Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-pfhbr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-11T23:52:39.895Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

12 - Language and cognition in bilinguals

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 May 2016

Vivian Cook
Affiliation:
University of Newcastle upon Tyne
Li Wei
Affiliation:
Institute of Education, University College London
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aitchison, J. 1987. Words in the Mind: An Introduction to the Mental Lexicon. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Ameel, E., Storms, G., Malt, B. C. and Sloman, S. A. 2005. ‘How bilinguals solve the naming problem’, Journal of Memory and Language 53, 6080.Google Scholar
Anderson, S. R. 2010. ‘How many languages are there in the world?Linguistic Society of America Brochure Series: Frequently Asked Questions. Washington, DC: Linguistic Society of America.Google Scholar
Athanasopoulos, P. 2006. ‘Effects of the grammatical representation of number on cognition in bilinguals’, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 9, 8996.Google Scholar
Athanasopoulos, P. 2009. ‘Cognitive representation of colour in bilinguals: the case of Greek blues’, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 12, 8395.Google Scholar
Athanasopoulos, P., Damjanovic, L., Krajciova, A. and Sasaki, M. 2011. ‘Representation of colour concepts in bilingual cognition: the case of Japanese blues’, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 14, 917.Google Scholar
Athanasopoulos, P. and Kasai, C. 2008. ‘Language and thought in bilinguals: the case of grammatical number and nonverbal classification preferences’, Applied Psycholinguistics 29, 105123.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. W. 1982. ‘Context-independent and context-dependent information in concepts’, Memory and Cognition 10, 8293.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. W. and Medin, D. L. 1986. ‘Concepts: static definitions or context-dependent representations?’, Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive 6, 197202.Google Scholar
Bassetti, B. 2007. ‘Bilingualism and thought: grammatical gender and concepts of objects in Italian–German bilingual children’, International Journal of Bilingualism 11, 251273.Google Scholar
Bassetti, B. and Cook, V. J. 2011. ‘Relating language and cognition: the second language user’, in Cook, V. J. and Bassetti, B. (eds), pp. 143190.Google Scholar
Berlin, B. and Kay, P. 1969. Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. 2009. ‘Bilingualism: the good, the bad, and the indifferent’, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 12, 311.Google Scholar
Boroditsky, L., Schmidt, L. A. and Phillips, W. 2003. ‘Sex, syntax, and semantics’, in Gentner, D. and Goldin-Meadow, S. (eds), Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Thought, pp. 6179. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Brysbaert, M. and Duyck, W. 2010. ‘Is it time to leave behind the Revised Hierarchical Model of bilingual language processing after fifteen years of service?’, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 13, 359371.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (ed.) 1956. Language, Thought, and Reality: Selected Writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press / New York and London: Wiley.Google Scholar
Caskey-Sirmons, L. A. and Hickerson, N. P. 1977. ‘Semantic shift and bilingualism: variation in the color terms of five languages’, Anthropological Linguistics 19, 358367.Google Scholar
Chen, H.-C. and Leung, Y.-S. 1989, ‘Patterns of lexical processing in a nonnative language’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 15, 316325.Google Scholar
Cook, V. J. 2003. ‘The changing L1 in the L2 user’s mind’, in Cook, V. J. (ed.), Effects of the Second Language on the First, pp. 118. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Cook, V. J. and Bassetti, B. (eds) 2011. Language and Bilingual Cognition. New York and Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Cook, V. J., Bassetti, B., Kasai, C., Sasaki, M. and Takahashi, J. A. 2006. ‘Do bilinguals have different concepts? The case of shape and material in Japanese L2 users of English’, International Journal of Bilingualism 10, 137152.Google Scholar
Costa, A., Caramazza, A. and Sebastián-Gallés, N. 2000. ‘The cognate facilitation effect: implications for models of lexical access’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 26, 12831296.Google Scholar
Crystal, D. 1987. The Cambridge Encyclopedia of Language. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Czechowska, N. and Ewert, A. 2011. ‘Perception of motion by Polish–English bilinguals’, in Cook, V. J. and Bassetti, B. (eds), pp. 287314.Google Scholar
De Groot, A. M. B. 1992. ‘Bilingual lexical representation: a closer look at conceptual representations’, in Frost, R. and Katz, L. (eds), Orthography, Phonology, Morphology and Meaning, pp. 389412. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.Google Scholar
De Groot, A. M. B. 1993. ‘Word-type effects in bilingual processing tasks: support for a mixed-representational system’, in Schreuder, R. and Weltens, B. (eds), The Bilingual Lexicon, pp. 2751. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
De Groot, A. M. B. 1995. ‘Determinants of bilingual lexicosemantic organisation’, Computer Assisted Language Learning 8, 151180.Google Scholar
De Groot, A. M. B. 2011. Language and Cognition in Bilinguals and Multilinguals: An Introduction. New York and Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
De Groot, A. M. B. 2012. ‘Bilingualism and cognition’, in Chapelle, C. A. (ed.), The Encyclopedia of Applied Linguistics, pp. 454462. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
De Groot, A. M. B. 2014. ‘About phonological, grammatical, and semantic accents in bilinguals’ language use and their cause’, in Filipović, L. and Pütz, M. (eds), Multilingual Cognition and Language Use: Processing and Typological Perspectives, pp. 229262. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Dong, Y., Gui, S. and MacWhinney, B. 2005. ‘Shared and separate meanings in the bilingual mental lexicon’, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 8, 221238.Google Scholar
Ervin, S. M. 1961. ‘Semantic shift in bilingualism’, The American Journal of Psychology 74, 233241.Google Scholar
Ervin, S. M. and Osgood, C. E. 1954. ‘Second language learning and bilingualism’, in Osgood, C. E. and Sebeok, F. (eds), Psycholinguistics, supplement to Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 139146.Google Scholar
Eysenck, M. W. and Keane, M. T. 1995. Cognitive Psychology: A Student’s Handbook, third edition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Finkbeiner, M., Forster, K., Nicol, J. and Nakamura, K. 2004. ‘The role of polysemy in masked semantic and translation priming’, Journal of Memory and Language 51, 122.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E. 1987. ‘The production of “new” and “similar” phones in a foreign language: evidence for the effect of equivalence classification’, Journal of Phonetics 15, 4765.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E. 2002. ‘Interactions between the native and second language phonetic systems’, in Burmeister, P., Piske, T. and Rohde, A. (eds), An Integrated View of Language Development: Papers in Honor of Henning Wode, pp. 217244. Trier: Wissenschaftlicher Verlag.Google Scholar
Francis, W. S. 2005. ‘Bilingual semantic and conceptual representation’, in Kroll, J. F. and de Groot, A. M. B. (eds), Handbook of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Approaches, pp. 251267. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gekoski, W. L. 1980. ‘Language acquisition context and language organization in bilinguals’, Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 9, 429449.Google Scholar
Grosjean, F. 1989. ‘Neurolinguists, beware! The bilingual is not two monolinguals in one person’, Brain and Language 36, 315.Google Scholar
Grosjean, F. 1998. ‘Studying bilinguals: methodological and conceptual issues’, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 1, 131149.Google Scholar
Hampson, P. J. and Morris, P. E. 1996. Understanding Cognition. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., Fischer, R. and Christoffels, I. K. 2011. ‘Bilingualism and creativity: benefits in convergent thinking come with losses in divergent thinking’, Frontiers in Psychology 2, article 273.Google Scholar
Jiang, N. 2000. ‘Lexical representation and development in a second language’, Applied Linguistics 21, 4777.Google Scholar
Johanson, L. and Utas, B. (eds) 2000. Evidentials: Turkic, Iranian and Neighbouring Languages. Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Kellogg, R. T. 1995. Cognitive Psychology. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Kharkhurin, A. V. 2007. ‘The role of cross-linguistic and cross-cultural experiences in bilinguals’ divergent thinking’, in Kecskes, I. and Albertazzi, L. (eds), Cognitive Aspects of Bilingualism, pp. 175210. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F. and Curley, J. 1988. ‘Lexical memory in novice bilinguals: the role of concepts in retrieving second language words’, in Gruneberg, M. M., Morris, P. E. and Sykes, R. N. (eds), Practical Aspects of Memory: Current Research and Issues, vol. 2: Clinical and Educational Implications, pp. 389395. Chichester, UK: Wiley.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Michael, E., Tokowicz, N. and Dufour, R. 2002. ‘The development of lexical fluency in a second language’, Second Language Research 18, 137171.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F. and Stewart, E. 1994. ‘Category interference in translation and picture naming: evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations’, Journal of Memory and Language 33, 149174.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., van Hell, J. G., Tokowicz, N. and Green, D. W. 2010. ‘The Revised Hierarchical Model: a critical review and assessment’, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 13, 373381.Google Scholar
