Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-cfpbc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-20T00:05:03.762Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - “Traditional” foraging behaviors of brown and black rats (Rattus norvegicus and Rattus rattus)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 October 2009

Bennett G. Galef Jr.
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario L8S 4K1, Canada
Dorothy M. Fragaszy
Affiliation:
University of Georgia
Susan Perry
Affiliation:
University of California, Los Angeles
Get access

Summary

The brown rat, in particular, appears especially able to develop local traditions, more so perhaps than other more-closely examined mammals, possibly including the anthropoids.

Steiniger, 1950, p. 368

Introduction

Imagine, if you will, an energetic, young graduate student who has established a study site near Para, Brazil, where she spends 3 years observing a geographically isolated population of capuchin monkeys that no other primatologist has looked at. Imagine further that our graduate student soon finds, to her great surprise and pleasure, that all of the members of one troop of capuchins at Para, unlike any previously studied capuchins, regularly hunt and eat small lizards. Many months of demanding field work show that the lizards are the source of more than 20% of the calories and 36% of the protein ingested by troop members.

Discovering a complex, biologically meaningful pattern of behavior that is unique to a particular population of monkeys would be a significant event in the career of any behavioral scientist. Surely, before very long, our imaginary graduate student is going to want to tell her colleagues, and quite possibly members of the media as well, about her discovery. To do so, she is going to have to decide how to refer to the unusual behavior that her field studies have documented.

