Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-skm99 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T16:06:10.075Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - A Psychobiosocial Model: Why Females Are Sometimes Greater Than and Sometimes Less Than Males in Math Achievement

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Diane F. Halpern
Affiliation:
Claremont McKenna College
Jonathan Wai
Affiliation:
Claremont McKenna College
Amanda Saw
Affiliation:
Claremont Graduate University
Ann M. Gallagher
Affiliation:
Law School Admissions Council, Newton, PA
James C. Kaufman
Affiliation:
California State University, San Bernardino
Get access

Summary

We have some numbers that may surprise you, but first you need to supply some of your own. Make your best estimate in answering the following questions: What percentage of all accountants and auditors in the United States in 1983 were female? Now answer the same question for the year 2000. What about other math-intensive professions, say economists? What percentage of economists in the United States in 1983 and in 2000 were female? What about the percentage of all engineers who were female in 1983 and in 2000? Finally, is the difference in achievement scores between girls and boys much larger on tests of reading literacy or tests of mathematical literacy?

Are you fairly confident about your answers? Give yourself a “point” for each answer you supplied that is within five percentage points of the correct answer. According to the U.S. Bureau of the Census (2001), the majority of accountants and auditors in 2000 were female (56.7%), up considerably from 1983 when females made up slightly more than one-third of this profession (36.7%). The comparable values for economists in 1983 and 2000 were 37.9% and 53.3%. Surprised that these values are so high? Most people are. What about engineers? Females were 5.9% of all engineers in 1983 and still only 9.9% in 2000 – values that are probably closer to what most people estimated for all these math-intensive professions.

Type
Chapter
Information
Gender Differences in Mathematics
An Integrative Psychological Approach
, pp. 48 - 72
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2004

