Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-p2v8j Total loading time: 0.001 Render date: 2024-05-23T03:59:51.746Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A shared novelty-seeking basis for creativity and curiosity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 August 2023

Tal Ivancovsky*
Affiliation:
The Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel moshe.bar@biu.ac.il Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Catalunya, Spain
Shira Baror
Affiliation:
The Edmond and Lily Safra Center for Brain Sciences, The Hebrew University, Jerusalem, Israel. baror.shira@gmail.com
Moshe Bar
Affiliation:
The Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel moshe.bar@biu.ac.il
*
Corresponding author: Tal Ivancovsky; Email: tala.iv@gmail.com

Abstract

Curiosity and creativity are central pillars of human growth and invention. Although they have been studied extensively in isolation, the relationship between them has not yet been established. We propose that both curiosity and creativity emanate from the same mechanism of novelty seeking. We first present a synthesis showing that curiosity and creativity are affected similarly by a number of key cognitive faculties such as memory, cognitive control, attention, and reward. We then review empirical evidence from neuroscience research, indicating that the same brain regions are involved in both curiosity and creativity, focusing on the interplay between three major brain networks: the default mode network, the salience network, and the executive control network. After substantiating the link between curiosity and creativity, we propose a novelty-seeking model (NSM) that underlies them and suggests that the manifestation of the NSM is governed by one's state of mind.

Type
Target Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abbott, J. T., Austerweil, J. L., & Griffiths, T. L. (2015). Random walks on semantic networks can resemble optimal foraging. Neural Information Processing Systems Conference; A Preliminary Version of this Work was Presented at the Aforementioned Conference, 122(3), 558.Google ScholarPubMed
Abraham, A. (2014). Is there an inverted-U relationship between creativity and psychopathology? Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 750.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abraham, A. (2019). The neuropsychology of creativity. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 27, 7176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Acar, O. A., & van den Ende, J. (2016). Knowledge distance, cognitive-search processes, and creativity: The making of winning solutions in science contests. Psychological Science, 27(5), 692699. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616634665CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adelstein, J. S., Shehzad, Z., Mennes, M., DeYoung, C. G., Zuo, X.-N., Kelly, C., … Castellanos, F. X. (2011). Personality is reflected in the brain's intrinsic functional architecture. PLoS ONE, 6(11), e27633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amabile, T. M. (1993). Motivational synergy: Toward new conceptualizations of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in the workplace. Human Resource Management Review, 3(3), 185201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amabile, T. M. (1995). Attributions of creativity: What are the consequences? Creativity Research Journal, 8(4), 423426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity and innovation in organizations (Vol. 5). Harvard Business School.Google Scholar
Anderson, N., Potočnik, K., & Zhou, J. (2014). Innovation and creativity in organizations: A state-of-the-science review, prospective commentary, and guiding framework. Journal of Management, 40(5), 12971333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andreasen, N. J., & Powers, P. S. (1975). Creativity and psychosis: An examination of conceptual style. Archives of General Psychiatry, 32(1), 7073.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Sepulcre, J., Poulin, R., & Buckner, R. L. (2010). Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brain's default network. Neuron, 65(4), 550562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Smallwood, J., & Spreng, R. N. (2014). The default network and self-generated thought: Component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1316(1), 2952.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andriopoulos, C., & Lewis, M. W. (2009). Exploitation-exploration tensions and organizational ambidexterity: Managing paradoxes of innovation. Organization Science, 20(4), 696717. https://doi.org/10.1287/ORSC.1080.0406CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ansburg, P. I., & Hill, K. (2003). Creative and analytic thinkers differ in their use of attentional resources. Personality and Individual Differences, 34(7), 11411152. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00104-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anselme, P. (2010). The uncertainty processing theory of motivation. Behavioural Brain Research, 208(2), 291310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Audia, P. G., & Goncalo, J. A. (2007). Past success and creativity over time: A study of inventors in the hard disk drive industry. Management Science, 53(1), 115. https://doi.org/10.1287/MNSC.1060.0593CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baas, M., Koch, S., Nijstad, B. A., & de Dreu, C. K. W. (2015). Conceiving creativity: The nature and consequences of laypeople's beliefs about the realization of creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 9(3), 340354. https://doi.org/10.1037/A0039420CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baas, M., Nijstad, B. A., Koen, J., Boot, N. C., & de Dreu, C. K. W. (2020). Vulnerability to psychopathology and creativity: The role of approach-avoidance motivation and novelty seeking. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 14(3), 334352. https://doi.org/10.1037/ACA0000223CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baird, B., Smallwood, J., Mrazek, M. D., Kam, J. W. Y., Franklin, M. S., & Schooler, J. W. (2012). Inspired by distraction: Mind wandering facilitates creative incubation. Psychological Science, 23(10), 11171122. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612446024CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bar, M., Aminoff, E., Mason, M., & Fenske, M. (2007). The units of thought. Hippocampus, 17(6), 420428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baranes, A., Oudeyer, P. Y., & Gottlieb, J. (2015). Eye movements reveal epistemic curiosity in human observers. Vision Research, 117, 8190. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.VISRES.2015.10.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barford, K. A., Fayn, K., Silvia, P. J., & Smillie, L. D. (2018). Individual differences in conflicting stimulus evaluations: Openness/intellect predicts mixed-valenced appraisals of visual art. Journal of Research in Personality, 73, 4655.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baronchelli, A., Ferrer-i-Cancho, R., Pastor-Satorras, R., Chater, N., & Christiansen, M. H. (2013). Networks in cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(7), 348360. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.TICS.2013.04.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baror, S., Aminoff, E. M., & Bar, M. (2021). Proactive by default. In Gilead, M., & Ochsner, K. N. (Eds.), The neural basis of mentalizing (pp. 467486). Springer International Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baror, S., & Bar, M. (2016). Associative activation and its relation to exploration and exploitation in the brain. Psychological Science, 27(6), 776789. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616634487CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barr, N., Beaty, R., & Seli, P. (2020). Autonomy and control across cognition: Insights from creativity, memory, mind wandering, and reasoning research. In Preiss, D. D., Cosmelli, D., & Kaufman, J. C. (Eds.), Creativity and the wandering mind: Spontaneous and controlled cognition (pp. 2554). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-816400-6.00002-XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Basadur, M. (1995). Optimal ideation–evaluation ratios. Creativity Research Journal, 8(1), 6375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Batey, M., & Furnham, A. (2006). Creativity, intelligence, and personality: A critical review of the scattered literature. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 132(4), 355429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Barry Kaufman, S., & Silvia, P. J. (2015). Default and executive network coupling supports creative idea production. Scientific Reports, 5(1), 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Silvia, P. J., & Schacter, D. L. (2016a). Creative cognition and brain network dynamics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(2), 8795. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.TICS.2015.10.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Wilkins, R. W., Jauk, E., Fink, A., Silvia, P. J., … Neubauer, A. C. (2014). Creativity and the default network: A functional connectivity analysis of the creative brain at rest. Neuropsychologia, 64, 9298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beaty, R. E., Christensen, A. P., Benedek, M., Silvia, P. J., & Schacter, D. L. (2017). Creative constraints: Brain activity and network dynamics underlying semantic interference during idea production. NeuroImage, 148, 189196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beaty, R. E., Kaufman, S. B., Benedek, M., Jung, R. E., Kenett, Y. N., Jauk, E., … Silvia, P. J. (2016b). Personality and complex brain networks: The role of openness to experience in default network efficiency. Human Brain Mapping, 37(2), 773779.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beaty, R. E., & Kenett, Y. N. (2020). Mapping the creative mind. American Scientist, 108(4), 218224. https://www.proquest.com/scholarly-journals/mapping-creative-mind/docview/2432563296/se-2?accountid=14483CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beaty, R. E., Seli, P., & Schacter, D. L. (2019). Network neuroscience of creative cognition: Mapping cognitive mechanisms and individual differences in the creative brain. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 27, 2230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beaty, R. E., & Silvia, P. J. (2012). Why do ideas get more creative across time? An executive interpretation of the serial order effect in divergent thinking tasks. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6(4), 309319. https://doi.org/10.1037/A0029171CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bendetowicz, D., Urbanski, M., Aichelburg, C., Levy, R., & Volle, E. (2017). Brain morphometry predicts individual creative potential and the ability to combine remote ideas. Cortex, 86, 216229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benedek, M. (2018). Internally directed attention in creative cognition. In Jung, R. & Vartanian, O. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of the neuroscience of creativity (pp. 180194). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316556238.011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedek, M., & Fink, A. (2019). Toward a neurocognitive framework of creative cognition: The role of memory, attention, and cognitive control. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 27, 116122. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.COBEHA.2018.11.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedek, M., Franz, F., Heene, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2012a). Differential effects of cognitive inhibition and intelligence on creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 53(4), 480485. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PAID.2012.04.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benedek, M., & Jauk, E. (2018). 22 spontaneous and controlled processes. In Fox, K. C. R. & Christoff, K. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of spontaneous thought: Mind-wandering, creativity, and Dreaming (pp. 285298). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Benedek, M., Jauk, E., Fink, A., Koschutnig, K., Reishofer, G., Ebner, F., & Neubauer, A. C. (2014a). To create or to recall? Neural mechanisms underlying the generation of creative new ideas. NeuroImage, 88, 125133. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2013.11.021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benedek, M., Jauk, E., Sommer, M., Arendasy, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2014b). Intelligence, creativity, and cognitive control: The common and differential involvement of executive functions in intelligence and creativity. Intelligence, 46(1), 7383. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.INTELL.2014.05.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benedek, M., Kenett, Y. N., Umdasch, K., Anaki, D., Faust, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2017). How semantic memory structure and intelligence contribute to creative thought: A network science approach. Thinking & Reasoning, 23(2), 158183. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2016.1278034CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedek, M., Könen, T., & Neubauer, A. C. (2012b). Associative abilities underlying creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6(3), 273281. https://doi.org/10.1037/A0027059CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedek, M., Nordtvedt, N., Jauk, E., Koschmieder, C., Pretsch, J., Krammer, G., & Neubauer, A. C. (2016). Assessment of creativity evaluation skills: A psychometric investigation in prospective teachers. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 21, 7584. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.TSC.2016.05.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedek, M., Schües, T., Beaty, R. E., Jauk, E., Koschutnig, K., Fink, A., & Neubauer, A. C. (2018). To create or to recall original ideas: Brain processes associated with the imagination of novel object uses. Cortex, 99, 93102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berg, M. (1991). Commerce and creativity in eighteenth-century Birmingham. In Berg, M. (Ed.), Markets and manufacture in early industrial Europe (pp. 173201). Routledge.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. McGraw Hill. https://doi.org/10.1037/11164-000CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1966). Curiosity and exploration. Science, 153(3731), 2533. http://www.jstor.org/stable/1719694CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berlyne, D. E., & Slater, J. (1957). Perceptual curiosity, exploratory behavior, and maze learning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 50(3), 228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biederman, I., & Vessel, E. A. (2006). Perceptual pleasure and the brain. American Scientist, 94, 247253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Graves, W. W., & Conant, L. L. (2009). Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, 19(12), 27672796.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanchard, T. C., Hayden, B. Y., & Bromberg-Martin, E. S. (2015). Orbitofrontal cortex uses distinct codes for different choice attributes in decisions motivated by curiosity. Neuron, 85(3), 602614. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEURON.2014.12.050CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blikstein, P., Silveira Gomes, J., Teruo Akiba, H., & Schneider, B. (2017). The effect of highly scaffolded versus general instruction on students’ exploratory behavior and arousal. Technology, Knowledge and Learning, 22, 105128. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10758-016-9291-yCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boden, M. A. (2004). The creative mind: Myths and mechanisms. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boot, N., Nevicka, B., & Baas, M. (2017). Subclinical symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) are associated with specific creative processes. Personality and Individual Differences, 114, 7381. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PAID.2017.03.050CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botvinick, M. M., Carter, C. S., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624652. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broday-Dvir, R., & Malach, R. (2021). Resting-state fluctuations underlie free and creative verbal behaviors in the human brain. Cerebral Cortex, 31(1), 213232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bromberg-Martin, E. S., & Hikosaka, O. (2009). Midbrain dopamine neurons signal preference for advance information about upcoming rewards. Neuron, 63(1), 119126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bromberg-Martin, E. S., & Hikosaka, O. (2011). Lateral habenula neurons signal errors in the prediction of reward information. Nature Neuroscience, 14(9), 12091216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brydevall, M., Bennett, D., Murawski, C., & Bode, S. (2018). The neural encoding of information prediction errors during non-instrumental information seeking. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain's default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124(1), 138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cabeza, R., Ciaramelli, E., Olson, I. R., & Moscovitch, M. (2008). The parietal cortex and episodic memory: An attentional account. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(8), 613625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind variation and selective retentions in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychological Review, 67(6), 380400. https://doi.org/10.1037/H0040373CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carroll, E. A. (2011). Convergence of self-report and physiological responses for evaluating creativity support tools. C and C 2011 – Proceedings of the 8th ACM Conference on Creativity and Cognition (pp. 455456). https://doi.org/10.1145/2069618.2069748CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carson, S. H. (2011). Creativity and psychopathology: A shared vulnerability model. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 56(3), 144153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carson, S. H., Higgins, D. M., & Peterson, J. B. (2003). Decreased latent inhibition is associated with increased creative achievement in high-functioning individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(3), 499. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.499CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cavanagh, J. F., & Frank, M. J. (2014). Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(8), 414421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cervera, R. L., Wang, M. Z., & Hayden, B. Y. (2020). Systems neuroscience of curiosity. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 35, 4855. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.COBEHA.2020.06.011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charpentier, C. J., & O'Doherty, J. P. (2018). The application of computational models to social neuroscience: Promises and pitfalls. Social Neuroscience, 13(6), 637647.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, Q., Beaty, R. E., & Qiu, J. (2020). Mapping the artistic brain: Common and distinct neural activations associated with musical, drawing, and literary creativity. Human Brain Mapping, 41(12), 34033419.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chermahini, S. A., & Hommel, B. (2012). Creative mood swings: Divergent and convergent thinking affect mood in opposite ways. Psychological Research, 76(5), 634640. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00426-011-0358-Z/FIGURES/1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christensen, A. P., Kenett, Y. N., Cotter, K. N., Beaty, R. E., & Silvia, P. J. (2018). Remotely close associations: Openness to experience and semantic memory structure. European Journal of Personality, 32(4), 480492. https://doi.org/10.1002/PER.2157CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christoff, K. (2012). Undirected thought: Neural determinants and correlates. Brain Research, 1428, 5159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christoff, K., Gordon, A. M., Smallwood, J., Smith, R., & Schooler, J. W. (2009). Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(21), 87198724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christoff, K., Irving, Z. C., Fox, K. C. R., Spreng, R. N., & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2016). Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: A dynamic framework. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(11), 718731.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chrysikou, E. G. (2018). The costs and benefits of cognitive control for creativity. In Jung, R. E. & Vartanian, O. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of the neuroscience of creativity (pp. 299317). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316556238.018CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chrysikou, E. G. (2019). Creativity in and out of (cognitive) control. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 27, 9499. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.COBEHA.2018.09.014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chrysikou, E. G., Weber, M. J., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2014). A matched filter hypothesis for cognitive control. Neuropsychologia, 62, 341355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chun, M. M., Golomb, J. D., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2010). A taxonomy of external and internal attention. Psychology, 62, 73101. https://doi.org/10.1146/ANNUREV.PSYCH.093008.100427CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ciaramelli, E., Grady, C. L., & Moscovitch, M. (2008). Top-down and bottom-up attention to memory: A hypothesis (AtoM) on the role of the posterior parietal cortex in memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia, 46(7), 18281851.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82(6), 407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cropley, A. (2006). In praise of convergent thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 18(3), 391404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, R. T., Settlage, P. H., & Harlow, H. F. (2012). Performance of normal and brain-operated monkeys on mechanical puzzles with and without food incentive. The Journal of Genetic Biology, 77(2), 305311. https://doi.org/10.1080/08856559.1950.10533556Google Scholar
Day, H. I. R., & Langevin, M. A. (1969). Curiosity and intelligence: Two necessary conditions for a high level of creativity. Journal of Special Education, 3, 263268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deci, E. L. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18(1), 105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Dreu, C. K. W., Nijstad, B. A., & Baas, M. (2010). Behavioral activation links to creativity because of increased cognitive flexibility. Social Psychology and Personality Science, 2(1), 7280. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550610381789CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Dreu, C. K. W., Nijstad, B. A., Baas, M., Wolsink, I., & Roskes, M. (2012). Working memory benefits creative insight, musical improvisation, and original ideation through maintained task-focused attention. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(5), 656669. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167211435795CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denneson, L. M., Smolenski, D. J., Bush, N. E., & Dobscha, S. K. (2017). Curiosity improves coping efficacy and reduces suicidal ideation severity among military veterans at risk for suicide. Psychiatry Research, 249, 125131. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PSYCHRES.2017.01.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derryberry, D., & Tucker, D. M. (1994). Motivating the focus of attention. In Neidenthal, P. M. & Kitayama, S. (Eds.), The heart's eye: Emotional influences in perception and attention (pp. 167196). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-410560-7.50014-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dewett, T. (2007). Linking intrinsic motivation, risk taking, and employee creativity in an R&D environment. R&D Management, 37(3), 197208. https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1467-9310.2007.00469.XGoogle Scholar