If our imaginary graduate student were to make the conventional choice, and there is little reason to doubt that she would, she would soon be referring to the lizard hunting she has observed as “cultural”, as a “tradition” of the capuchins at Para.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Biology of Traditions
Models and Evidence
, pp. 159 - 186
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aisner, R. and Terkel, J. 1992. Ontogeny of pine-cone opening behaviour in the black rat (Rattus rattus). Animal Behaviour, 44, 327–336CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnett, S. A. 1958. Experiments on “neophobia” in wild and laboratory rats. British Journal of Psychology, 49, 195–201CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowman, R. I. and Billeb, S. I. 1965. Blood-eating in a Galapagos finch. Living Bird, 4, 29–44Google Scholar
Bronstein, P. M., Levine, M. J., and Marcus, M. 1975. A rat's first bite: the non-genetic, cross-generational transfer of information. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 89, 295–298CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, R. W. and Byrne, J. M. E. 1994. The complex leaf-gathering skills of mountain gorillas (Gorilla g. beringei): variability and standardization. American Journal of Primatology, 31, 241–261CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calhoun, J. B. 1962. The Ecology and Sociology of the Norway Rat. Bethesda, MD: US Department of Health, Education, and Welfare
Caro, T. M. and Hauser, M. D. 1992. Is there teaching in nonhuman animals. Quarterly Review of Biology, 67, 151–174CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Church, R. M. 1957. Two procedures for the establishment of “imitative behavior”. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 50, 315–318CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cottam, C. 1948. Aquatic habits of the Norway rat. Journal of Mammalogy, 29, 299CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flandera, V. and Novakova, V. 1974. Effect of the mother on the development of aggressive behavior in rats. Developmental Psychobiology, 8, 49–54CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fragaszy, D. M. and Visalberghi, E. 1996. Social learning in monkeys: primate primacy reconsidered. In Social Learning and Imitation: The Roots of Culture, ed. C. M. Heyes and B. G. Galef Jr., pp. 65–84. New York: Academic Press
Fragaszy, D. M. and Visalberghi, E. 2001. Recognizing a swan: socially-biased learning. Psychologia, 44, 82–98Google Scholar
Galef, B. G. Jr. 1970. Aggression and timidity: responses to novelty in feral Norway rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 75, 358–362CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galef, B. G. Jr. 1976. Social transmission of acquired behavior: a discussion of tradition and social learning in vertebrates. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 6, 77–100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galef, B. G., Jr. 1977. Mechanisms for the social transmission of food preferences from adult to weanling rats. In Learning Mechanisms in Food Selection, ed. L. M. Barker, M. Best and M. Domjan, pp. 123–150. Waco, TX: Baylor University Press
Galef, B. G. Jr. 1980. Diving for food: analysis of a possible case of social learning in wild rats (Rattus norvegicus). Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 94, 416–425CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galef, B. G. Jr. 1981. Development of olfactory control of feeding-site selection in rat pups. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 95, 615–622CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galef, B. G. Jr. 1984. Reciprocal heuristics: a discussion of the study of learned behavior in laboratory and field. Learning and Motivation, 15, 479–493CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galef, B. G., Jr. 1985. Direct and indirect behavioral pathways to the social transmission of food avoidance. In Experimental Assessments and Clinical Applications of Conditioned Food Aversions, ed. P. Bronstein and N. S. Braveman, pp. 203–215. New York: New York Academy of Sciences
Galef, B. G. Jr. 1986a. Social interaction modifies learned aversions, sodium appetite, and both palatability and handling-time induced dietary preference in rats (Rattus norvegicus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 100, 432–439CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galef, B. G., Jr. 1986b. Olfactory communication among rats of information concerning distant diets. In Chemical Signals in Vertebrates, Vol. IV: Ecology, Evolution, and Comparative Biology, ed. D. Duvall, D. Muller-Schwarze, and R. M. Silverstein, pp. 487–505. New York: Plenum Press
Galef, B. G., Jr. 1988. Communication of information concerning distant diets in a social, central-place foraging species (Rattus norvegicus). In Social Learning: Psychological and Biological Perspectives, ed. T. R. Zentall and B. G. Galef Jr., pp. 119–140. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Galef, B. G. Jr. 1992. The question of animal culture. Human Nature, 3, 157–178CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galef, B. G. Jr. 1995. Why behaviour patterns that animals learn socially are locally adaptive. Animal Behaviour, 49, 1325–1334CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galef, B. G., Jr. 1996a. Social influences on food preferences and feeding behaviors of vertebrates. In Why We Eat What We Eat, ed. E. Capaldi, pp. 207–232. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association
Galef, B. G., Jr. 1996b. Social enhancement of food preferences in Norway rats. In Social Learning and Imitation: The Roots of Culture, ed. C. M. Heyes and B. G. Galef, Jr., pp. 49–64. New York: Academic Press
Galef, B. G., Jr. 1996c. Tradition in animals: field observations and laboratory analyses. In Readings in Animal Cognition, ed. M. Bekoff and D. Jamieson, pp. 91–106. Cambridge: MIT Press
