Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-vsgnj Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-20T22:47:44.826Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

12 - Developmental perspectives on great ape traditions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 October 2009

Anne E. Russon
Affiliation:
Psychology Department, Glendon College of York University, 2275 Bayview Avenue, Toronto, Ontario M4N 3M6, Canada
Dorothy M. Fragaszy
Affiliation:
University of Georgia
Susan Perry
Affiliation:
University of California, Los Angeles
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Interest in nonhuman primate culture arose primarily because of the insights promised into human culture, given the likelihood that evolutionary continuities link the two. Concepts of human culture are not directly applicable to nonhuman primates, however, because nonhuman primates do not share all the capacities deemed intrinsic to human culture. Scholars interested in comparative evolutionary questions, therefore, set aside features considered beyond the reach of nonhuman primates in order to focus on what is taken as the core feature of culture: a collective system of shared, learned practices. The focal phenomena in studies of nonhuman primate culture are then its products, enduring behavioral traditions, and the processes that generate them, social influences on learning operating at the group level over long periods of time (e.g., Donald, 1991; Kummer, 1971; McGrew, 1998; Nishida, 1987). It would be surprising, in fact, if such traditions were not prominent in the lives of nonhuman primates. Nonhuman primates typically rely on intricate forms of sociality for survival (Humphrey, 1976; Jolly, 1966; Smuts et al., 1987) and on lifelong learning for much of their expertise (Fobes and King, 1982; King, 1994; Parker and Gibson, 1990). How widely practices must be shared, how much they must owe to social influence, and how long they must endure to qualify as “traditions” remain matters of debate (see Ch. 1).

Great apes stand out in this enterprise because their traditions may be more complex than those of other nonhuman primates (Parker and Russon, 1996; Whiten et al., 1999).

Type
Chapter
Information
The Biology of Traditions
Models and Evidence
, pp. 329 - 364
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Altmann, J. 1980. Baboon Mothers and Infants. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Baltes, P. P., Staudinger, U. M., and Lindenberger, U. 1999. Lifespan psychology: theory and application to intellectual functioning. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 471–507CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boesch, C. and Boesch-Achermann, H. 2000. The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Boesch, C. and Tomasello, M. 1998. Chimpanzee and human cultures. Current Anthropology, 39, 591–614CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bogin, B. 1999. Evolutionary perspective on human growth. Annual Review of Anthropology, 28, 109–153CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Box, H. O. and Gibson, K. R. (ed.) 1999. Mammalian Social Learning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Brewer, S. M. and McGrew, W. C. 1990. Chimpanzee use of a tool-set to get honey. Folia Primatologica, 54, 100–104CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, R. W. 1995. The Thinking Ape. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Byrne, R. W. 1997. The technical intelligence hypothesis: an alternative evolutionary stimulus to intelligence? In Machiavellian Intelligence II, ed. A. Whiten and R. W. Byrne, pp. 289–311, Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press
Byrne, R. W. and Byrne, J. M. E. 1991. Hand preferences in the skilled gathering tasks of mountain gorillas (Gorilla gorilla berengei). Cortex, 27, 521–546CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, R. W. and Byrne, J. M. E. 1993. Complex leaf-gathering skills of mountain gorillas (Gorilla g. beringei): variability and standardization. American Journal of Primatology, 31, 241–261CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, R. W. and Russon, A. E. 1998. Learning by imitation: a hierarchical approach. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 21, 667–721CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheney, D. and Seyfarth, R. 1990. How Monkeys See the World. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press
Coussi-Korbel, S. and Fragaszy, D. M. 1995. On the relation between social dynamics and social learning. Animal Behaviour, 50, 1441–1453CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Rousseau, C. J. (ed.). 1990. Monographs in Primatology, Vol, 14, Primate Life History and Evolution. New York: Wiley-Liss
