Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-5nwft Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-30T12:46:25.203Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - Production decisions and time allocation: a guide to data collection

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Ismael Vaccaro
Affiliation:
McGill University, Montréal
Eric Alden Smith
Affiliation:
University of Washington
Shankar Aswani
Affiliation:
University of California, Santa Barbara
Get access

Summary

Introduction

In this chapter I deal with methods for collecting behavioral and economic data on productive inputs and outputs. Any attempt at the collection of quantitative data requires that the researcher should ideally have prior knowledge of the full range of economic activities and perform preliminary evaluations of the accuracy of data collection procedures and coding schemes. This will prevent false starts, increase cross-cultural comparability, and lead to a more systematic account of activities. Whenever possible, I encourage researchers to rely on observational data as a kind of gold standard: it produces data amenable to sophisticated quantitative analysis, is crucial for theory testing, is more easily used for cross-cultural comparison than qualitative observations, and reduces the known errors in recall data (see Stange et al. 1998 for an illuminating account of recall errors compared to direct observation). Nevertheless, because of intrusiveness, labor intensiveness, and cultural sensitivities in direct observation, recall data are oftentimes required, but may be integrated into behavioral records. Techniques for reducing recall error (e.g., short time frames) and cross-checking are recommended in such cases.

Conventionally, economic activities may be defined as behaviors whose end result is the production of a material good (e.g., food or artifact), maintenance of an object (e.g., tool repair), or provisioning of a service (e.g., assisting a neighbor building a house). Production activities can be easily characterized along a set of input measures (time, energy, or even risk) but output measures are more complex. In food production outputs are conventionally weight, kilocalories, or macronutrients (carbohydrates, fats, and proteins) and represent common denominators for comparative and analytic purposes. Which of these currencies is appropriate will depend on the research question posed.