Galef, B. G. Jr. and Allen, C. 1995. A new model system for studying animal traditions. Animal Behaviour, 50, 705–717CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galef, B. G. Jr. and Beck, M. 1985. Aversive and attractive marking of toxic and safe foods by Norway rats. Behavioral and Neural Biology, 43, 298–310CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galef, B. G. Jr. and Buckley, L. L 1996. Use of foraging trails by Norway rats. Animal Behaviour, 51, 765–771CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galef, B. G. Jr. and Clark, M. M 1971a. Parent–offspring interactions determine time and place of first ingestion of solid food by wild rat pups. Psychonomic Science, 25, 5–16CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galef, B. G. Jr. and Clark, M. M 1971b. Social factors in the poison avoidance and feeding behavior of wild and domesticated rat pups. Journal of Comparative andPhysiological Psychology, 25, 341–357CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galef, B. G. Jr. and Heiber, L. 1976. The role of residual olfactory cues in the determination of feeding site selection and exploration patterns of domestic rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 90, 727–739CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galef, B. G. Jr., and Henderson, P. W. 1972. Mother's milk: a determinant of food preferences of weaning rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 78, 213–219CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galef, B. G. Jr. and Sherry, D. F 1973. Mother's milk: a medium for transmission of information about mother's diet. Journal of Comparative and PhysiologicalPsychology, 83, 374–378Google Scholar
Galef, B. G. Jr. and Stein, M. 1985. Demonstrator influence on observer diet preference: analyses of critical social interactions and olfactory signals. Animal Learning and Behavior, 13, 131–138CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galef, B. G. Jr. and Whiskin, E. E 1992. Social transmission of information about multiflavored foods. Animal Behaviour, 20, 56–62CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galef, B. G. Jr. and Whiskin, E. E 1997. Effects of asocial learning on longevity of food-preference traditions. Animal Behaviour, 53, 1313–1322CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galef, B. G. Jr. and Whiskin, E. E 1998a. Limits on social influence on food choices of Norway rats. Animal Behaviour, 56, 1015–1020CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galef, B. G. Jr. and Whiskin, E. E 1998b. Determinants of the longevity of socially learned food preferences of Norway rats. Animal Behaviour, 55, 967–975CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galef, B. G. Jr. and Whiskin, E. E 2001. Interaction of social and asocial learning in food preferences of Norway rats. Animal Behaviour, 62, 41–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galef, B. G. Jr. and Wigmore, S. W 1983. Transfer of information concerning distant foods: a laboratory investigation of the “information-centre” hypothesis. Animal Behaviour, 31, 748–758CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galef, B. G. Jr., Kennett, D. J., and Wigmore, S. W. 1984. Transfer of information concerning distant foods in rats: a robust phenomenon. Animal Learning and Behavior, 12, 292–296CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galef, B. G. Jr., Kennett, D. J., and Stein, M. 1985. Demonstrator influence on observer diet preference: effects of simple exposure and presence of a demonstrator. Animal Learning and Behavior, 13, 25–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galef, B. G. Jr., Mason, J. R., Pretti, G., and Bean, N. J. 1988. Carbon disulfide: a semiochemical mediating socially-induced diet choice in rats. Physiology andBehaviour, 42, 119–124CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galef, B. G. Jr., Attenborough, K. S., and Whiskin, E. E. 1990. Responses of observer rats to complex, diet-related signals emitted by demonstrator rats. Journal ofComparative Psychology, 104, 11–19CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galef, B. G. Jr., Whiskin, E. E., and Bielavska, E. 1997. Interaction with demonstrator rats changes their observers' affective responses to flavors. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 111, 393–398CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gandolfi, G. and Parisi, V. 1972. Predazione su Unio pictorum L. da parte del ratto, Rattus norvegicus (Berkenhout). Acta Naturalia, 8, 1–27Google Scholar
Gandolfi, G. and Parisi, V. 1973. Ethological aspects of predation by rats, Rattus norvegicus (Berkenhout) on bivalves, Unio pictorum, L. and Cerastoderma lamarcki (Reeve). Bullettino di Zoologia, 40, 69–74CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaulin, S. J. C. and Kurland, J. A 1976. Primate predation and bioenergetics. Science, 191, 314–315CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gove, P. B. 1971. Webster's Third New International Dictionary of the English Language Unabridged. Springfield, MA: G. and C. Merriam
Hall, W. G. 1975. Weaning and growth of artificially reared rats. Science, 190, 1313–1315CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hepper, P. G. 1988. Adaptive fetal learning: Prenatal exposure to garlic affects postnatal preference. Animal Behaviour, 36, 935–936CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heyes, C. M. 1993. Imitation, culture, and cognition. Animal Behaviour, 46, 999–1010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heyes, C. M. and Durlach, P. J. 1990. “Social blockade” of taste-aversion learning in Norway rats (R. norvegicus): is it a social phenomenon? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 104, 82–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenyon, K. W. 1961. Birds of Amchitka, Alaska. Auk, 78, 305–326CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, B. J. 1994a. Primate infants as skilled information gatherers. Pre- and Perinatal Psychology Journal, 8, 287–307Google Scholar
King, B. J. 1994b. The Information Continuum. Santa Fe, NM: SAR Press
