Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-06T18:35:27.812Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - The mad (creative) genius: what do we know after a century of historiometric research?

from Part I - Creativity and mental illness: the state of the field

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 August 2014

Dean Keith Simonton
Affiliation:
University of California, Davis
James C. Kaufman
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
Get access

Summary

On November 19, 1909, Frederick A. Woods, a geneticist at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology (MIT), coined a “new name for a new science.” This he called historiometry, which covers when “the facts of history of a personal nature have been subjected to statistical analysis by some more or less objective method” (p. 703). Woods (1911) later identified historiometry not just as a science but an “exact science” and claimed that the technique was particularly well suited for the scientific “psychology of genius.” In the original article, Woods (1909) also listed a dozen examples of historiometric inquiries that appeared before the method had acquired a formal name. The list included Francis Galton’s (1869) Hereditary genius, Alphonse de Candolle’s (1873) Histoire des sciences et des savants depuis deux siècles, James McKeen Cattell’s (1903) “A statistical study of eminent men,” and Havelock Ellis’ (1904) A study of British genius. Although Wood intended his list to be comprehensive, he actually overlooked the first bona fide historiometric study published by Adolphe Quételet (1835/1968) more than a third of a century before Galton’s (1869) book (cf. Galton, 1865). Of course, Woods’ (1909) bibliography could not possibly encompass examples of historiometric research published in the century or so since the method acquired a name. Even so, among the most notable examples are Lewis M. Terman (1917), Catharine Cox (1926), Edward L. Thorndike (1950), and R. B. Cattell (1963). It is noteworthy that many historiometricians are themselves noteworthy, and some could themselves count as genuine geniuses – but I will not name names.

If historiometry is truly useful in the scientific study of genius, then it must be able to address some of the central questions in the psychology of genius (Simonton, 2009a). Is genius born or made? Is genius generic or domain specific? Is genius isolated or necessarily situated in a sociocultural context? Is genius mad? Because this chapter is part of a volume devoted to creativity and mental illness, it is the last question that I address here. What do historiometric investigations tell us about the relation between genius and madness – and especially the relation between creative genius and madness? But before I can review the main empirical results dealing with this question, I first must give a brief overview of the methods involved.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2014