Donald, M. 1991. Origins of the Modern Mind: Three Stages in the Evolution of Culture and Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Durham, W. H. 1991. Coevolution: Genes, Culture and Human Diversity. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press
Fleagle, J. G. 1999. Primate Adaptation and Evolution, 2nd edn. San Diego, CA: Academic Press
Fobes, J. L. and King, J. E. 1982. Primate Behaviour. New York: Academic Press
Fox, E. A. Sitompul, A. F., and van Schaik, C. P. 1999. Intelligent tool use in wild Sumatran orangutans. In The Mentalities of Gorillas and Orangutans: Comparative Perspectives, ed. S. T. Parker, R. W. Mitchell, and H. L. Miles, pp. 99–116. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Freeland, W. J. and Janzen, D. H. 1974. Strategies in herbivory by mammals: the role of plant secondary compounds. American Naturalist, 108, 269–289CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuentes, A. 2000. Hylobatid communities: changing views of pair bonding and social organization in hominoids. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology,43 (Supplement 31 to the American Journal of Physical Anthropology), 33–60
Galdikas, B. M. F. 1985. Adult male sociality and reproductive tactics among orangutans at Tanjung Puting. Folia Primatologica, 45, 9–24CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galdikas, B. M. F. 1988. Orangutan diet, range, and activity at Tanjung Puting, Central Borneo. International Journal of Primatology, 9, 1–35CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galdikas, B. M. F. 1995. Social and reproductive behaviour of wild adolescent female orangutans. In The Neglected Ape, ed. R. D. Nadler, B. F. M. Galdikas, L. K. Sheeran, and N. Rosen, pp. 163–182. New York: Plenum Press
Galdikas, B. M. F. and Teleki, G. 1981. Variations in subsistence activities of male and female pongids: new perspectives on the origins of hominid labour division. Current Anthropology, 22, 241–256Google Scholar
Galdikas, B. M. F. and Wood, J. 1990. Great ape and human birth intervals. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 83, 185–192CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giraldeau, L.-A. 1997. The ecology of information use. In Handbook of Behavioral Ecology, ed. N. Davies and J. Krebs, pp. 42–68. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific
Goodall, J., Bandora, A., Bergmann, E., Busse, C., Matama, H., Mpongo, E., Peirce, A., and Riss, D. 1979. Intercommunity interactions in the chimpanzee population of the Gombe National Park. In The Great Apes, ed. D. Hamburg and E. R. McCown, pp. 15–33. Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin/Cummings
Gosselain, O. P. 2000. Materializing identities: an African perspective. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 7, 187–217CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grundmann, E., Lestel, D., Boestani, A. N., and Bomsel, M.-C. 2000. Learning to survive in the forest: what every orangutan should know. Presented at The Apes: Challenges for the 21st Century, Brookfield Zoo, Chicago, 10–13 May
Horr, D. A. 1977. Orang-utan maturation: growing up in a female world. In Primate Bio-social Development: Biological, Social, and Ecological Determinants, ed. S. Chevalier-Skolnikoff and F. E. Poirier, pp. 289–321. New York: Garland
Huffman, M. A. 1997. Current evidence for self-medication in primates: a (Supplement 28 to the American Journal of Physical Anthropology), multidisciplinary perspective. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 40 171–2003.0.CO;2-7>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphrey, N. K. 1976. The social function of intellect. In Growing Points in Ethology, ed. P. P. G. Bateson and R. A. Hinde, pp. 303–317. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Inoue-Nakamura, N. and Matsuzawa, T. 1997. Development of stone tool use by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 111, 159–173CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janson, C. H. and van Schaik, C. P. 1993. Ecological risk aversion in juvenile primates: slow and steady wins the race. In Juvenile Primates: Life History, Development, and Behaviour, ed. M. E. Pereira and L. A. Fairbanks, pp. 57–76. New York: Oxford University Press
Joffe, T. H. 1997. Social pressures have selected for an extended juvenile period in primates. Journal of Human Evolution, 32, 593–605CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jolly, A. 1966. Lemur social behaviour and primate intelligence. Science, 153, 501–506CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joulian, F. (1995). Mise en evidence de differences traditionnelles dans le cassage des noix chez les chimpanzés (Pan troglodytes) de la Côte d'Ivoire, implications paléoanthropologiques. Journal des Africanistes, 65, 57–77CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, B. J. 1994. The Information Continuum. Santa Fe, NM: School of American Research
