Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-2xdlg Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-25T22:33:59.888Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

1 - Comprehending implicit meanings in text without making inferences

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 May 2015

Edward J. O'Brien
Affiliation:
University of New Hampshire
Anne E. Cook
Affiliation:
University of Utah
Robert F. Lorch, Jr
Affiliation:
University of Kentucky
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Brown, C., & Hagoort, P. (1993). The processing nature of the N400: evidence from masked priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5(1), 3444.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. (eds.), (2008). Children’s Comprehension Problems in Oral and Written Language: A Cognitive Perspective. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Cain, K., Oakhill, J. V., Barnes, M. A., & Bryant, P. E. (2001). Comprehension skill, inference-making ability, and their relation to knowledge. Memory & Cognition, 29(6), 850–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cutting, L. E., Materek, A., Cole, C. A., Levine, T. M., & Mahone, E. M. (2009). Effects of fluency, oral language, and executive function on reading comprehension performance. Annals of Dyslexia, 59(1), 3454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Federmeier, K. D. (2007). Thinking ahead: the role and roots of prediction in language comprehension. Psychophysiology, 44(4), 491505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gernsbacher, M. A. (1990). Language Comprehension as Structure Building. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gernsbacher, M. A., & Faust, M. E. (1991). The mechanism of suppression: a component of general comprehension skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17(2), 245–62.Google ScholarPubMed
Gordon, P. C., & Hendrick, R. (1997). Intuitive knowledge of linguistic co-reference. Cognition, 62(3), 325–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., Johnson, M., & Lee, Y. (2006). Similarity-based interference during language comprehension: evidence from eye tracking during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(6), 1304–21.Google ScholarPubMed
Graesser, A. C., Singer, M., & Trabasso, T. (1994). Constructing inferences during narrative text comprehension. Psychological Review, 101(3), 371–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1980). Mental models in cognitive science. Cognitive Science, 4(1), 71115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). A theory of reading: from eye fixations to comprehension. Psychological review, 87, 329–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: a construction-integration model. Psychological Review, 95, 163–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuperberg, G. R., Paczynski, M., & Ditman, T. (2011). Establishing causal coherence across sentences: an ERP study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(5), 1230–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2000). Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(12), 463–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207(4427), 203–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lau, E., Almeida, D., Hines, P. C., & Poeppel, D. (2009). A lexical basis for N400 context effects: evidence from MEG. Brain and Language, 111(3), 161–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKoon, G., Gerrig, R. J., & Greene, S. B. (1996). Pronoun resolution without pronouns: some consequences of memory-based text processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(4), 919.Google ScholarPubMed
McKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (1992). Inference during reading. Psychological Review, 99(3), 440–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myers, J. L., & O’Brien, E. J. (1998). Accessing the discourse representation during reading. Discourse Processes, 26(2–3), 131–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oakhill, J. V., Cain, K., & Bryant, P. E. (2003). The dissociation of word reading and text comprehension: evidence from component skills. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18(4), 443–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Brien, E. J., Rizzella, M. L., Albrecht, J. E., & Halleran, J. G. (1998). Updating a situation model: a memory-based text processing view. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(5), 1200–10.Google Scholar
Otten, M., & van Berkum, J. J. (2008). Discourse-based word anticipation during language processing: prediction or priming? Discourse Processes, 45(6), 464–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C. (2007). Reading ability: lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(4), 357–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C., & Stafura, J. (2014). Word knowledge in a theory of reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18(1), 2237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C., Yang, C. L. and Schmalhofer, F. 2008. Comprehension skill and word-to-text integration processes. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 303–18. [CrossRef], [Web of Science ®]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pressley, M., & Afflerbach, P. (1995). Verbal Protocols of Reading: The Nature of Constructively Responsive Reading. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Singer, M., & Halldorson, M. (1996). Constructing and validating motive bridging inferences. Cognitive Psychology, 30(1), 138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stafura, J., & Perfetti, C. A. (2014). Word-to-text integration: message level and lexical level influence in ERPs. Neuropsychologia, 64, 4153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stafura, J. Rickles, B., & Perfetti, C. A. (unpublished). ERP indicators of memory resonance processes in word-to-text integration.Google Scholar
Tyler, L. K., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1977). The online effects of semantic context on syntactic processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16(6), 683–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van den Broek, P., Risden, K., & Husebye-Hartmann, E. (1995). The role of readers’ standards for coherence in the generation of inferences during reading. In Lorch, R. F. and O’Brien, E. J. (eds.), Sources of Coherence in Reading (pp. 353–73). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Van Dijk, T. A., & Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies of discourse, ed. Van Dijk, T. A. & Kintsch, W., Strategies of Discourse Comprehension. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Van Dyke, J. A., & McElree, B. (2006). Retrieval interference in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 55(2), 157–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Dyke, J. A., Johns, C. L., & Kukona, A. (2014). Low working memory capacity is only spuriously related to poor reading comprehension. Cognition, 131, 373403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, C. L., Perfetti, C. A., & Schmalhofer, F. (2007). Event-related potential indicators of text integration across sentence boundaries. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(1), 5589.Google ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×