLoS Biology 6:e159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hampshire, Adam, Highfield, Roger R., Parkin, Beth, and Owen, Adrian M.. 2012. “Fractionating Human Intelligence.” Neuron 76:1225–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
He, Yong, Wang, Jinhui, Wang, Liang, Chen, Zhang J., Yan, Chaogan, Yang, Hong, Tang, Hehan, Zhu, Chaozhe, Gong, Qiyong, Zang, Yufeng, and Evans, Alan C.. 2009. “Uncovering Intrinsic Modular Organization of Spontaneous Brain Activity in Humans.” PLoS ONE 4: e5226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaplan, Jonathan M. 2002. “Historical Evidence and Human Adaptations.” Philosophy of Science 69 (Proceedings): S294S304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, Colin. 2012. “Cognitive Ontology and Region- versus Network-Oriented Analyses.” Philosophy of Science 79:952–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lickliter, Robert, and Honeycutt, Hunter. 2003a. “Developmental Dynamics and Contemporary Evolutionary Psychology: Status quo or Irreconcilable Views? Replies to Critics.” Psychological Bulletin 129:866–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lickliter, Robert, and Honeycutt, Hunter 2003b. “Developmental Dynamics: Toward a Biologically Plausible Evolutionary Psychology.” Psychological Bulletin 129:819–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lloyd, Elisabeth A., and Feldman, Marcus W.. 2002. “Evolutionary Psychology: A View from Evolutionary Biology.” Psychological Inquiry 13:150–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Machery, Edouard. Forthcoming. “Discovery and Confirmation in Evolutionary Psychology.” In Oxford Handbook of Philosophy of Psychology, ed. Jesse J. Prinz. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Machery, Edouard, and Barrett, Clark H.. 2006. “Debunking Adapting Minds.Philosophy of Science 73:232–46.Google Scholar
Marr, David. 1982. Vision. San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
Marr, David, Ullman, Shimon, and Poggio, Tomaso. 1979. “Bandpass Channels, Zero-Crossings and Early Visual Information Processing.” Journal of the Optical Society of America 69:914–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McIntosh, A. Randall. 2000. “Towards a Network Theory of Cognition.” Neural Networks 13:861–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meunier, David, Lambiotte, Renaud, and Bullmore, Edward. 2010. “Modular and Hierarchically Modular Organization of Brain Networks.” Frontiers in Neuroscience 4:200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meunier, David, Lambiotte, Renaud, Fornito, Alex, Ersche, Karen D., and Bullmore, Edward. 2009. “Hierarchical Modularity in Human Brain Functional Networks.” Frontiers in Neuroinformatics 3:37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newman, Mark E. J. 2006. “Modularity and Community Structure in Networks.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 103:85778696.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newman, Mark E. J., and Girvan, Michelle. 2004. “Finding and Evaluating Community Structure in Networks.” Physical Review E 69:026113.Google Scholar
Passingham, Richard E., Stephan, Klaas E., and Kötter, Rolf. 2002. “The Anatomical Basis of Functional Localization in the Cortex.” Nature Review Neuroscience 3:606–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinker, Steven. 1997. How the Mind Works. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Pinker, Steven 2005. “So How Does the Mind Work?Mind and Language 20:124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Price, Cathy J., and Friston, Karl J.. 2005. “Functional Ontologies for Cognition: The Systematic Definition of Structure and Function.” Cognitive Neuropsychology 22:262–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richardson, Robert C. 2007. Evolutionary Psychology as Maladapted Psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubinov, Mikail, and Sporns, Olaf. 2010. “Complex Network Measures of Brain Connectivity: Uses and Interpretations.” Neuroimage 52:1059–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samuels, Richard. 2000. “Massively Modular Minds: Evolutionary Psychology and Cognitive Architecture.” In Evolution and the Human Mind: Modularity, Language and Meta-cognition, ed. Carruthers, Peter and Chamberlain, Andrew, 1346. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schulz, Armin W. 2011. “Heuristic Evolutionary Psychology.” In Philosophy of Behavioral Biology, ed. Plaisance, Kathryn S. and Reydon, Thomas A. C., 217–34. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
Segal, Gabriel. 1996. “The Modularity of Theory of Mind.” In Theories of Theories of Mind, ed. Carruthers, Peter and Smith, Peter K., 141–57. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sperber, Dan. 1994. “The Modularity of Thought and the Epidemiology of Representations.” In Mapping the Mind: Domain-Specificity in Cognition and Culture, ed. Hirschfeld, Lawrence A. and Gelman, Susan A., 3967. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sperber, Dan, and Girotto, Vittorio. 2003. “Does the Selection Task Detect Cheater Detection?” In From Mating to Mentality: Evaluating Evolutionary Psychology, ed. Sterelny, Kim and Fitness, Julie, 197226. New York: Psychology.Google Scholar
Sporns, Olaf. 2007. “Brain Connectivity.” Scholarpedia 2 (10): 4695.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sporns, Olaf 2011. Networks of the Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Sporns, Olaf, Tononi, Giulio, and Kötter, Rolf. 2005. “The Human Connectome: A Structural Description of the Human Brain.” PLoS Computational Biology 1:245–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sterelny, Kim, and Griffiths, Paul E.. 1999. Sex and Death: An Introduction to Philosophy of Biology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tononi, Giulio, Sporns, Olaf, and Edelman, Gerald M.. 1994. “A Measure for Brain Complexity: Relating Functional Segregation and Integration in the Nervous System.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 91:5033–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tooby, John, and Cosmides, Leda. 1992. “The Psychological Foundations of Culture.” In The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture, ed. Barkow, Jerome H., Cosmides, Leda, and Tooby, John, 19136. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Barkow, Jerome H., Cosmides, Leda, and Tooby, John 2005. “Conceptual Foundations of Evolutionary Psychology.” In The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology, ed. Buss, David M., 567. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Tooby, John, Cosmides, Leda, and Barrett, Clark H.. 2003. “The Second Law of Thermodynamics Is the First Law of Psychology: Evolutionary Developmental Psychology and the Theory of Tandem, Coordinated Inheritances; Comment on Lickleiter and Honeycutt (2003).” Psychological Bulletin 129:858–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Twyman, Alexandra D., and Newcombe, Nora S.. 2010. “Five Reasons to Doubt the Existence of a Geometric Module.” Cognitive Science 34:1315–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagner, Günter P., Mezey, Jason, and Calabretta, Raffaele. 2005. “Natural Selection and the Origin of Modules.” In Modularity: Understanding the Development and Evolution of Natural Complex Systems, ed. Callebaut, Werner and Rasking-Gutman, Diego, 3350. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wagner, Günter P., Pavlicev, Mihaela, and Cheverud, James M.. 2007. “The Road to Modularity.” Nature Review Genetics 8:921–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed