Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-dfsvx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-28T03:59:37.442Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - Gender Differences in Math: Cognitive Processes in an Expanded Framework

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

James P. Byrnes
Affiliation:
University of Maryland
Ann M. Gallagher
Affiliation:
Law School Admissions Council, Newton, PA
James C. Kaufman
Affiliation:
California State University, San Bernardino
Get access

Summary

Scientists and nonscientists alike construct theories to explain variations in the environment (Byrnes, 2001a). For example, zoologists devise theories to explain observable differences in the physical appearance of species, and developmental scientists create theories to explain changes in performance that occur between early childhood and adulthood. The authors in this book are chiefly concerned with variations in math performance that are evident when one compares boys with girls or men with women. Explaining these gender-based variations is not an easy task because the size and direction of differences change with age, content, measure, and context (Hyde, Fennema, & Lamon, 1990). The purpose of this chapter is to present a comprehensive account of gender differences that explains most of the variations that have been revealed to date. As I will argue later, the primary virtue of such an account is that it could form the basis of highly effective forms of intervention.

The rest of this chapter is organized as follows. In the first section, I provide a brief overview of the pattern of gender differences that have been reported in the literature. This pattern represents the phenomenon that needs to be explained by any theory of gender differences. In the second section, I summarize and critique existing explanations of these findings (including a Cognitive Process approach that my colleagues and I proposed in the mid-1990s). In the third section, I present a new explanatory model that was created to extend and integrate the existing explanations (called the Three Conditions model).

Type
Chapter
Information
Gender Differences in Mathematics
An Integrative Psychological Approach
, pp. 73 - 98
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2004

