Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-sjtt6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-04T06:31:49.106Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

17 - Interpretive inferences in literature

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 May 2015

Edward J. O'Brien
Affiliation:
University of New Hampshire
Anne E. Cook
Affiliation:
University of Utah
Robert F. Lorch, Jr
Affiliation:
University of Kentucky
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Carlsen, G. R. (1981). Literature isn’t supposed to be realistic. English Journal, 70, 810.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chi, M. T. H., Feltovich, P. J., & Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices. Cognitive Science, 5, 121–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Claassen, E. (2012). Author Representation in Literary Reading. Utrecht, Netherlands: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fish, S. (1980). Is There a Text in This Class? The Authority of Interpretive Communities. London: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. (2001). Authorial intentions in text understanding. Discourse Processes, 32, 7380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, S. R. (1997). Learning from text: reflections on the past and suggestions for the future. Discourse Processes, 23, 357–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, S. R. Cognitive aspects of constructing meaning through and across multiple texts. In Shuart-Faris, N. & Bloome, D. (eds.), Uses of Intertextuality in Classroom and Educational Research (pp. 317–51). Greenwich, CT: Information Age.Google Scholar
Goldman, S. R., & Bloome, D. M. (2005). Learning to construct and integrate. In Healy, A. F. (ed.), Experimental Cognitive Psychology and Its Applications: Festshrift in Honor of Lyle Bourne, Walter Kintsch, and Thomas Landauer (pp. 169–82). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Goldman, S. R. Reyes, M., & Varnhagen, C. K. (1984). Understanding fables in first and second languages. Bilingual Research Journal, 8, 3566.Google Scholar
Goldman, S. R., Varma, S., & Coté, N. (1996). Extending capacity-constrained construction integration: toward “smarter” and flexible models of text comprehension. In Britton, B. K. & Graesser, A. C. (eds.), Models of Text Comprehension (pp. 73113). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Graesser, A. C., Olde, B., & Klettke, B. (2002). How does the mind construct and represent stories? In Green, M. C., Strange, J. J., & Brock, T. C (eds.) Narrative Impact: Social and Cognitive Foundations (pp. 229–62). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Graesser, A. C., Louwerse, M. M., McNamara, D. S., Olney, A., Cai, Z., & Mitchell, H. H. (2007). Inference generation and cohesion in the construction of situation models: some connections with computational linguistics. In Schmalhofer, F. & Perfetti, C. A. (eds.), Higher Level Language Processes in the Brain (pp. 289310). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Graesser, A. C., Singer, M., & Trabasso, T. (1994). Constructing inferences during narrative text comprehension. Psychological Review, 101, 371–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graves, B., & Frederiksen, C. H. (1991). Literary expertise in the description of fictional narrative. Poetics, 20, 126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heider, F. (1958). The Psychology of Interpersonal Relations. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hillocks, G., & Ludlow, L. H. (1984). A taxonomy of skills in reading and interpreting fiction. American Educational Research Journal, 21, 724.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, D. F., & Goldman, S. R. (1987). Children’s recognition and use of rules of moral conduct in stories. The American Journal of Psychology, 100, 205–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W. (1994). Text comprehension, memory, and learning. American Psychologist, 49(4), 294303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A Paradigm for Cognition. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kintsch, W., & van Dijk, T. A. (1978). Toward a model of text comprehension and production. Psychological Review, 85, 363–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kurtz, V., & Schober, M. F. (2001). Readers’ varying interpretations of theme in short fiction. Poetics, 29, 139–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langer, J. A. (2010). Envisioning Literature: Literary Understanding and Literature Instruction, 2nd edn. New York: Teachers College Press.Google Scholar
Lee, C. D. (2001). Is October Brown Chinese? A cultural modeling activity system for underachieving students. American Educational Research Journal,38(1), 97141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, C. D. (2007). Culture, Literacy, and Learning: Taking Bloom in the Midst of the Whirlwind. New York: Teachers College Press.Google Scholar
Levine, S. R. (2013). Making interpretation visible with an affect-based strategy, (unpublished doctoral dissertation). Northwestern University, Evanston, IL.Google Scholar
