Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-x24gv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-01T01:18:44.758Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7 - The Request-Return Sequence

What Can Happen at the Interface between Picking up a Repaired Item and Paying for It

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 January 2023

Barbara Fox
Affiliation:
University of Colorado Boulder
Lorenza Mondada
Affiliation:
University of Basel
Marja-Leena Sorjonen
Affiliation:
University of Helsinki
Get access

Summary

This study focuses on sequences in which shoes and other leather items are returned to customers at a shoe repair shop in North America after having been repaired in some way. Before customers’ final payment for the items, the staff engage in three different ways of presenting the returned items, each making relevant a different kind of response from the customer. First, most frequent type, the staff person places the item on the counter and then turns to ring up the sale; in the second, the staff person engages in manual displays of the repair work that has been done; and in the third, the staff person verbally pursues a response through questions. The first type of presentation does not make conditionally relevant any kind of inspection or assessment from the customer, although customers may still inspect and assess; in the second, the pursuit is a bit stronger; and in the third, through questions from the staff such as ‘good?’, an assessment is made conditionally relevant. The study queries the theoretical implications for the general lack of pursuit of assessment at the shoe shop, compared with, for example, hair salons, in which assessments are typically actively pursued by the stylist.

Type
Chapter
Information
Encounters at the Counter
The Organization of Shop Interactions
, pp. 214 - 254
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aggerholm, H. K., & Asmuß, B. (2016). When “good” is not good enough: Power dynamics and performative aspects of organizational routines. In Howard-Grenville, J., Rerup, C., Langley, A., & Tsoukas, H., eds., Organizational Routines. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 140178. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198759485.003.0007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Antaki, C., Houtkoop-Steenstra, H., & Rapley, M. (2000). “Brilliant. Next question …”: High-grade assessment sequences in the completion of interactional units. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 33(3), 235262. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327973RLSI3303_1Google Scholar
Aston, G. (1986). Ah: A corpus-based exercise in conversational analysis. In Morley, J. & Partington, A., eds., Spoken Discourse: Phonetics, Theory and Practice. Camerino: University of Camerino Press, pp. 123137.Google Scholar
Bilmes, J. (1988). The concept of preference in conversation analysis. Language in Society, 17(2), 161181. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0047404500012744CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolden, G. (2008). “So what’s up?”: Using the discourse marker so to launch conversational business. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 41(3), 302337. https://doi.org/10.1080/08351810802237909Google Scholar
Fox, B., & Heinemann, T. (2015). The alignment of manual and verbal displays in requests for the repair of an object. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 48(3), 342362. https://doi.org/10.1080/08351813.2015.1058608Google Scholar
Goffman, E. (1978). Response cries. Language, 54(4), 787815. https://doi.org/10.2307/413235CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Golato, A. (2004). Compliments and Compliment Responses. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/sidag.15Google Scholar
Goodwin, C., & Goodwin, M. H. (1992). Assessments and the construction of context. In Duranti, A. & Goodwin, C., eds., Rethinking Context: Language as an Interactive Phenomenon. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 147190.Google Scholar
Heinemann, T., & Fox, B. (2019). Dropping off or picking up? Professionals’ use of objects as a resource for determining the purpose of a customer encounter. In Day, D. & Wagner, J., eds., Objects, Bodies, and Work Practice. Bristol: Multilingual Matters, pp. 143163. https://doi.org/10.21832/9781788924535-009Google Scholar
Heritage, J. (1998). Oh-prefaced responses to inquiry. Language in Society, 27(3), 291334. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0047404500019990Google Scholar
Jefferson, G. (2004). Glossary of transcript symbols with an introduction. In Lerner, G. H., ed., Conversation Analysis: Studies from the First Generation. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 1331. https://doi.org/10.1075/pbns.125.02jefGoogle Scholar
Kaukomaa, T., Peräkylä, A., & Ruusuvuori, J. (2014). Foreshadowing a problem: Turn-opening frowns in conversation. Journal of Pragmatics, 71, 132147. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2014.08.002Google Scholar
Kendrick, K. H., & Torreira, F. (2015). The timing and construction of preference: A quantitative study. Discourse Processes, 52(4), 255289. https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2014.955997Google Scholar
Lindström, A., & Heinemann, T. (2009). Good enough: Low-grade assessments in caregiving situations. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 42(4), 309328. https://doi.org/10.1080/08351810903296465Google Scholar
Mondada, L. (2014). Conventions for multimodal transcription. Available at http://icar.univ-lyon2.fr/projets/corinte/documents/convention_transcription_multimodale.pdfGoogle Scholar
Ogden, R. (2013). Clicks and percussives in English conversation. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 43(3), 299320. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0025100313000224CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oshima, S. (2009). Beauty and Consensus: Practices for Agreeing on the Quality of Service in Client-Professional Interactions. PhD dissertation, University of Texas at Austin.Google Scholar
Oshima, S. (2014). Balancing multiple roles through consensus: Making revisions in haircutting sessions. Text & Talk, 34(6), 713136. https://doi.org/10.1515/text-2014-0024CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oshima, S., & Streeck, J. (2015). Coordinating talk and practical action: The case of hair salon service assessments. Pragmatics and Society, 6(4), 538564. https://doi.org/10.1075/ps.6.4.04oshCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pomerantz, A. (1984) Agreeing and disagreeing with assessments: Some features of preferred/dispreferred turn shapes. In Atkinson, J. M. & Heritage, J., eds., Structures of Social Action: Studies in Conversation Analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 57101. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511665868.008Google Scholar
Raclaw, J. (2013). Indexing Inferables and Organizational Shifts: ‘No’-prefaces in English Conversation. PhD thesis, University of Colorado.Google Scholar
Raclaw, J., Robles, J. S., & DiDomenico, S. M. (2016). Providing epistemic support for assessments through mobile-supported sharing activities. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 49(4), 362379. https://doi.org/10.1080/08351813.2016.1199089Google Scholar
Robles, J. S. (2012). Troubles with assessments in gifting occasions. Discourse Studies, 14(6), 753777. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461445612457490Google Scholar
Schegloff, E. A. (1998). Body torque. Social Research, 65(3), 535596.Google Scholar
Schegloff, E. A. (2007). Sequence Organization in Interaction: A Primer in Conversation Analysis, Vol. 1. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511791208CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stivers, T., & Rossano, F. (2010). Mobilizing response. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 43(1), 331. https://doi.org/10.1080/08351810903471258Google Scholar
Thompson, S. A., Fox, B., & Couper-Kuhlen, E. (2015). Grammar in Everyday Talk: Building Responsive Actions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139381154Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×