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alexander, J. E., O'Boyle, M. W., & Benbow, C. P. (1996). Developmentally advanced EEG alpha power in gifted male and female adolescents. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 23, 25–31CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Becker, B. J. (1990). Item characteristics and gender differences on SAT-M for mathematically able youths. American Educational Research Journal, 27, 65–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benbow, C. P. (1986). Physiological correlates of extreme intellectual precocity. Neuropsychologia, 24, 719–725CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benbow, C. P. (1988). Sex differences in mathematical reasoning ability in intellectually talented preadolescents: Their nature, effects, and possible causes. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 169–232CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benbow, C. P., & Stanley, J. C. (1980). Sex differences in mathematical ability: Fact or artifact? Science, 210, 1262–1264CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryden, M. P., McManus, I. C., & Bulman-Fleming, M. B. (1994). Evaluating the support for the Geschwind-Behan-Galabura model of cerebral lateralization. Brain and Cognition, 26, 103–167CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrnes, J. P. (2001a). Cognitive development and learning in instructional contexts (2nd ed.). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon
Byrnes, J. P. (2001b). Minds, brains, and learning: Understanding the psychological and educational relevance of neuroscientific research. New York: Guilford
Byrnes, J. P. (2003). Factors predictive of mathematics achievement in White, Black, and Hispanic 12th graders. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 316–326CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrnes, J. P., Li, H., & Shaoying, X. (1997). Gender differences on the math subtest of the scholastic aptitude test may be culture-specific. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 34, 49–66CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrnes, J. P., & Takahira, S. (1993). Explaining gender differences on SAT-math items. Developmental Psychology, 29, 805–810CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrnes, J. P., & Takahira, S. (1994). Why some students perform well and others perform poorly on SAT-math items. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 19, 63–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casey, M. B., Nuttall, R., Pezaris, E., & Benbow, C. P. (1995). The influence of spatial ability on gender differences in mathematics college entrance test scores across diverse samples. Developmental Psychology, 31, 697–705CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casey, M. B., Pezaris, E., & Nuttall, R. L. (1992). Spatial ability as a predictor of math achievement: The importance of sex and handedness patterns. Neuropsychologia, 30, 35–45CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eccles, J. (1983). Expectancies, values, and academic behavior. In J. T. Spence (Ed.), Achievement and achievement motives: Psychological and sociological approaches (pp. 77–146). San Francisco: W. H. Freeman
Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998). Motivation to succeed. In W. Damon (Series Ed.), N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed., pp. 1018–1095). New York: Wiley
Entwisle, D. R., & Alexander, K. L. (1992). Summer setback: Race, poverty, school composition, and mathematics achievement in the first two years of school. American Sociological Review, 57, 72–84CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (1996). The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts, sports, and games. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Fennema, E., & Sherman, J. (1977). Sex-related differences in mathematics achievement, spatial visualization, and affective factors. American Educational Research Journal, 14, 51–71CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fennema, E., & Sherman, J. (1978). Sex-related differences in mathematics achievement and related factors: A further study. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 9, 189–203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallagher, A. M., Lisi, R., Holst, P. C., McGillicuddy-De Lisi, A. V., Morely, M., & Cahalan, C. (2000). Gender differences in advanced mathematical problem solving. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 75, 165–190CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gettinger, M. (1984). Achievement as a function of time spent in learning and time needed for learning. American Educational Research Journal, 21, 617–628CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geschwind, N., & Galabura, A. M. (1985). Cerebral lateralization: Biological mechanisms, associations, and pathology I: A hypothesis and a program for research. Archives of Neurology, 42, 428–459CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. (1987, August). Sex differences in item performance on a standardized achievement battery. Paper presented at the 95th annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, New York
Guiton, G., & Oakes, J. (1995). Opportunity to learn and conceptions of educational equality. Educational Evaluation & Policy Analysis, 17, 323–336CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halpern, D. F. (2000). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (4th ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Hanna, G., Kundinger, E., & Larouche, C. (1992). Mathematical achievement of grade 12 girls from fifteen countries. In L. Burton (Ed.), Gender and mathematics: An international perspective (pp. 86–97). London: Cassell
Hyde, J. S., Fennema, E., & Lamon, S. J. (1990). Gender differences in mathematical performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 139–155CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kimball, M. M. (1989). A new perspective on women's math achievement. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 198–214CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leder, G. C. (1992). Mathematics and gender: Changing perspectives. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematical teaching and learning (pp. 597–624). New York: Macmillan
Lewis, J., & Hoover, H. D. (1987). Differential prediction of academic achievement in elementary and junior high school by sex. Journal of Early Adolescence, 7, 107–115CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linn, M. C., & Kessel, C. (1996). Success in mathematics: Increasing talent and gender diversity among college majors. CBMS Issues in Mathematics Education, 6, 101–144CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1973). The working brain. New York: Basic Books
Morrison, F. J., Griffith, E. M., & Alberts, D. M. (1997). Nature–nurture in the classroom: Entrance age, school readiness, and learning in children. Developmental Psychology, 33, 254–262CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newman, R. S. (1984). Children's achievement and self-evaluations in mathematics: A longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 857–873CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Math = male, me = female, therefore math ≠ me. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 44–59CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Boyle, M. W., & Benbow, C. P. (1990). Enhanced right hemisphere involvement during cognitive processing may relate to intellectual precocity. Neuropsychologia, 28, 211–216CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Boyle, M. W., & Gill, H. S. (1998). On the relevance of research findings in cognitive neuroscience to educational practice. Educational Psychology Review, 10, 397–410CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pallas, A. M., & Alexander, K. L. (1983). Sex differences in quantitative SAT performance: New evidence on the differential coursework hypothesis. American Educational Research Journal, 20, 165–182CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Royer, J. M., Tronsky, L. N., Chan, Y., Jackson, S. J., & Marchant, H. (1999). Math-fact retrieval and the cognitive mechanism underlying gender differences in math test performance. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 24, 181–266CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, W. H. (2001). Why schools matter: A cross-national comparison of curriculum and learning. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass
Smith, A. E., Jussim, L., & Eccles, J. E. (1999). Do self-fulfilling prophecies accumulate, dissipate, or remain stable over time? Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 77, 548–565CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, R. E. (1998). Abilities as aptitudes and achievements in learning situations. In J. J. McArdle & R. W. Woodcock (Eds.), Human cognitive abilities in theory and practice (pp. 93–112). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360–407CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Takahira, S. (1995). Cross-cultural study on variables influencing gender differences in mathematics performance. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Maryland, College Park
Taylor, B. M., Frye, B. J., & Maruyama, G. M. (1990). Time spent reading and reading growth. American Educational Research Journal, 27, 351–362CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, B. M., Pressley, M., & Pearson, D. (2000). Effective Teachers and Schools: Trends across Recent Studies. Ann Arbor, MI: Center for the Improvement of Early Reading Achievement, Ann Arbor, MI
Waltz, J. A., Knowlton, B. J., Holyoak, K. J., Boone, K. B., Mishkin, F. S., Menezes Santos, M., Thomas, C. R., & Miller, B. L. (1999). A system for relational reasoning in human prefrontal cortex. Psychological Science, 10, 119–125CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., Yoon, K. S., Harold, R. D., Arbreton, A. J. A., Freedman-Doan, C., & Blumenfeld, P. C. (1997). Change in children's competence beliefs and subjective task values across the elementary school years: A 3-year study. Developmental Psychology, 89, 451–468Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×