Kummer, H. 1971. Primate Societies: Group Techniques of Ecological Adaptation. Chicago, IL: Aldine
Lackman-Ancrenaz, I. 2001. Report on the Kinabatangan Orangutan Conservation Project. Presentation to the Orangutan Reintroduction and Protection Workshop, Balikpapan, Indonesia, 15–18 June
Laland, K., Odling-Smee, J., and Feldman, M. 2000. Niche construction, biological evolution, and cultural change. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 131–146CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacKinnon, J. R. 1974. The behaviour and ecology of wild orang-utans. Animal Behaviour, 22, 3–74CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsuzawa, T. 2001. Primate foundations of human intelligence: a view of tool use in nonhuman primates and fossil hominids. In Primate Origins of Human Cognition and Behavior, ed. T. Matsuzawa, pp. 3–25. Tokyo: Springer-Verlag
Matsuzawa, T. 1996. Chimpanzee intelligence in nature and in captivity: isomorphism of symbol use and tool use. In Great Ape Societies, ed. W. C. McGrew, L. F. Marchant, and T. Nishida, pp. 196–209. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Matsuzawa, T., Biro, D., Humle, T., Inoue-Nakamura, N., Tonooka, R., and Yamakoshi, G. 2001. Emergence of culture in wild chimpanzees: education by master–apprenticeship. In Primate Origins of Human Cognition and Behavior, ed. T. Matsuzawa, pp. 557–574. Tokyo: Springer-Verlag
McGrew, W. C. 1992. Chimpanzee Material Culture: Implications for Human Evolution. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press
McGrew, W. C. 1998. Culture in nonhuman primates?Annual Review of Anthropology, 27, 301–328CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menzel, C. 2001. Umprompted recall and reporting of hidden objects by a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) after extended delays. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113, 426–434CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyfarth, E. 1998. Biological conservation: orangutan-rattan relationships in Indonesian Borneo. Master's Thesis, York University, Toronto
Miles, H. L. 1978. Conversations with apes: the use of sign language with two chimpanzees. PhD Thesis, Yale University New Haven, CT
Miles, H. L., Mitchell, R. W., and Harper, S. 1996. Simon says: the development of imitation in an enculturated orangutan. In Reaching into Thought: The Minds of the Great Apes, ed. A. E. Russon, K. A. Bard, and S. T. Parker. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Milton, K. 1984. The role of food processing factors in primate food choice. In Adaptations for Foraging in Nonhuman Primates: Contributions to an Organismal Biology of Prosimians, Monkeys and Apes, ed. P. S. Rodman and J. G. H. Cant, pp. 249–279. New York: Columbia University Press
Mitani, J. C. 1985. Mating behaviour of male orangutans in the Kutai Game Reserve, Indonesia. Animal Behaviour, 33, 392–402CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitani, J. C. and Gros-Louis, J. 1998. Chorusing and call convergence in chimpanzees: test of three hypotheses. Behaviour, 135, 1041–1064CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitani, J. C., Hunley, K. L., and Murdoch, M. E. 1999. Geographic variation in the calls of wild chimpanzees: a reassessment. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 47, 133–151Google ScholarPubMed
Myowa-Yamakoshi, M. and Matsuzawa, T. 1999. Factors influencing imitation of manipulatory actions in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113, 128–136CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. 1987. Local traditions and cultural transmission. In Primate Societies, ed. B. B. Smuts, D. L. Cheney, R. M. Seyfarth, R. W. Wrangham, and T. T. Struhsaker, pp. 462–474. Chicago IL: University Chicago Press
Nishida, T. 1979. The social structure of chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains. In The Great Apes, ed. D. A. Hamburg and E. R. McCown. pp. 73–121. Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin/Cummings
