Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-4rdrl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-04T07:11:05.205Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

15 - What about expository text?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 May 2015

Edward J. O'Brien
Affiliation:
University of New Hampshire
Anne E. Cook
Affiliation:
University of Utah
Robert F. Lorch, Jr
Affiliation:
University of Kentucky
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Albrecht, J. E., & O'Brien, E. J. (1993). Updating a mental model: maintaining both local and global coherence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1061–70.Google Scholar
Cook, A. E., & O'Brien, E. J. (2014). Knowledge activation, integration, and validation during narrative text comprehension. Discourse Processes, 51, 2649.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilabert, R., Martınez, R., & Vidal-Abarca, E. (2005).Some good texts are always better: text revision to foster inferences of readers with high and low prior background knowledge. Learning and Instruction, 15, 4568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A. M., Meyer, M., and Lindem, K. (1987). Mental models contribute to foregrounding during text comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 6983.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graesser, A. C., Singer, M., & Trabasso, T. (1994). Constructing inferences during narrative text comprehension. Psychological Review, 101(3), 371–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyöna, J., Lorch, R. F. Jr., & Kaakinen, J. K. (2002). Individual differences in reading to summarize expository text: evidence from eye fixation patterns. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 4455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendeou, P., & O'Brien, E. J. (2014). Knowledge revision: processes and mechanisms. In Rapp, D. N. & Braasch, J. L. G. (eds.), Processing Inaccurate Information: Theoretical and Applied Perspectives from Cognitive Science and the Educational Sciences (pp.353–77). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kendeou, P., Walsh, E., Smith, E. R., & O'Brien, E. J. (2014). Knowledge revision processes in refutation texts. Discourse Processes.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendeou, P., & van den Broek, P. (2005). The effects of readers’ misconceptions on comprehension of scientific text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 235–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendeou, P., & van den Broek, P. (2007). Interactions between prior knowledge and text structure during comprehension of scientific texts. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1567–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: a construction–integration model. Psychological Review, 95, 163–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kintsch, W., & van Dijk, T.A. (1978). Towards a model of text comprehension and production. Psychological Review, 85, 363–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linderholm, T., & van den Broek, P. (2002). The effects of reading purpose and working memory capacity on the processing of expository text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 778–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magliano, J.P., Miller, J., & Zwaan, R.A. (2001). Indexing space and time in film understanding. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, 533–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (1992). Inference during reading. Psychological Review, 99(3), 440–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNamara, D. S., Kintsch, E., Songer, N. B., & Kintsch, W. (1996). Are good texts always better? Interactions of text coherence, background knowledge, and levels of understanding in learning from text. Cognition and Instruction, 14(1), 143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, B. J. F. (1975). The Organization of Prose and Its Effect on Memory. Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Millis, K. K., & Graesser, A. C. (1994). The time-course of constructing knowledge-based inferences for scientific texts. Journal of Memory and Language, 33(5), 583–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrow, D.G., Greenspan, S.I., & Bower, G.H. (1987). Accessibility and situation models in narrative comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 165–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myers, J. L., & O'Brien, E. J. (1998). Accessing the discourse representation during reading. Discourse Processes, 26, 131–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noordman, L., Vonk, W., & Kempf, H. (1992). Causal inferences during reading of expository texts. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 573–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Brien, E. J. (1995). Automatic components of discourse comprehension. In Lorch, R. F. & O'Brien, E. J. (eds.), Sources of Coherence in Reading (pp. 159–76). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
O'Brien, E. J., & Albrecht, J. E. (1991). The role of context in accessing antecedents in text. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17(1), 94102.Google Scholar
O'Brien, E. J., Albrecht, J. E., Hakala, C. M., & Rizzella, M. L. (1995). Activation and suppression of antecedents during reinstatement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(3), 626–34.Google Scholar
O'Brien, E. J., Cook, A. E., & Guéraud, S. (2010). Accessibility of outdated information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 979–91.Google ScholarPubMed
Radvansky, G. A. (2009). Spatial directions and situation model organization. Memory & Cognition, 37, 796806.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radvansky, G. A., Zwaan, R. A., Federico, T., & Franklin, N. (1998). Retrieval from temporally organized situation models. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 1224–37.Google ScholarPubMed
Singer, M., Harkness, D., & Stewart, S. T. (1997). Constructing inferences in expository text comprehension, Discourse Processes, 24, 198228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trabasso, T., Secco, T., & van den Broek, P. W. (1984). Causal cohesion and story coherence. In Mandl, H., Stein, N. L., & Trabasso, T. (eds.), Learning and Comprehension of Text (pp. 83111). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Trabasso, T., & Suh, S. (1993). Understanding text: achieving explanatory coherence through online inferences and mental operations in working memory. Discourse Processes, 16, 334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trabasso, T., & van den Broek, P. W. (1985). Causal thinking and the representation of narrative events. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 612–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trabasso, T., van den Broek, P. W., & Suh, S. Y. (1989) Logical necessity and transitivity of causal relations in stories. Discourse Processes, 12, 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van den Broek, P., Bohn-Gettler, C. M., Kendeou, P., Carlson, S., & White, M. J. (2011). When a reader meets a text: the role of standards of coherence in reading comprehension. In McCrudden, M. T., Magliano, J., & Schraw, G. (eds.), Relevance Instructions and Goal-focusing in Text Learning. Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Van den Broek, P., & Kendeou, P. (2008). Cognitive processes in comprehension of science texts: The role of co-activation in confronting misconceptions. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 335–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van den Broek, P., Lorch, R. F., Linderholm, T., & Gustafson, M. (2001). The effects of readers’ goals on inference generation and memory for texts. Memory & Cognition, 29, 1081–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van den Broek, P., Risden, K., & Husebye-Hartmann, E. (1995). The role of readers’ standards for coherence in the generation of inference during reading. In Lorch, R. F. & O'Brien, E. J. (eds.), Sources of Coherence in Reading (pp. 353–73). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Van den Broek, P., Young, M., Tzeng, Y., & Linderholm, T. (1998). The landscape model of reading: Inferences and the online construction of a memory representation. In van Oostendorp, H., & Goldman, S. R. (eds.), The Construction of Mental Representations during Reading (pp. 7198). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Wiley, J., Ash, I. K., Sanchez, C. A., & Jaeger, A. (2011). Clarifying goals of reading for understanding from expository science text. In McCrudden, M. T., Magliano, J. P., & Schraw, G. (eds.), Text Relevance and Learning from Text (pp. 353–74). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Wiley, J., & Myers, J. L. (2003). Availability and accessibility of information and causal inferences from scientific text. Discourse Processes, 36, 109–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiley, J., & Sanchez, C. A. (2010). Constraints on learning from expository science texts. In Stein, N. L. & Raudenbush, S. (eds.), Developmental Cognitive Science Goes to School (pp. 4558). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Zwaan, R. A., Magliano, J. P., & Graesser, A. C. (1995). Dimensions of situation model construction in narrative comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 386–97.Google Scholar
Zwaan, R. A., & Radvansky, G. A. (1998). Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 162185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×