Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-dvmhs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-09-27T23:01:39.898Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Drinking and feasting are perceived as facilitating cooperation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 October 2023

Yuhan Fu
Affiliation:
Department of Philosophy, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, UK Yfu33@sheffield.ac.uk G.viera@sheffield.ac.uk; http://gerardoviera.weebly.com/
Gerardo Viera
Affiliation:
Department of Philosophy, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, UK Yfu33@sheffield.ac.uk G.viera@sheffield.ac.uk; http://gerardoviera.weebly.com/

Abstract

We argue that the occurrence of puritanical norms cannot simply be explained by appealing to the need for cooperation. Anthropological and archaeological studies suggest that across history and cultures’ self-indulgent behaviours, such as excessive drinking, eating, and feasting, have been used to enhance cooperation by enforcing social and group identities.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

According to Fitouchi et al., moral cognition is grounded in a need for cooperation. They argue that people believe that engaging in self-indulgent behaviours, such as drinking, dancing, and feasting, amplifies the motivational force of short-term cravings, leading to a lack of self-control, which hinders cooperation. As a result, puritanical norms, which prohibit such self-indulgent, yet apparently harmless, behaviours, arise out of a need to preserve self-control which is essential for cooperation.

We argue that the relationship between puritanical norms and cooperation is more complicated than the target article claims. Archaeological and anthropological studies provide evidence that collective self-indulgent behaviours, such as drinking, dancing, and feasting, have played a role in solidifying social and group identities, which have ultimately promoted cooperation in communities across human history and culture. As a result, the existence of puritanical norms cannot be explained simply by the need for cooperation, because behaviours that violate such norms can help, rather than hinder, cooperation.

To see this, consider studies of communal drinking in ancient China. During the Neolithic and Bronze Ages (ca. 8000–1200 B.C.), China underwent rapid population growth because of the emergence of settled communities which engaged in the domestication of plants and animals. In Neolithic Northern China, unpredictable fluctuations in climate and rainfall directly affected the agricultural and economic activities that resulted from attempts to farm arid land (Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Yang, Cheng, Zhao, Yang, Liang and Edwards2018). As a result, cooperation was vital to ensuring an adequate food supply against the background of rapid population growth and environmental pressures. If Fitouchi et al. were correct, then ancient China would have been a culture in which puritanical norms would have likely emerged to preserve precious resources and facilitate cooperation. However, evidence shows that these communities spent time and effort to take part in drinking and feasting, treating such collective self-indulgent behaviours as a crucial way of building social connections to further communal goals (Feng et al., Reference Feng, Liu, Wang, Levin, Li and Ma2021; He, Liu, Sun, Shao, & Di, Reference He, Liu, Sun, Shao and Di2021; Liu, Reference Liu2021; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Wang, Levin, Sinnott-Armstrong, Zhao, Zhao and Zhang2019; Wang, Zhao, Wang, & Liu, Reference Wang, Zhao, Wang and Liu2019).

Consider first the development of community infrastructure during the mid-Yangshao period (ca. 4000–3500 B.C.) (see Liu, Reference Liu2021). During this period, with population growth there was a shift in the organisation of settlements. Public houses, located in village centres, increased in size and smaller family dwellings were built to surround these public centres. As part of these village centres, large communal hearths were constructed for communal cooking. Large, decorated, amphorae for the sharing of alcohol were also located in these centres. During this period, as populations grew there was a substantial investment of time and resources for creating the means for social interactions centred around drinking and feasting.

Why might these cultural practices have emerged? One explanation is that they helped to solidify relationships within the group, encourage the sharing of goods, and solidify group identity. Different stories can be told for how this might work, yet we can see these activities helping to establish tribal boundaries or in-group member status, thereby directly facilitating cooperation between those that indulge as part of the ritual.

These behaviours aren't unique to ancient China either. We see evidence for the role of feasting and drinking in the promotion of cooperation in a number of cultures, for example, Andean Peru (Williams & Nash, Reference Williams and Nash2021), Mesopotamia (Paulette, Reference Paulette2021), and Egypt (Wang, Friedman, & Baba, Reference Wang, Friedman and Baba2021) (for a review see Dietler, Reference Dietler2006). Similar practices can also be found in modern day rural Southwest China, where individuals drink alcohol from a communal zajiu vessel as part of the guozhuang ritual dance (Li, Reference Li2011; Liu, Reference Liu2021). This activity has the explicit goal of promoting group solidarity.