Parker, S. T. 1996. Apprenticeship in tool-mediated extractive foraging: the origins of imitation, teaching and self-awareness in great apes. In Reaching into Thought: The Minds of the Great Apes, ed. A. E. Russon, K. A. Bard, and S. T. Parker, pp. 348–370. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Parker, S. T. and Gibson, K. R. 1977. Object manipulation, tool use and sensorimotor intelligence as feeding adaptations in Cebus monkeys and great apes. Journal of Human Evolution, 6, 623–641CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parker, S. T. and Gibson, K. R. 1979. A model of the evolution of language and intelligence in early hominids. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 2, 367–407CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parker, S. T., and Gibson, K. R. (ed.) 1990. “Language” and Intelligence in Monkeys and Apes: Comparative Developmental Perspectives. New York: Cambridge University Press
Parker, S. T. and McKinney, M. L. 1999. Origins of Intelligence: The Evolution of Cognitive Development in Monkeys, Apes, and Humans. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins
Parker, S. T., and Russon, A. E. 1996. On the wild side of culture and cognition in the great apes. In Reaching into Thought: The Minds of the Great Apes, ed. A. E. Russon, K. A. Bard, and S. T. Parker, pp. 430–450. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Pereira, M. E. 1993. Juvenility in animals. In Juvenile Primates: Life History, Development, and Behaviour, ed. M. E. Pereira and L. A. Fairbanks, pp. 7–27. New York: Oxford University Press
Pereira, M. E. and Altmann, J. 1985. Development of social behaviour in free-living nonhuman primates. In Nonhuman Primate Models for Human Growth and Development, ed. E. S. Watts, pp. 217–309. New York: Liss
Peters, H. 2001. Tool use to modify vocalisations by wild orang-utans. Folia Primatologica, 72, 242–244CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rijksen, H. D. 1978. A Field Study on Sumatran Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus Abelii Lesson 1827): Ecology, Behaviour, and Conservation. Wageningen: H. Veenman and Zonen B. V.
Rodman, P. 1973. Population composition and adaptive organization among orangutans of the Kutai Reserve. In Comparative Ecology and Behaviour of Primates, ed. J. H. Crook and R. P. Michael, pp. 171–209. London: Academic Press
Rodman, P. S. 1979. Individual activity patterns and the solitary nature of orangutans. In The Great Apes, ed. D. A. Hamburg and E. R. McCown, pp. 235–255. Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin-Cummings
Rodman, P. S. 2000. Great ape models for the evolution of human diet.www.cast.uark.edu/local/icaes/conferences/wburg/posters/psrodman/GAMHD.htm, Dec
Rodman, P. W. 1977. Feeding behaviour of orang-utans of the Kutai Nature Reserve, East Kalimantan. In Primate Ecology, ed. T. H. Clutton-Brock, pp. 383–413. New York: Academic Press
Rogers, L. and Kaplan, G. 1994. A new form of tool-using by orangutans in Sabah, East Malaysia. Folia Primatologica, 63, 50–52CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogoff, B. 1992. Apprenticeship in Thinking. New York: Oxford University Press
Russon, A. E. 1997. Exploiting the expertise of others. In Machiavellian Intelligence II: Evaluations and Extensions, ed. A. Whiten and R. W. Byrne, pp. 174–206. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Russon, A. E. 1998. The nature and evolution of orangutan intelligence. Primates, 9, 485–503CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russon, A. E. 1999a. Naturalistic approaches to orangutan intelligence and the question of enculturation. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 12, 181–202Google Scholar
Russon, A. E. 1999b. Orangutans' imitation of tool use: a cognitive analysis. In Mentalities of Gorillas and Orangutans, ed. S. T. Parker, R. W. Mitchell, and H. L. Miles, pp. 117–146. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Russon, A. E. 2002. Return of the native: Cognition and site specific expertise in orangutan rehabilitation. International Journal of Primatology, 23, 461–478CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russon, A. E. 2003. Comparative developmental perspectives on culture: the great apes. In Between Biology and Culture: Perspectives on Ontogenetic Development, ed. H. Keller, Y. H. Poortinga, and A. Schoelmerich. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, in press
Russon, A. E., and Galdikas, B. M. F. 1995. Constraints on great apes' imitation: model and action selectivity in rehabilitant orangutan imitation (Pongo pygmaeus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 109, 5–17CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russon, A. E., Bard, K. A., and Parker, S. T. (eds.) 1996. Reaching into Thought: The Minds of the Great Apes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Russon, A. E., Mitchell, R. W., Lefebvre, L., and Abravanel, E. 1998. The comparative evolution of imitation. In Piaget, Evolution, and Development, ed. J. Langer and M. Killen, pp. 103–143. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Shennan, S. J. and Steele, J. 1999. Cultural learning in hominids: a behavioural ecological approach. In Mammalian Social Learning: Comparative and Ecological Perspectives, ed. H. O. Box and K. R. Gibson, pp. 367–388. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W, and Struhsaker, T. T. (ed.) 1987. Primate Societies. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press