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ackerman, P. L. (2002). Gender differences in intelligence and knowledge: How should we look at achievement score differences? Issues in Education: Contributions from Educational Psychology
Ackerman, P. L., Bowen, K. R., Beier, M., & Kanfer, R. (2001). Determinants of individual differences and gender differences in knowledge. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 797–825CrossRefGoogle Scholar
American Association of University Women. (1992). How schools shortchange girls: The AAUW report. New York: Marlowe
Astin, A. W. (1993). What matters in college? Four critical years revisited. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass
Banaji, M. R., & Hardin, C. D. (1996). Automatic stereotyping. Psychological Science, 7, 136–141CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F. (1988). Should we stop studying sex differences altogether? American Psychologist, 42, 756–757Google Scholar
Benbow, C. P. (1988). Sex differences in mathematical reasoning ability in intellectually talented preadolescents: Their nature, effects, and possible causes. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 169–232CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benbow, C. P., & Stanley, J. C. (1983). Sex differences in mathematical reasoning ability: More facts. Science, 222, 1029–1031CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birenbaum, M., Kelly, A. E., & Levi-Keren, M. (1994). Stimulus features and sex differences in mental rotation test performance. Intelligence, 19, 51–64CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryden, M. P. (1977). Measuring handedness with questionnaires. Neuropsychologia, 15, 617–624CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casey, M. B., Nuttall, R., Pezaris, E., & Benbow, C. P. (1995). The influence of spatial ability on gender difference in mathematics college entrance test scores across diverse samples. Developmental Psychology, 31, 697–705CrossRefGoogle Scholar
College Entrance Examination Board. (1997). National report on college-bound seniors, various years. New York: Author
De Lisi, R., & McGillicuddy-De Lisi, A. (2002). Sex differences in mathematical abilities and achievement. In McGillicuddy-De Lisi, A., & De Lisi, R. (Eds.), Biology, Society, and Behavior: The development of sex differences in cognition (pp. 155–181). CT: Ablex Publishing
Diamond, M. C. (1999). Enrichment response of the brain. In G. Adelman & J. DePasquale (Eds.), Elsevier's encyclopedia of neuroscience (pp. 655–657). New York: Elsevier Science
Dwyer, C. A., & Johnson, L. M. (1997). Grades, accomplishments, and correlates. In Willingham, W. W., & Cole, N. S. (Eds.), Gender and fair assessment (pp. 127–156). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Eagly, A. H. (1990). On the advantages of reporting sex comparisons. American Psychologist, 45, 560–562CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eagly, A. H. (1994). On comparing women and men. Feminism & Psychology, 4, 513–522CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eccles, J. S. (1987). Gender roles and women's achievement-related decisions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 11, 135–172CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eccles, J. S. (1994). Understanding women's educational and occupational choices: Applying the Eccles et al. model of achievement-related choices. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18, 585–609CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evelyn, J. (2002, June 28). Community colleges start to ask, where are the men? 151 women receive associate degrees for every 100 men who do. The Chronicle of Higher Education, p. A32
Gallagher, A. M., & Lisi, R. (1994). Gender differences in scholastic aptitude test mathematics problem solving among high ability students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 204–211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geschwind, N. (1983). Biological associations of left-handedness. Annals of Dyslexia, 33, 29–40CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geschwind, N. (1984). Cerebral dominance in biological perspective. Neuropsychologia, 22, 675–683CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grandy, J. (1994). GRE – Trends & profiles: Statistics about general test examinees by sex and ethnicity (ETS RR-94–1) and Supplementary tables (ETS RR-94–1A). Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service
Greenough, W. T., Black, J. E., & Wallace, C. S. (1987). Experience and brain development. Child Development, 58, 539–559CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halpern, D. F. (1997). Sex differences in intelligence: Implications for educations. American Psychologist, 52, 1091–1102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halpern, D. F. (2000). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Halpern, D. F., & Collaer, M. L. (in press). Sex differences in visuospatial abilities: More than meets the eye. In P. Shah & A. Miyake (Eds.), Higher-level visuospatial thinking and cognition. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press
Halpern, D. F., Haviland, M. G., & Killian, C. D. (1998). Handedness and sex differences in intelligence: Evidence from the Medical College Admission Test. Brain and Cognition, 38, 87–101CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hampson, E. (1990a). Estrogen-related variations in human spatial and articulatory-motor skills. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 15, 97–111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hampson, E. (1990b). Variations in sex-related cognitive abilities across the menstrual cycle. Brain and Cognition, 14, 26–43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hampson, E., & Kimura, D. (1988). Reciprocal effects of hormonal fluctuations on human motor and perceptual-spatial skills. Behavioral Neuroscience, 102, 456–495CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hedges, L. V., & Nowell, A. (1995). Sex differences in mental test scores, variability, and numbers of high-scoring individuals. Science, 269, 41–45CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heister, G., Landis, T., Regard, M., & Schroeder-Heister, P. (1989). Shift of functional cerebral asymmetry during the menstrual cycle. Neuropsychologia, 27, 871–880CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hines, M. (1982). Prenatal gonadal hormones and sex differences in human behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 56–80CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyde, J. S., Fennema, E., & Lamon, S. J. (1990). Gender differences in mathematics performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 139–153CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyde, J. S., Fennema, E., Ryan, M., Frost, L. A., & Hopp, C. (1990). Gender comparisons of mathematics attitudes and affect: A meta-analysis. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 2, 299–324CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobson, L. (2002, April 24). Poll: Public sees schools as a priority. Education Week. Retrieved June 29, 2002, from http://www.edweek.org/ew/newstory