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barron, F. X. (1963). Creativity and psychological health: Origins of personal vitality and creative freedom. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.
Candolle, A. de (1873). Histoire des sciences et des savants depuis deux siècles [History of science and scientists for two centuries]. Geneve: Georg.
Carson, S. H. (2011). Creativity and psychopathology: A shared vulnerability model. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 56, 144–153.Google Scholar
Cattell, J. M. (1903). A statistical study of eminent men. Popular Science Monthly, 62, 359–377.Google Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1963). The personality and motivation of the researcher from measurements of contemporaries and from biography. In Taylor, C. W. and Barron, F. (Eds.), Scientific creativity: Its recognition and development (pp. 119–131). New York: Wiley.
Cox, C. (1926). The early mental traits of three hundred geniuses. Stanford University Press.
Csikszentmihályi, M. (1997). Creativity: Flow and the psychology of discovery and invention. New York: HarperCollins.
Ellis, H. (1904). A study of British genius. London: Hurst & Blackett.
Galton, F. (1865). Hereditary talent and character. Macmillan’s Magazine, 12, 157–166, 318–327.Google Scholar
Galton, F. (1869). Hereditary genius: An inquiry into its laws and consequences. London: Macmillan.
Goertzel, M. G., Goertzel, V. and Goertzel, T. G. (1978). 300 eminent personalities: A psychosocial analysis of the famous. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Goertzel, V. and Goertzel, M. G. (1962). Cradles of eminence. Boston, MA: Little, Brown.
Hasenfus, N., Martindale, C. and Birnbaum, D. (1983). Psychological reality of cross-media artistic styles. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 841–863.Google Scholar
Juda, A. (1949). The relationship between highest mental capacity and psychic abnormalities. American Journal of Psychiatry, 106, 296–307.Google Scholar
Karlsson, J. I. (1970). Genetic association of giftedness and creativity with schizophrenia. Hereditas, 66, 177–182.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C. (2000–2001). Genius, lunatics and poets: Mental illness in prize-winning authors. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 20, 305–314.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C. (2001). The Sylvia Plath effect: Mental illness in eminent creative writers. Journal of Creative Behavior, 35, 37–50.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C. (2003). The cost of the muse: Poets die young. Death Studies, 27, 813–821.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C. (2005). The door that leads into madness: Eastern European poets and mental illness. Creativity Research Journal, 17, 99–103.Google Scholar
Ko, Y. and Kim, J. (2008). Scientific geniuses’ psychopathology as a moderator in the relation between creative contribution types and eminence. Creativity Research Journal, 20, 251–261.Google Scholar
Kuhn, T. S. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions (2nd edn.). University of Chicago Press.
Lombroso, C. (1891). The man of genius. London: Scott.
Ludwig, A. M. (1990). Alcohol input and creative output. British Journal of Addiction, 85, 953–963.Google Scholar
Ludwig, A. M. (1992a). Creative achievement and psychopathology: Comparison among professions. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 46, 330–356.Google Scholar
Ludwig, A. M. (1992b). The Creative Achievement Scale. Creativity Research Journal, 5, 109–124.
Ludwig, A. M. (1995). The price of greatness: Resolving the creativity and madness controversy. New York: Guilford Press.
Ludwig, A. M. (1998). Method and madness in the arts and sciences. Creativity Research Journal, 11, 93–101.Google Scholar
Ludwig, A. M. (2002). King of the mountain: The nature of political leadership. Lexington, KY: University Press of Kentucky.
MacKinnon, D. W. (1978). In search of human effectiveness. Buffalo, NY: Creative Education Foundation.
Martindale, C. (1972). Father absence, psychopathology, and poetic eminence. Psychological Reports, 31, 843–847.Google Scholar
Martindale, C. (1975). Romantic progression: The psychology of literary history. Washington, DC: Hemisphere.
Martindale, C. (1990). The clockwork muse: The predictability of artistic styles. New York: Basic Books.
Miles, C. C. and Wolfe, L. S. (1936). Childhood physical and mental health records of historical geniuses. Psychological Monographs, 47, 390–400.Google Scholar
Murray, C. (2003). Human accomplishment: The pursuit of excellence in the arts and sciences, 800 B.C. to 1950. New York: HarperCollins.
Planck, M. (1949). Scientific autobiography and other papers (Gaynor, F., Trans.). New York: Philosophical Library.
Post, F. (1994). Creativity and psychopathology: A study of 291 world-famous men. British Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 22–34.Google Scholar
Post, F. (1996). Verbal creativity, depression and alcoholism: An investigation of one hundred American and British writers. British Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 545–555.Google Scholar
Quételet, A. (1968). A treatise on man and the development of his faculties. New York: Franklin. (Reprint of 1842 Edinburgh translation of 1835 French original.)
Raskin, E. A. (1936). Comparison of scientific and literary ability: A biographical study of eminent scientists and men of letters of the nineteenth century. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 31, 20–35.Google Scholar
Runyan, W. M. (1981). Why did Van Gogh cut off his ear? The problem of alternative explanations in psychobiography. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 1070–1077.Google Scholar
Schaller, M. (1997). The psychological consequences of fame: Three tests of the self-consciousness hypothesis. Journal of Personality, 65, 291–309.Google Scholar
Schlesinger, J. (2009). Creative mythconceptions: A closer look at the evidence for the “mad genius” hypothesis. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3, 62–72.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1975). Age and literary creativity: A cross-cultural and transhistorical survey. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 6, 259–277.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1986). Popularity, content, and context in 37 Shakespeare plays. Poetics, 15, 493–510.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1990). Psychology, science, and history: An introduction to historiometry. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Simonton, D. K. (1992). Gender and genius in Japan: Feminine eminence in masculine culture. Sex Roles, 27, 101–119.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1997). Achievement domain and life expectancies in Japanese civilization. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 44, 103–114.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1999). Significant samples: The psychological study of eminent individuals. Psychological Methods, 4, 425–451.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2009a). Historiometry in personality and social psychology. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 3, 49–63.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2009b). Varieties of (scientific) creativity: A hierarchical model of disposition, development, and achievement. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 441–452.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2010a). The curious case of Catharine Cox: The 1926 dissertation and her Miles-Wolfe 1936 follow-up. History of Psychology, 13, 205–206.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2010b). So you want to become a creative genius? You must be crazy! In Cropley, D., Kaufman, J., Cropley, A. and Runco, M. (Eds.), The dark side of creativity (pp. 218–234). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Simonton, D. K. and Song, A. V. (2009). Eminence, IQ, physical and mental health, and achievement domain: Cox’s 282 geniuses revisited. Psychological Science, 20, 429–434.Google Scholar
Terman, L. M. (1917). The intelligence quotient of Francis Galton in childhood. American Journal of Psychology, 28, 209–215.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1950). Traits of personality and their intercorrelations as shown in biographies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 41, 193–216.Google Scholar
Woods, F. A. (1909). A new name for a new science. Science, 30, November 19, 703–704.Google Scholar
Woods, F. A. (1911). Historiometry as an exact science. Science, 33, April 14, 568–574.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×