Stanford, C. B. 1996. The hunting ecology of wild chimpanzees: implications for the evolutionary ecology of Pliocene hominids. American Anthropologist, 98, 96–113CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanford, C. B. 1998. Chimpanzee and Red Colobus: The Ecology of Predator and Prey. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Stokes, E. J. 1999. Feeding skills and the effect of injury on wild chimpanzees. PhD Thesis, University of St. Andrews, St. Andrews, Scotland
Suddendorf, T. and Whiten, A. 2001. Mental evolution and development: evidence for secondary representation in children, great apes, and other animals. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 629–650CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. and Call, J. 1997. Primate Cognition. New York: Oxford University Press
Tomasello, M., Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., and Kruger, A. 1993. Imitative learning of actions on objects by children, chimpanzees, and enculturated chimpanzees. Child Development, 64, 1688–1705CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Utami, S. S. 2000. Bimaturism in Orang-Utan Males: Reproductive and Ecological Strategies. Utrecht: Central Reproductie FSB Universiteit Utrecht
Schaik, C. P. and Knott, C. 2001. Geographic variation in tool use in Neesia fruits in orangutans. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 114, 331–342CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Schaik, C. P., and van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. 1996. Toward an understanding of the orangutan's social system. In Great Ape Societies, ed. W. C. McGrew, L. F. Marchant, and T. Nishida, pp. 3–15. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Schaik, C. P., Fox, E. A., and Sitompul, A. F. 1996. Manufacture and use of tools in wild Sumatran orangutans. Naturwissenschaften, 83, 186–188Google ScholarPubMed
Schaik, C. P., Deaner, R. O., and Merrill, M. Y. 1999. The conditions for tool use in primates: implications for the evolution of material culture. Journal of Human Evolution, 36, 719–741CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Schaik, C. P., Preuschoft, S., and Watts, D. P. 2003. Great ape social systems. In The Evolutionary Origins of Great Ape Intelligence, ed. A. E. Russon and D. Begun. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, in press
Voysey, B. C., McDonald, K. E., Rogers, M. E., Tutin, C. E. G., and Parnell, R. J. 1999. Gorillas and seed dispersal in the Lope Reserve, Gabon. II: survival and growth of seedlings. Journal of Tropical Ecology, 15, 39–60CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. 1962 Thought and Language, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Waterman, P. G. 1984. Food acquisition and processing as a function of plant chemistry. In Food Acquisition and Processing in Primates, ed. D. J. Chivers, B. A. Wood, and A. Bilsborough, pp. 177–211. New York: Plenum Press
Watts, D. P. and Pusey, A. E. 1993. Behaviour of juvenile and adolescent great apes. In Juvenile Primates: Life History, Development, and Behaviour, ed. M./.E. Pereira and L. A. Fairbanks, pp. 148–171. New York: Oxford University Press
Watts, E. 1985. Adolescent growth and development in monkeys, apes, and humans. In Nonhuman Primate Models for Human Growth and Development, ed. E. Watts, pp. 41–65. New York: Alan R. Liss
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., Tutin, C. E. G., Wrangham, R. W., and Boesch, C. 1999. Culture in chimpanzees. Nature, 399, 682–685CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wrangham, R. W. 1979. On the evolution of ape social systems. Social Science Information, 18, 334–368CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yamagiwa, J. 2003. Diet and foraging of the great apes: ecological constraints on their social organizations and implications for their divergence. In The Evolutionary Origins of Great Ape Intelligence, ed. A. E. Russon and D. Begun. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, in press
Yamakoshi, G. 1998. Dietary responses to fruit scarcity of wild chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea: possible implications for ecological importance of tool use. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 106, 283–2953.0.CO;2-O>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×