Labov, W. 1973. ‘The boundaries of words and their meanings’, in Bailey, C.-J. N. and Shuy, R. W. (eds), New Ways of Analyzing Variation in English, pp. 340373. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Lambert, W. E., Havelka, J. and Crosby, C. 1958. ‘The influence of language-acquisition contexts on bilingualism’, Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 56, 239244.Google Scholar
Li, P. and Farkas, I. 2002. ‘A self-organizing connectionist model of bilingual processing’, in Heredia, R. R. and Altarriba, J. (eds), Bilingual Sentence Processing, pp. 5985. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.Google Scholar
Li, P. and Zhao, X. 2015. ‘Computational modeling of bilingual language acquisition and processing: conceptual and methodological considerations’, in Schwieter, J. W. (ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of Bilingual Processing, pp. 85107. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lucy, J. A. and Gaskins, S. 2001. ‘Grammatical categories and the development of classification preferences: a comparative approach’, in Bowerman, M. and Levinson, S. C. (eds), Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development, pp. 257283. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Malt, B. C. and Sloman, S. A. 2003. ‘Linguistic diversity and object naming by non-native speakers of English’, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 6, 4767.Google Scholar
Martin, L. 1986. ‘Eskimo words for snow: a case study in the genesis and decay of an anthropological example’, American Anthropologist 88, 418423.Google Scholar
Mitchell, J. 1969. ‘Both sides now’, Beverly Hills, CA: Siquomb Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Murphy, G. L. 1991. ‘Meaning and concepts’, in Schwanenflugel, P. J. (ed.), The Psychology of Word Meanings, pp. 1135. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Paap, K. R. and Greenberg, Z. I. 2013. ‘There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing’, Cognitive Psychology 66, 232258.Google Scholar
Pavlenko, A. 2005. ‘Bilingualism and thought’, in Kroll, J. F. and de Groot, A. M. B. (eds), Handbook of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Perspectives, pp. 433453. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pavlenko, A. 2006. ‘Bilingual selves’, in Pavlenko, A. (ed.), Bilingual Minds: Emotional Experience, Expression, and Representation, pp. 133. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Pavlenko, A. 2009. ‘Conceptual representation in the bilingual lexicon and second language vocabulary learning’, in Pavlenko, A. (ed.), The Bilingual Mental Lexicon: Interdisciplinary Approaches, pp. 125160. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Potter, M. C., So, K.-F., Von Eckardt, B. and Feldman, L. B. 1984. ‘Lexical and conceptual representation in beginning and proficient bilinguals’, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 23, 2338.Google Scholar
Roberson, D., Pak, H. and Hanley, J. R. 2008. ‘Categorical perception of colour in the left and right visual field is verbally mediated: evidence from Korean’, Cognition 107, 752762.Google Scholar
Rosch, E. 1975. ‘Cognitive representations of semantic categories’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 104, 192233.Google Scholar
Rosch Heider, E. 1972. ‘Universals in color naming and memory’, Journal of Experimental Psychology 93, 1020.Google Scholar
Siegal, M., Frank, C. K., Surian, L. and Hjelmquist, E. 2011. ‘Theory of mind and bilingual cognition’, in Cook, V. J. and Bassetti, B. (eds), pp. 431452.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. 1987. ‘Thinking for speaking’, Proceedings of the Thirteenth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society 13, 435445.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. 1996. ‘From “thought and language” to “thinking for speaking”’, in Gumperz, J. J. and Levinson, S. C. (eds), Rethinking Linguistic Relativity, pp. 7096. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Starreveld, P. A., De Groot, A. M. B., Rossmark, B. M. M. and van Hell, J. G. 2014. ‘Parallel language activation during word processing in bilinguals: evidence from word production in sentence context’, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 17, 258276.Google Scholar
Taylor, I. and Taylor, M. M. 1990. Psycholinguistics: Learning and Using Languages. Englewood-Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Van Hell, J. G. and De Groot, A. M. B. 1998. ‘Conceptual representation in bilingual memory: effects of concreteness and cognate status in word association’, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 3, 193211.Google Scholar
Weinreich, U. 1953. Languages in Contact: Findings and Problems. The Hague, Netherlands: Mouton.Google Scholar
Whorf, B. L. 1940/1956. ‘Linguistics as an exact science’, Technology Review 43, 6163, 8083. Reprinted in Carroll, J. B. (ed.), Language, Thought, and Reality: Selected Writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf, pp. 220232. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Winawer, J., Witthoft, N., Frank, M. C., Wu, L., Wade, A. R. and Boroditsky, L. 2007. ‘Russian blues reveal effects of language on color discrimination’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104, 77807785.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×