Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T20:13:04.083Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - The cue reliability approach to social transmission: designing tests for adaptive traditions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 October 2009

Gwen Dewar
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
Dorothy M. Fragaszy
Affiliation:
University of Georgia
Susan Perry
Affiliation:
University of California, Los Angeles
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Traditions are behaviors that persist over time and are shared among group members by virtue of social learning processes (see Ch. 1). The direct observation of animals using social cues to discover or learn a behavior is perhaps the most straightforward evidence of a tradition, and numerous longitudinal, naturalistic studies and controlled laboratory experiments have yielded such evidence (see, for instance, Chs. 7, 9, 13, and 14). However, efforts to collect direct evidence are sometimes deemed impractical; consequently investigators have sought ways to infer the existence of traditions on the basis of indirect evidence. Can we identify traditions when we lack direct observations of social learning?

I present a new approach for dealing with indirect evidence. This cue reliability approach (CRA) addresses a special category of potential traditions: behaviors that (a) reflect an individual's classification of a stimulus or tactic as either safe or harmful, and (b) are costly if the individual makes classification errors. Is hemlock a safe food or a dangerous toxin? Should garter snakes be dismissed as benign trespassers or avoided as lethal predators? Animals can answer these questions by consulting local traditions. However, traditional knowledge is not necessarily the only source of information available. The CRA is designed to help us to determine if animals need social cues to classify correctly potentially dangerous stimuli or bad tactics. It begins by identifying a decision-maker's options regarding an unfamiliar stimulus or untested tactic, and the possible outcomes associated with each option.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Biology of Traditions
Models and Evidence
, pp. 127 - 158
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aoki, K. and Feldman, M. W. 1987. Toward a theory for the evolution of cultural communication: Coevolution of signal transmission and reception. Proceeding of the National Academy of Science, USA, 84, 7164–7168CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, B. A. and Meese, G. B. 1979. Social behaviour in pigs studied by means of operant conditioning. Animal Behaviour, 27, 947–957CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyd, R. and Richerson, P. J. 1985. Culture and the Evolutionary Process. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press
Boyd, R. and Richerson, P. J. 1988. An evolutionary model of social learning: the effects of spatial and temporal variation. In Social Learning: Psychological and Biological Perspectives, ed. T. R. Zentall and B. G. Galef Jr., pp. 29–48. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Brown, J. 1999. Vigilance, patch use and habitat selection: foraging under predation risk. Evolutionary Ecology Research, 1, 49–71Google Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. and Feldman, M. W. 1981. Cultural Transmission and Evolution: A Quantitative Approach. Princeton: Princeton University Press
Coss, R. G. 1991. Context and animal behavior III. The relationship between early development and evolutionary persistence of ground squirrel antisnake behavior. Ecological Psychology, 3, 277–315CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curio, E. 1988. Cultural transmission of enemy recognition in birds. In Social Learning: Psychological and Biological Perspectives, ed. T. R. Zentall and B. G. Galef Jr., pp. 75–98. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Dominy, N. J., Lucas, P. W., Osorio, D., and Yamashita, N. 2001. The sensory ecology of primate food perception. Evolutionary Anthropology, 10, 171–186CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dukas, R. and Waser, N. 1994. Categorization of food types enhances the foraging efficiency of bumblebees. Animal Behaviour, 48, 1001–1006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forbes, J. M. 1998. Dietary awareness. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 57, 287–297CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fragaszy, D. M. and Visalberghi, E. 1996. Social learning in monkeys: primate primacy reconsidered. In Social Learning and Imitation: The Roots of Culture, ed. C. M. Heyes and B. G. Galef Jr., pp. 49–64. New York: Academic Press
Freeland, W. J. and Janzen, D. H. 1974. Strategies in herbivory by mammals: the role of plant secondary compounds. American Naturalist, 108, 269–289CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galef, B. G. Jr. 1995. Why behaviour patterns that animals learn socially are locally adaptive. Animal Behaviour, 49, 1325–1334CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galef, B. G. Jr., McQuoid, L. M., and Whiskin, E. E. 1990. Further evidence that Norway rats do not socially transmit learned aversions to toxic baits. Animal Learning and Behavior, 18, 199–205CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giraldeau, L. A. and Caraco, T. 2000. Social Foraging Theory. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press
Giraldeau, L. A., Caraco, T., and Valone, T. J. 1994. Social foraging: individual learning and cultural transmission of innovations. Behavioral Ecology, 5, 35–43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glendinning, J. I. 1994. Is the bitter rejection response always adaptive?Physiological Behavior, 56, 1217–1227CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goatcher, W. D. and Church, D. C. 1970. Taste responses in ruminants. IV. Reactions of pygmy goats, normal goats, sheep and cattle to acetic acid and quinine hydrochloride. Journal of Animal Science, 31, 373–382CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heyes, C. M. 1993. Imitation, culture and cognition. Animal Behaviour, 46, 999–1010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hladik, C. M. and Simmen, B. 1996. Taste perception and feeding behavior in nonhuman primates and human populations. Evolutionary Anthropology, 5, 58–713.0.CO;2-S>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huffman, M. 1996. Acquisition of innovative cultural behaviors in nonhuman primates: a case study of stone-handling, a socially transmitted behavior in Japanese macaques. In Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture, ed. C. M. Heyes and B. G. Galef Jr., pp. 267–286. San Diego, CA: Academic Press