Koster, F. and Koster, M. 1983. Twelve days among the “vampire finches” of Wolf Island. Noticias de Galapagos, 38, 4–10Google Scholar
Lack, D. 1947. Darwin's Finches. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Lack, D. 1969. Subspecies and sympatry in Darwin's finches. Evolution, 23, 252–263CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laland, K. N., and Plotkin, H. C. 1990. Social learning and social transmission of foraging information in Norway rats (Rattus norvegicus). Animal Learning and Behavior, 18, 246–251CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laland, K. N. and Plotkin, H. C. 1991. Excretory deposits surrounding food sites facilitate social learning of food preferences in Norway rats. Animal Behaviour, 41, 997–1005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laland, K. N. and Plotkin, H. C. 1992. Further experimental analysis of the social leaning and transmission of foraging information amongst Norway rats. Behavioural Processes, 27, 53–64CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laland, K. N., Richerson, P. J., and Boyd, R. 1993. Animal social learning: toward a new theoretical approach. Perspectives in Ethology, 10, 249–277Google Scholar
Lewis, C. T. and Short, C. 1969. A Latin Dictionary. Oxford: Clarendon Press
Lore, R., Blanc, A., and Suedfeld, P. (1971). Empathic learning of a passive-avoidance response in domesticated Rattus norvegicus. Animal Behaviour, 19, 112–114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, L. T. and Alberts, J. R. 1979. Taste aversions to mother's milk: the age-related role of nursing in the acquisition and expression of a learned association. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 93, 430–445CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGrew, W. C. 1998. Culture in nonhuman primates. Annual Review of Anthropology, 27, 301–308CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, C. L. (1894). Introduction to Comparative Psychology. London: Scott
Nieder, L., Cagnin, M., and Parisi, V. 1982. Burrowing and feeding behaviour in the rat. Animal Behaviour, 30, 837–844CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parisi, V. and Gandolfi, G. 1974. Further aspects of the predation by rats on various mollusc species. Bollettino di Zoologia, 41, 87–106CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posadas-Andrews, A. and Roper, T. J. 1983. Social transmission of food preferences in adult rats. Animal Behaviour, 31 265–271CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pye, T. and Bonner, W. N. 1980. Feral brown rats, Rattus norvegicus, in South Georgia (South Atlantic Ocean). Journal of the Zoological Society of London, 192, 237–235CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richard, M. M., Grover, C. A., and Davis, S. F. 1987. Galef's transfer-of-information effect occurs in a free-foraging situation. Psychological Record, 37, 79–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schluter, D. and Grant, P. R. 1982. The distribution of Geospiza difficilis in relation to G. fuliginosa in the Galapagos Islands: tests of three hypotheses. Evolution, 36, 1213–1226Google Scholar
Schluter, D. and Grant, P. R. 1984. Ecological correlates of morphological evolution in a Darwin's finch species. Evolution, 38, 856–869CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, C. C. and Balda, R. P. 1979. Competition among insects, birds and mammals for conifer seeds. American Zoologist, 19, 1065–1083CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smotherman, W. P. 1982. Odor aversion learning by the rat fetus. Physiology and Behavior, 29, 769–771CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanford, C. B. 1998. Chimpanzee and Red Colobus: The Ecology of Predator and Prey. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Steiniger, F. 1950. Beitrage zur Soziologie und sonstigen Biologie der Wanderratte. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 7, 356–379CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strum, S. C. 1975. Primate predation: interim report on the development of a tradition in a troop of olive baboons. Science, 187, 755–757CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strum, S. C. 1976. Untitled. Science, 191, 314–317Google Scholar
Strupp, B. J. and Levitsky, D. E. 1984. Social transmission of food preferences in adult hooded rats (Rattus norvegicus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 98, 257–266CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Telle, H. J. 1966. Beitrag zur Kenntnis der Verhaltensweise von Ratten, vergleichend dargestellt bei Rattus norvegicus und Rattus rattus. Zeitschrift für Angewandte Zoologie, 53, 129–196Google Scholar
Terkel, J. 1996. Cultural transmission of feeding behavior in the black rat (Rattus rattus). In Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture, ed. C. M. Heyes and B. G. Galef Jr., pp. 17–49. San Diego, CA: Academic Press
Thorpe, W. H. 1963. Learning and Instinct in Animals, 2nd edn. London: Methuen
von Frisch, K. 1967. The Dance Language and Orientation of Bees. Cambridge, UK: Belknap Press
Whiten, A. and Ham, R. 1992. On the nature and evolution of imitation in the animal kingdom: reappraisal of a century of research. Advances in the Study of Behaviour, 22, 239–283CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., Tutin, C. E. G., Wrangham, R. W., and Boesch, C. 1999, Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399, 682–685CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, G. C. 1966. Adaptation and Natural Selection: A Critique of Some Current Evolutionary Thought. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press
Zohar, O. and Terkel, J. 1992. Acquisition of pinecone stripping behaviour in black rats (Rattus rattus). International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 5, 1–6Google Scholar
Zohar, O. and Terkel, J. 1995. Spontaneous learning of pinecone opening behavior by black rats. Mammalia, 59, 481–487CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×