Type
Chapter
Information
Environmental Social Sciences
Methods and Research Design
, pp. 35 - 56
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Altmann, J. 1974 The observational study of behaviorBehaviour 48 1Google Scholar
Alvard, M. 1998 Evolutionary ecology and resource conservationEvolutionary Anthropology 73.0.CO;2-I>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aspelin, L. 1979 Food distribution and social bonding among the Mamainde of Mato Gross, BrazilJournal of Anthropological Research 35 309CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aswani, S. 1998 Patterns of marine harvest effort in southwestern New Georgia, Solomon Islands: resource management or optimal foragingOcean and Coastal Management 40 207CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aswani, S. 2002 Assessing the effects of changing demographic and consumption patterns on sea tenure regimes in the Roviana Lagoon, Solomon IslandsAmbio 31 272CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Augner, R. 1994 Are food avoidances maladaptive in the Ituri Forest of Zaire?Journal of Anthropological Research 15 5472Google Scholar
Behrens, C. A. 1981 Time allocation and meat procurement among the Shipibo Indians of eastern PeruHuman Ecology 9 189CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Betzig, L.Turke, P. 1985 Measuring time allocation: observation and intentionCurrent Anthropology 26 647CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bird, D. W.Bliege Bird, R. 1997 Contemporary shellfish gathering strategies among the Meriam of the Torres Strait Islands, Australia: testing predictions of a central place foraging modelJournal of Archaeological Science 24 39CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bird, D. W.Bliege Bird, R. 2005 Hewlett, B.Lamb, M.Hunter–Gatherer Childhoods: Evolutionary, Developmental and Cultural PerspectivesNew Brunswick, NJTransaction PublishersGoogle Scholar
Bliege Bird, R.Bird, D. 2002 Constraints of knowing or constraints of growing?Human Nature 13 239CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bliege Bird, R.Smith, E. A. 2005 Signaling theory, strategic interaction, and symbolic capitalCurrent Anthropology 46 22148CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bock, J. 2002 Learning, life history, and productivityHuman Nature 13 161CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borgerhoff Mulder, M.Caro, T. 1985 The use of quantitative observation techniques in anthropologyCurrent Anthropology 26 232Google Scholar
Durnin, J.Passmore, G. 1967 Energy, Work, and LeisureLondonHeinemannGoogle Scholar
Erasmus, C. J. 1955 Work patterns in a Mayo villageAmerican Anthropologist 57 322CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flinn, M. 1988 Betzig, L.Borgerhoff Mulder, M.Turke, P.Human Reproductive BehaviourCambridge, UKCambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Fouts, H. N.Lamb, M. E. 2005 Hewlett, B.Lamb, M.Hunter–Gatherer Childhoods: Evolutionary, Developmental and Cultural PerspectivesNew Brunswick, NJTransaction PublishersGoogle Scholar
Gross, D. R. 1984 Time allocation: a tool for the study of cultural behaviorAnnual Review of Anthropology 13 519CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gurven, M. 2004 To give and to give not: the behavioral ecology of human food transfersBehavioral and Brain Sciences 27 120CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gurven, M.Kaplan, H. 2006 Determinants of time allocation across the lifespan: a theoretical model and an application to the Machiguenga and Piro of PeruHuman Nature 17 1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gurven, M.Kaplan, H.Gutierrez, M. 2006 How long does it take to become a proficient hunter? Implications for the evolution of extended development and long life spanJournal of Human Evolution 51 454CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gurven, M.Walker, R. 2006 Energetic demand of multiple dependents and the evolution of slow human growthProceedings of the Royal Society, B 273CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hames, R. 1979 A comparison of the efficiencies of the shotgun and bow in neotropical forest huntingHuman Ecology 7 219CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hames, R. 1987 Relatedness and garden labor exchange among the Ye’kwanaEvolution and Human Behavior 8 354Google Scholar
Hames, R. 1988 Betzig, L.Borgerhoff Mulder, M.Turke, P.Human Reproductive BehaviourCambridge, UKCambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Hames, R. 1990 Cashdan, E.Risk and Reciprocity in Tribal and Peasant EconomicsBoulder, COWestview PressGoogle Scholar
Hames, R. 1991 Oldfield, M.Alcorn, J.Biodiversity: Culture, Conservation, and EcodevelopmentWestview Press172Google Scholar
Hames, R. 1992 Smith, E. A.Winterhalder, B.Evolutionary Ecology and Human BehaviorChicago, ILAldine de GruyterGoogle Scholar
Hames, R. 2007 Annual Review of Anthropology 36 177
Hames, R.McCabe, C. 2007 Meal sharing among the Ye’kwanaHuman Nature 18 1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawkes, K.Hill, K.Kaplan, H.Hurtado, M. 1987 Some problems with instantaneous scan samplingJournal of Anthropological Research 43 239CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hewlett, B. S.Lamb, M. E. 2005 Hunter–Gatherer Childhoods: Evolutionary, Developmental, and Cultural PerspectivesNew Brunswick, NJAldine de Gruyter