Huffman, M. 1997. Current evidence for self-medication in primates: a multidisciplinary perspective. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 40, 171–2003.0.CO;2-7>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huffman, M. and Wrangham, R. W. 1994. Diversity of medicinal plant use by chimpanzees in the wild. In Chimpanzee Cultures, ed. R. W. Wrangham, W. C. McGrew, F. B. M. de Waal, and P. G. Heltne, pp. 129–148. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Isbell, L. A. 1994. Predation on primates: ecological patterns and evolutionary consequences. Evolutionary Anthropology, 3, 61–71CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johns, T. 1994. Ambivalence to the palatability factors in wild plant foods. In Eating on the Wild Side: The Pharmacologic, Ecologic, and Social Implications of Using Noncultigens, ed. N. L. Etkin, pp. 46–61. Tuscon, AZ: University of Arizona Press
Laland, K. N. 1999. Exploring the dynamics of social transmission with rats. In Mammalian Social Learning: Comparative and Ecological Perspectives, ed. H. O. Box and K. R. Gibson, pp. 174–187. New York: Cambridge University Press
Laland, K. N. and Williams, K. 1998. Social transmission of maladaptive information in the guppy. Behavioral Ecology, 9, 493–499CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laland, K. N., Richerson, P. J., and Boyd, R. 1996. Developing a theory of animal social learning. In Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture, ed. C. M. Heyes and B. G. Galef Jr., pp. 129–154. San Diego, CA: Academic Press
Mason, J. R., Arzt, A. H., and Reidinger, R. F. 1984. Comparative assessment of food preferences and aversions acquired by blackbirds via observational conditioning. Auk, 101, 796–803CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mathis, A., Chivers, D. P., and Smith, R. J. F. 1996. Cultural transmission of predator recognition in fishes: intraspecific and interspecific learning. Animal Behaviour, 51, 185–201CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKey, D. 1978. Soil, vegetation and seed-eating by black colobus monkeys. In The Ecology of Arboreal Folivores, ed. G. G. Montgomery, pp. 423–237. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institute Press
Mineka, S. and Cook, M. 1988. Social learning and the acquisition of snake fear in monkeys. In Social Learning: Psychological and Biological Perspectives, ed. T. R. Zentall and B. G. Galef Jr., pp. 51–73. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Nishida, T. 1987. Local traditions and cultural transmission. In Primate Societies, ed. B. B. Smuts, D. L. Cheney, R. M. Seyfarth, R. W. Wrangham, and T. T. Struhsaker, pp. 462–474. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press
Nishida, T., Ohigashi, H., and Koshimizu, K. 2000. Tastes of chimpanzee plant foods. Current Anthropology, 41, 431–438CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, A. 1988. Does biology constrain culture?American Anthropologist, 90, 819–831CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rowe, C. and Guilford, T. 1999. Novelty effects in a multimodal warning signal. Animal Behaviour 57, 341–346CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozin, P. 1976. Specific aversions as a function of specific hungers. Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology, 64, 237–242CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schuler, W. and Roper, T. J. 1992. Responses to warning coloration in avian predators. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 21, 111–146CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sclafani, A. 1991. Starch and sugar tastes in rodents: an update. Brain Research Bulletin, 27, 383–386CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seligman, M. 1971. Phobias and preparedness. Behavior Therapy, 2, 307–320CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephens, D. W. and Krebs, J. R. 1986. Foraging Theory. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press
Takahata, Y., Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M., Takasaki, H., and Nyundo, R. 1986. Newly acquired feeding habits among the chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Human Evolution, 1, 277–284CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Terkel, J. 1996. Cultural transmission of feeding behavior in the black rat (Rattus rattus). In Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture, ed. C. M. Heyes and B. G. Galef Jr., pp. 267–286. San Diego, CA: Academic Press
Visalberghi, E. and Addessi, E. 2000. Seeing group members eating familiar food enhances the acceptance of novel foods in capuchin monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 59, 1–8Google Scholar
Visalberghi, E. and Fragaszy, D. 1990. Food-washing behaviour in tufted capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella, and crabeating macaques, Macaca fascicularis. Animal Behaviour, 40, 829–836CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, J., Austin, R., and MacDonald, D. 2000. A simulation model of foraging behaviour and the effect of predation risk. Journal of Animal Ecology, 69, 16–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiss, M. R. 1997. Innate colour preferences and flexible colour learning in the pipevine swallowtail. Animal Behaviour, 53, 1043–1052CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., Tutin, C. E. G., Wrangham, R. W., and Boesch, C. 1999. Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 39, 682–685CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wink, M., Hofer, A., Bilfinger, M., Englert, E., Martin, M., and Schneider, D. 1993. Geese and dietary allelochemicals – food palatability and geophagy. Chemoecology, 4, 93–107CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×