Turning to Latin America, Spier (Reference Spier1995) found evidence speaking directly to the folk belief that Fitouchi et al. attempt to establish. In the Peruvian parish of Zurite, Andean women opposed the conversion of their husbands to Protestantism because the resulting abstinence, because of puritanical norms, would eliminate a means of developing connections across households, and ultimately would destroy mutual aid networks. Once again, there is a belief that anti-puritan behaviours promote cooperation, rather than hinder it.

To this day, drinking is often considered an important catalyst for social bonding which promotes cooperation. In Western cultures this is often part of workplace “pub cultures” in which partaking in these activities is taken as evidence of being a “team player” and abstaining is cause for a lack of trust. Similar phenomena can be found across the world, such as in modern day Korea, the United Kingdom, Japan, China, Australia, and numerous other countries (Schweitzer & Kerr, Reference Schweitzer and Kerr2000). Partaking in these collective indulgent activities is taken to be an indicator of trustworthiness, and not as an indicator of a lack of self-control.

Fitouchi et al.'s account neglects a rich literature and oversimplifies the functions of puritanical norms and self-indulgent behaviours in complex social practice. Throughout human history, folk-psychological beliefs about alcohol consumption are more complex than Fitouchi et al. claim. Self-indulgent behaviours such as drinking and feasting are not always perceived as hindering cooperation. Rather, studies have shown that people treat self-indulgent behaviours as a crucial way of facilitating cooperation. From this we see how their account fails to explain why puritanical norms on prohibiting self-indulgent behaviours only exist in some societies whereas other communities promote such behaviours. It also suggests that something other than the simple need for cooperation has brought about puritanical norms in certain regions.

Competing interest

None.

References

Dietler, M. (2006). Alcohol: Anthropological/archaeological perspectives. Annual Review of Anthropology, 35, 229249. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.anthro.35.081705.123120CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feng, S., Liu, L., Wang, J., Levin, M. J., Li, X., & Ma, X. (2021). Red beer consumption and elite utensils: The emergence of competitive feasting in the Yangshao culture, North China. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 64(12), 101365. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaa.2021.101365CrossRefGoogle Scholar
He, Y., Liu, L., Sun, Z., Shao, J., & Di, N. (2021). “Proposing a toast” from the first urban center in the North Loess Plateau, China: Alcoholic beverages at Shimao. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 64, 101352. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaa.2021.101352CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, F. (2011). Zuqun yichan de xiandai bianqian: jiyu Jiarong tiao guozhuang wu de tianye kaocha (The modern changes of ethnic heritage: Fieldwork on the Jiarong Guozhuang dance). Journal of South-Central University for Nationalities (Humanities and Social Sciences), 31, 6165. Retrieved from https://www.scopus.com/inward/record.uri?eid=2-s2.0-85107040683&partnerID=40&md5=4b88103b91058610b5e65e8234d30951Google Scholar
Liu, L. (2021). Communal drinking rituals and social formations in the Yellow River valley of Neolithic China. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 63, 101310. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaa.2021.101310CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, L., Wang, J., Levin, M. J., Sinnott-Armstrong, N., Zhao, H., Zhao, Y., … Zhang, T. (2019). The origins of specialized pottery and diverse alcohol fermentation techniques in Early Neolithic China. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 116(26), 1276712774. https://doi.org/10.1073/PNAS.1902668116CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paulette, T. (2021). Inebriation and the early state: Beer and the politics of affect in Mesopotamia. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 63, 101330. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JAA.2021.101330CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schweitzer, M. E., & Kerr, J. L. (2000). Bargaining under the influence: The role of alcohol in negotiations. Academy of Management Perspectives, 14(2), 4757.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spier, F. (1995). San Nicolás de Zurite: Religion and daily life of a Peruvian Andean village in a changing world (vol. 18). VU University Press.Google Scholar
Wang, J., Friedman, R., & Baba, M. (2021). Predynastic beer production, distribution, and consumption at Hierakonpolis, Egypt. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 64, 101347. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JAA.2021.101347CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, J., Zhao, X., Wang, H., & Liu, L. (2019). Plant exploitation of the first farmers in Northwest China: Microbotanical evidence from Dadiwan. Quaternary International, 529, 39. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.QUAINT.2018.10.019CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, P. R., & Nash, D. J. (2021). Consuming kero: Molle beer and Wari social identity in Andean Peru. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 63, 101327. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaa.2021.101327CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, N., Yang, Y., Cheng, H., Zhao, J., Yang, X., Liang, S., … Edwards, R. L. (2018). Timing and duration of the East Asian summer monsoon maximum during the Holocene based on stalagmite data from North China. The Holocene, 28(10), 16311641. https://doi.org/10.1177/0959683618782606/ASSET/IMAGES/LARGE/10.1177_0959683618782606-FIG2.JPEGCrossRefGoogle Scholar