Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-16T22:50:54.802Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Strong and clitic pronouns in monolingual and bilingual acquisition of French and Italian*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 March 2008

KATRIN SCHMITZ*
Affiliation:
University of Wuppertal
NATASCHA MÜLLER
Affiliation:
University of Wuppertal
*
Address for correspondence: Dr. Katrin Schmitz, c/o University of Wuppertal, Gaußstraße 20, 42119 Wuppertal, Germanykschmitz@uni-wuppertal.de

Abstract

The present article investigates the acquisition of the pronominal systems by French and Italian monolingual children and by bilingual German–French and German–Italian children, demonstrating a stable asymmetry: object and reflexive clitics are acquired later than nominative clitics and strong subject and object pronouns. We will widen the scope of former investigations to include the acquisition of strong pronouns and argue that the observed asymmetry can be accounted for if we combine the external (categorial status) and internal syntax of pronouns (internal structure). In particular, we argue for the relevance of the absence/presence of a nominal layer (N-layer) in the internal structure of a pronoun. This approach can account for the observation that pronouns containing an N-layer, i.e., strong subject pronouns, subject clitics and strong object pronouns, are acquired simultaneously and earlier than pronouns which lack the N-layer, i.e., object clitics and reflexive clitics.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

We are grateful for the comments of the anonymous reviewers. We would also like to thank Jeff Richards for correcting our English.