Hill, K.Hurtado, A.
Hipsley, E. H.Kirk, N. E. 1966 Studies of dietary intake and the expenditure of energy by New GuineansNoumea, New CaledoniaSouth Pacific CommissionGoogle Scholar
Ivey, P. 2000 Cooperative reproduction in Ituri forest hunter-gatherers: Who cares for Efe infantsCurrent Anthropology 41 857Google Scholar
Ivey Henry, P. K.Morelli, G. A.Tronick, E. Z. 2005 Child caretakers among Efe foragers of the Ituri Forest. In B. S. Hewlett and M. E. Lamb, eds., Hunter–Gatherer Childhoods: Evolutionary, Developmental and Cultural PerspectivesNew Brunswick, NJTransaction PublishersGoogle Scholar
Johnson, A. 1975 Time allocation in a Machiguenga communityEthnology 14 301CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, A. 1990 Rogers, B. L.Schlossman, N.Intra-Household Resource Allocation: Issues and Methods for Development Policy and PlanningTokyoUnited Nations University PressGoogle Scholar
Johnson, A.Sackett, R. 1998 Bernard, H. R.Handbook of Methods in Cultural AnthropologyWalnut Creek, CAAltamira PressGoogle Scholar
Kaplan, H. 1994 Evolutionary and wealth flows theories of fertility: empirical tests and new modelsPopulation and Development Review 20 75391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaplan, H.Hill, K.Hawkes, K.Hurtado, A. 1984 Food sharing among Ache foragers of Eastern ParaguayCurrent Anthropology 25 113CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Konner, M. J.Worthman, C. M. 1980 Nursing frequency, gonadal function, and birth spacing among !Kung hunter-gatherersScience 207 788CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kramer, K. 2002 Variability in the duration of juvenile dependence: the benefits of Maya children’s work to parentsHuman Nature 13 299CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kramer, K. 2005 Children’s help and the pace of reproduction: cooperative breeding in humansEvolutionary Anthropology 14 224CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, R. 1968 Lee, R.DeVore, I.Man the HunterChicago, ILAldineGoogle Scholar
Lee, R. 1969 Damas, D.Ecological EssaysOttawaNat. Museum of CanadaGoogle Scholar
Lee, R. 1979 The !Kung San: Men, Women, and Work in a Foraging SocietyCambridge, UKCambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Marlowe, F. W. 2005 Hewlett, B.Lamb, M.Hunter–Gatherer Childhoods : Evolutionary, Developmental and Cultural PerspectivesNew Brunswick, NJTransaction PublishersGoogle Scholar
Martin, P.Bateson, P. 1993 Measuring BehaviorCambridge, UKCambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitani, J. C.Watts, D. P. 2005 Correlates of territorial boundary patrol behaviour in wild chimpanzeesAnimal Behaviour 70 1079CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, E.Johnson, A. 1976 Machiguenga energy expenditureEcology of Food and Nutrition 6 97CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ohtsuka, R.Sudo, N.Sekiyama, M. 2004 Gender difference in daily time and space use among Bangladeshi villagers under arsenic hazard: application of the compact spot-check methodJournal of Biosocial Science 36 317CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panter-Brick, C. 1996 Seasonal and sex variation in physical activity levels of agro-pastoralists in NepalAmerican Journal of Physical Anthropology 100 73.0.CO;2-Y>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paolisso, M.Hames, R.
Peregrine, P.Drews, D. R.North, M.Slupe, A. 1993 Sampling techniques and sampling error in naturalistic observation: an empirical evaluation with implications for cross-cultural researchCross-Cultural Research 27 232CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scaglion, R. 1986 Importance of nighttime observations in time allocation studiesAmerican Ethnologist 13 537CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stange, K.Zyzanski, S.Fedirko Smith, T.Kelly, R. 1998 How valid are medical records and patient questionnaires for physician profiling and health services research? A comparison with direct observation of patient visitsMedical Care 36 851CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sugawara, K. 1988 Visiting relations and social interactions between residential groups of the Central Kalahari San: hunter–gatherer camp as a micro-territoryAfrican Study Monographs 8 173Google Scholar
Takada, A. 2005 Hewlett, B.Lamb, M.Hunter–Gatherer Childhoods: Evolutionary, Developmental and Cultural PerspectivesNew Brunswick, NJTransaction PublishersGoogle Scholar
Turke, P. 1989 Evolution and the demand for childrenPopulation and Development Review 5 6190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Umezaki, M.Yamauchi, T.Ohtsuka, R. 2002 Time allocation to subsistence activities among the Huli in rural and urban Papua New GuineaJournal of Biosocial Science 34 133Google ScholarPubMed
Yasuoka, H. 2006 Long-term foraging expeditionsMolongo 34 27595.Google Scholar
Veblen, T. 1899 The Theory Of The Leisure Class. An Economic Study in the Evolution of InstitutionsNew YorkMacmillanGoogle Scholar
Winking, J.Gurven, M.Kaplan, H.Stieglitz, J. 2009 The goals of direct paternal care among a South Amerindian populationAmerican Journal of Physical Anthropology 139 295CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×