Magliano, J. P., & Millis, K. K. (2003). Assessing reading skill with a think-aloud procedure. Cognition and Instruction. 21, 251–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandler, J. M., & Goodman, M. S. (1982). On the psychological validity of story structure. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 507–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandler, J. M., & Johnson, N. S. (1977). Remembrance of things parsed: story structure and recall. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 111–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, K. S., & Goldman, S. R. (in press). Comprehension of short stories: effect of task instruction on literary interpretation. Discourse Processes.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., Rouet, J. F., & Britt, M. A. (1999). Toward a theory of documents representation. In van Oostendorp, H. & Goldman, S. R. (eds.), The Construction of Mental Representations during Reading (pp. 99122). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Peskin, J. (1998). Constructing meaning when reading poetry: an expert-novice study. Cognition and Instruction, 16, 135263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purves, A. (1971). Evaluation of learning in literature. In Bloom, B. S., Hastings, J. T., & Madaus, G. (eds.), Handbook on Formative and Summative Evaluation of Student Learning (pp. 697766). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Rabinowitz, P. (1987). Before Reading: Narrative Conventions and the Politics of Interpretation. Ohio State University Press.Google Scholar
RAND Reading Study Group (2002). Reading for Understanding: Toward an R&D Program in reading comprehension. Santa Monica, CA: Rand Education. Also available at www.rand.org/multi/achievementforall/reading/Google Scholar
Rosenblatt, L. (1994). The transactional theory of reading and writing. In Ruddell, R. B., Ruddell, M. R., & Singer, H. (eds.), Theoretical Models and Processes of Reading (4th edn., pp. 1057–92). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.Google Scholar
Rouet, J. F. (2006). The Skills of Document Use: From Text Comprehension to Web-based Learning. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rouet, J. F., & Britt, M. A. (2011). Relevance processes in multiple document comprehension. In McCrudden, M. T., Magliano, J. P., & Schraw, G. (eds.), Text Relevance and Learning from Text (pp. 1952). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E. (1975). Notes on a schema for stories. In Bobrow, D. & Collins, A. (eds.), Representation and Understanding: Studies in Cognitive Science. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Schmidt, S. J. (1982). Foundations of the Empirical Study of Literature. Hamburg: Buske.Google Scholar
Scholes, R. (1985). Textual Power: Literary Theory and the Teaching of English. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Schraw, G. (1997). Situational interest in literary text. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 22, 436–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, M. (1991). Understanding Unreliable Narrators: Reading between the Lines in the Literature Classroom. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English.Google Scholar
Stein, N. L., & Glenn, C. G. (1979). An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. In Freedle, R. (ed.), Discourse Processing: Multidisciplinary Perspectives. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Van den Broek, P., Bohn-Gettler, C. M., Kendeou, P., Carlson, S., & White, M. K. (2011). When a reader meets a text. In McCrudden, M. T., Magliano, J. P., & Schraw, G. (eds.), Text Relevance and Learning from Text (pp. 123–39). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Van den Broek, P., Lorch, R. F., Linderholm, T., & Gustafson, M. (2001). The effect of readers’ goals on inference generation and memory for texts. Memory & Cognition, 29, 1081–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Dijk, T. A., & Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies of Discourse Comprehension. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Vipond, D., & Hunt, R. A. (1984). Point-driven understanding: pragmatic and cognitive dimensions of literary reading. Poetics, 13, 261–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vonnegut, K. 1968. Harrison Bergeron. In Welcome to the Monkey House (pp. 713). New York: Delacorte.Google Scholar
Warren, J. E. (2011). “Generic” and “specific” expertise in English: an expert/novice study in poetry interpretation and academic argument. Cognition and Instruction, 29, 349–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winner, E. (1988). The Point of Words: Children’s Understanding of Metaphor and Irony. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wolfe, M. B., & Goldman, S. R. (2005). Relationships between adolescents’ text processing and reasoning. Cognition and Instruction, 23, 467502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeitz, C. M. (1994). Expert-novice differences in memory, abstraction, and reasoning in the domain of literature. Cognition and Instruction, 4, 277312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×