References

Belletti, A. 1999. Italian/Romance Clitics: Structure and derivation. In van Riemsdijk, (ed.), pp. 543–579.Google Scholar
Burzio, L. 1986. Italian syntax: A government-binding approach. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cardinaletti, A. 1991. On pronoun movement: The Italian dative loro. Probus, 3 (2), 127153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cardinaletti, A. & Starke, M. 1999. The typology of structural deficiency: A case study of the three classes of pronouns. In van Riemsdijk, (ed.), pp. 145–233.Google Scholar
Cardinaletti, A. & Starke, M. 2000. Overview: The grammar (and acquisition) of clitics. In Powers, & Hamann, (eds.), pp. 165–186.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1981. Lectures on government and binding: The Pisa lectures. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1982. Some concepts and consequences of the theory of government and binding. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H. (ed.) 1996. Generative perspectives on language acquisition. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crysmann, B. & Müller, N. 2000. On the non-parallelism in the acquisition of reflexive and non-reflexive object clitics. In Powers, & Hamann, (eds.), pp. 207–236.Google Scholar
Déchaine, R.-M. & Wiltschko, M. 2002. Decomposing pronouns. Linguistic Inquiry, 33 (3), 409442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dekkers, J., van der Leeuw, F. & van der Weijer, J. (eds.) 2000. Optimality theory: Morphology, syntax and acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabriel, C. & Müller, N. 2005. Zu den romanischen Pronominalklitika: Kategorialer Status und syntaktische Derivation. In Kaiser, G. (ed.), Deutsche Romanistik – generativ, pp. 161180. Tübingen: Narr.Google Scholar
Gawlitzek-Maiwald, I. & Tracy, R. 1996. Bilingual boot–invertedstrapping. Linguistics, 34, 901926.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grondin, N. & White, L. 1996. Functional categories in child L2 acquisition of French. Language Acquisition, 5, 134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamann, C. 2002. From syntax to discourse: Pronominal clitics, null subjects and infinitives in child language. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamann, C., Cronel-Ohayon, S., Dubé, S., Frauenfelder, U., Rizzi, L., Starke, M. & Zesiger, P. 2003. Aspects of grammatical development in young French children with SLI. Developmental Science, 6, 151158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamann, C., Rizzi, L. & Frauenfelder, U. 1996. On the acquisition of subject and object clitics in French. In Clahsen, (ed.), pp. 309–334.Google Scholar
Hulk, A. & Müller, N. 2000. Crosslinguistic influence at the interface between syntax and pragmatics. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 3 (3), 227244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jakubowicz, C., Müller, N., Ok-Kyung, K., Riemer, B. & Rigaut, C. 1996. On the acquisition of the pronominal system in French and German. In Springfellow, A., Cahana-Amitay, D., Hughes, E. & Zukowski, A. (eds.), Proceedings of the 20th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, pp. 374385. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Jakubowicz, C., Müller, N., Riemer, B. & Rigaut, C. 1997. The case of subject and object omissions in French and German. In Hughes, E., Hughes, M. & Greenhill, A. (eds.), Proceedings of the 21st Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, pp. 331342. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Jakubowicz, C., Nash, L., Rigaut, C. & Gérard, Ch.-L. 1998. Determiners and pronouns in French-speaking children with SLI. Language Acquisition, 7, 113160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jakubowicz, C. & Rigaut, C. 2000. L'acquisition des clitiques nominatifs en français. The Canadian Journal of Linguistics/Revue canadienne de linguistique, 45 (1–2), 119159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kato, M. 1999. Strong and weak pronominals in the null-subject parameter. Probus, 11, 137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kayne, R. 1975. French syntax: The transformational cycle. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
López, L. 2003. Steps for a well-adjusted dislocation. Studia Linguistica, 57 (3), 193231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. & Snow, C. 1985. The child language data exchange system. Journal of Child Language, 12, 271296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manzini, R. 1986. On Italian SI. In Borer, H. (ed.), The syntax of pronominal clitics, pp. 241262. New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meisel, J. M. 1990. INFL-ection: Subjects and subject–verb agreement. In Meisel, J. M. (ed.), Two first languages: Early grammatical development in bilingual children (Studies in Language Acquisition 10), pp. 237298. Dordrecht: Foris.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meisel, J. M. 1997. The acquisition of syntax of negation in French and German: Contrasting first and second language development. Second Language Research, 13, 227263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer-Lübke, W. 1894. Grammatik der romanischen Sprachen (vol. 2): Formenlehre. Leipzig: Reisland.Google Scholar
Müller, N. 1998. Transfer in bilingual first language acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1 (3), 151171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller, N. 2004. Null-arguments in bilingual children: French topics. In Paradis, J. & Prévost, P. (eds.), The acquisition of French in different contexts: Focus on functional categories, pp. 275304. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller, N., Cantone, K., Kupisch, T. & Schmitz, K. 2002. Zum Spracheneinfluss im bilingualen Erstspracherwerb: Italienisch–Deutsch. Linguistische Berichte, 190, 157206.Google Scholar
Müller, N., Crysmann, B. & Kaiser, G. 1996. Interactions between the acquisition of French object drop and the development of the C-system. Language Acquisition, 5 (1), 3563.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller, N. & Hulk, A. 2000. Crosslinguistic influence in bilingual children: Object omissions and root infinitives. In Howell, C., Fish, S. A. & Keith-Lucas, T. (eds.), Proceedings of the 24th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, pp. 546557. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Müller, N. & Hulk, A. 2001. Crosslinguistic influence in bilingual language acquisition: Italian and French as recipient languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4 (1), 121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller, N., Kupisch, T., Schmitz, K. & Cantone, K. 2006. Einführung in die Mehrsprachigkeitsforschung. Tübingen: Narr.Google Scholar
Müller, N. & Penner, Z. 1996. Early subordination: The acquisition of free morphology in French, German, and Swiss German. Linguistics, 34, 133165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller, N. & Pillunat, A. (in press). Balanced bilingual children with two weak languages: A French–German case study. To appear in Guijarro-Fuentes, P., Larrañaga, P. & Clibbens, J. (eds.), First language acquisition of morphology and syntax: Perspectives across languages and learners. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Müller, N., Schmitz, K., Cantone, K. & Kupisch, T. 2006. Null arguments in monolingual children: A comparison of Italian and French. In Torrens, V. & Escobar, L. (eds.), The acquisition of syntax in Romance languages, pp. 6993. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., Crago, M. & Genesee, F. 2003. Object clitics as a clinical marker of SLI in French: Evidence from French–English bilingual children. In Beachley, B., Brown, A. & Conlin, F. (eds.), Proceedings of the 27th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (vol. 2), pp. 638649. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Pillunat, A., Schmitz, K. & Müller, N. 2006. Die Schnittstelle Syntax–Pragmatik: Subjektauslassungen bei bilingual deutsch–französisch aufwachsenden Kindern. Zeitschrift für Literaturwissenschaft und Linguistik (LiLi), 36 (143), 724.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Powers, S. M. & Hamann, C. (eds.) 2000. The acquisition of scrambling and cliticization. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radford, A. 1990. Syntactic theory and the acquisition of English syntax: The nature of early child grammars of English. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Raposo, E. 1998. Definite/zero alternations in Portuguese: Towards a unification of topic constructions. In Schwegler, A., Tranel, B. & Uribe-Etxebarria, M. (eds.), Romance linguistics: Theoretical perspectives, pp. 197212. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Riemsdijk, H. (ed.) 1999. Clitics in the languages of Europe. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Roberge, Y. 1991. On the recoverability of null objects. In Wanner, D. & Kibbee, D. A. (eds.), New analyses in Romance linguistics: Selected papers from the XVIIIth Linguistic Symposium on Romance Languages, pp. 299312. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ronjat, J. 1913. Le développement du langage observé chez un enfant bilingue. Paris: Champion.Google Scholar
Schmitz, K. 2006a. Indirect objects and dative case in monolingual German and bilingual German/Romance language acquisition. In Hole, D., Abraham, W. & Meinunger, A. (eds.), Datives and other cases, pp. 240267. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Schmitz, K. 2006b. Zweisprachigkeit im Fokus: Der Erwerb der Verben mit zwei Objekten durch bilingual deutsch–französisch und deutsch–italienisch aufwachsende Kinder. Tübingen: Narr.Google Scholar
Shlonsky, U. 1997. Clause structure and word order in Hebrew and Arabic. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tesar, B. & Smolensky, P. 1998. Learnability in optimality theory. Linguistic Inquiry, 29, 229268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tiedemann, C. 1999. Erwerb des italienischen Pronominalsystems. Master's thesis, University of Hamburg.Google Scholar
White, L. 1996. Clitics in L2 French. In Clahsen, (ed.), pp. 335–368.Google Scholar
Yang, C. D. 1999. A selectionist theory of language acquisition. In ACL anthology, 429–435. http://acl.ldc.upenn.edu/P/P99/P99-1055.pdf (accessed 7 February 2008).Google Scholar
Zribi-Hertz, A. 1987. La réflexivité ergative en français moderne. Le Français moderne, 55, 2354.Google Scholar
Zubizarreta, M.-L. 1982. On the relationship of the lexicon to syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Zushi, M. 2003. Null arguments: The case of Japanese and Romance. Lingua, 113, 559604.CrossRefGoogle Scholar