Kimura, D., & Hampson, E. (1994). Cognitive pattern in men and women is influenced by fluctuations in sex hormones. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3, 57–61CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loring-Meier, S., & Halpern, D. F. (1999). Sex differences in visuospatial working memory: Components of cognitive processing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6(3), 464–471CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maccoby, E. E. (1966). Sex differences in intellectual functioning. In E. E. Maccoby (Ed.), The development of sex differences (pp. 25–55). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press
MacGillis, A. (2001, December 31). Gender gap dogs nation's vet schools: Nearly 75٪ of students are women, leading some to predict far-reaching changes in the profession. The Baltimore Sun. Retrieved June 29, 2002, from http://web.lexis-nexis.com/universe
Matlin, M. W. (2002). Cogntion (5th ed.). Fort Worth: Harcourt College Publishers
Moffat, S. D., & Hampson, E. (1996). A curvilinear relationship between testosterone and spatial cognition in humans: Possible influence of hand preference. Psychoneuroendocrinolgy, 21, 323–337CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naglieri, J. A., & Rojahn, J. (2001). Gender differences in planning, attention, simultaneous, and successive (PASS) cognitive processes and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 430–437CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Assessment of Educational Progress. (1999). NAEP trends in academic progress. U.S. Department of Education
National Center for Education Statistics (NCES). (1997). Pursuing excellence: A study of U.S. fourth-grade mathematics and science achievement in international context (NCES 97–255). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office
National Center for Education Statistics (NCES). (1998). Pursuing excellence: A study of U.S. twelfth-grade mathematics and science achievement in international context (NCES 98–049). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office
National Center for Education Statistics (NCES). (2000). Pursuing excellence: Comparisons of international eighth-grade mathematics and science achievement from a U.S. perspective, 1995 and 1999 (NCES 2001–028). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office
National Center for Education Statistics (NCES). (2002). Outcomes of learning: Results from the 2000 Program for International Student Assessment of 15-year-olds in reading, mathematics, and science literacy (NCES 2002–115). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Retrieved July 5, 2002, from http://nces.ed.gov/quicktables
Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Math = Male, Me = Female, Therefore Math ≠ Me. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 44–59CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nyborg, H. (1983). Spatial ability in men and women: Review and new theory. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, 5, 89–140CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nyborg, H. (1990). Sex hormones, brain development and spatio-perceptual strategies in Turner syndrome. In D. B. Berch & B. G. Bender (Eds.), Sex chromosome abnormalities and human behavior (pp. 100–128). Washington, DC: American Association for the Advancement of Science
Petersen, A. C. (1976). Physical androgyny and cognitive functioning in adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 12, 524–533CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Resnick, S. M., Berenbaum, S. A., Gottesman, I. I., & Bouchard, T. J. Jr. (1986). Early hormonal influences of cognitive functioning in congenital adrenal hyperplasis. Developmental Psychology, 22, 191–198CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Resnick, S. M., Maki, P. M., Golski, S., Kraut, M. A., & Zonderman, A. B. (1998). Effects of estrogen replacement therapy on PET cerebral blood flow and neuropsychological performance. Hormones and Behavior, 34, 171–182CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rovet, J., Szekely, C., & Hockenberry, M. (1994). Specific arithmetic calculation deficits in children with Turner syndrome. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 16, 820–839CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sells, L. W. (1980). The mathematics filter and the education of women and minorities. In L. H. Fox & D. Tobin (Eds.), Women and the mathematical mystique (pp. 66–75). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press
Shih, M., Pittinsky, T. L., & Ambady, N. (1999). Stereotype susceptibility: Identity salience and shifts in quantitative performance. Psychological Science, 10, 80–83CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sommers, C. (2000). The war against boys: How misguided feminism is harming our young men. New York: Simon & Schuster
Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. American Psychologist, 52, 613–629CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steele, C. M. (1998). Stereotyping and its threat are real. American Psychologist, 53, 680–681CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stricker, L. J. (1998). Inquiring about examinees' ethnicity and sex: Effects on AP Calculus AB Examination performance (Report No. 98–1). New York: The College BoardCrossRef
Stricker, L. J., & Ward, W. C. (1998). Inquiring about examinees' ethnicity and sex: Effects on computerized placement tests performance (Report No. 98–2). New York: The College BoardCrossRef
Swainson, G. (2002, January 10). Girls not wired for science, author claims. Toronto Star. Retrieved June 29, 2002, from http://web.lexis-nexis.com/universe
Tanner, J. M. (1962). Growth and adolescence. Oxford, England: Blackwell
Ungerleider, L. G. (1995). Functional brain imaging studies of cortical mechanisms for memory. Science, 270, 769–775CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
U.S. Bureau of the Census. (2001). Statistical abstract of the United States: 2001 (121st ed.). Washington, DC: Author
Vermeer, H. J., Boekaerts, M., & Seegers, G. (2000). Motivational and gender differences: sixth-grade students' mathematical problem-solving behavior. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(2), 308–315CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Willingham, W. W., & Cole, N. S. (1997). Gender and fair assessment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×