DeYoung, C. G. (2015). Openness/intellect: A dimension of personality reflecting cognitive exploration. In Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., Cooper, M. L., & Larsen, R. J. (Eds.), APA handbook of personality and social psychology, Vol. 4. Personality processes and individual differences (pp. 369399). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/14343-017Google Scholar
Digman, J. M. (1997). Higher-order factors of the big five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(6), 12461256. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.73.6.1246CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duszkiewicz, A. J., McNamara, C. G., Takeuchi, T., & Genzel, L. (2019). Novelty and dopaminergic modulation of memory persistence: A tale of two systems. Trends in Neurosciences, 42(2), 102114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberger, R., Pierce, W. D., & Cameron, J. (1999). Effects of reward on intrinsic motivation – Negative, neutral, and positive: Comment on Deci, Koestner, and Ryan (1999). Psychological Bulletin, 125(6), 677691. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.125.6.677CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberger, R., & Rhoades, L. (2001). Incremental effects of reward on creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(4), 728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenman, R. (2013). Creativity, birth order, and risk taking. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 25(2), 8788. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03330292CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellamil, M., Dobson, C., Beeman, M., & Christoff, K. (2012). Evaluative and generative modes of thought during the creative process. NeuroImage, 59(2), 17831794.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erez, M., & Nouri, R. (2010). Creativity: The influence of cultural, social, and work contexts. Management and Organization Review, 6(3), 351370. https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1740-8784.2010.00191.XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T. (2007). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59(1), 255278. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093629CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fan, J. (2014). An information theory account of cognitive control. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 680.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faust-Socher, A., Kenett, Y. N., Cohen, O. S., Hassin-Baer, S., & Inzelberg, R. (2014). Enhanced creative thinking under dopaminergic therapy in Parkinson disease. Annals of Neurology, 75(6), 935942.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2(4), 290309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flaherty, A. W. (2005). Frontotemporal and dopaminergic control of idea generation and creative drive. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 493(1), 147153. https://doi.org/10.1002/CNE.20768CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foley, N. C., Jangraw, D. C., Peck, C., & Gottlieb, J. (2014). Novelty enhances visual salience independently of reward in the parietal lobe. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(23), 79477957.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foley, N. C., Kelly, S. P., Mhatre, H., Lopes, M., & Gottlieb, J. (2017). Parietal neurons encode expected gains in instrumental information. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(16), E3315E3323. https://doi.org/10.1073/PNAS.1613844114/SUPPL_FILE/PNAS.201613844SI.PDFGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ford, J. K., Weissbein, D. A., Smith, E. M., Gully, S. M., & Salas, E. (1998). Relationships of goal orientation, metacognitive activity, and practice strategies with learning outcomes and transfer. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83(2), 218. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.83.2.218CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, K. C. R., & Christoff, K. (2014). Metacognitive facilitation of spontaneous thought processes: When metacognition helps the wandering mind find its way. In Fleming, S. & Frith, C. (Eds.), The cognitive neuroscience of metacognition (pp. 293319). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedman, R. S., & Förster, J. (2010). Implicit affective cues and attentional tuning: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 136(5), 875893. https://doi.org/10.1037/A0020495CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garcia-Ruiz, P. J., Castrillo, J. C. M., & Desojo, L. V. (2019). Creativity related to dopaminergic treatment: A multicenter study. Parkinsonism & Related Disorders, 63, 169173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gasper, K., & Middlewood, B. L. (2014). Approaching novel thoughts: Understanding why elation and boredom promote associative thought more than distress and relaxation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 52, 5057. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JESP.2013.12.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, T. E., Keefe, R. S. E., Goldman, R. S., Robinson, D. G., & Harvey, P. D. (2010). Circumstances under which practice does not make perfect: A review of the practice effect literature in schizophrenia and its relevance to clinical treatment studies. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(5), 10531062.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gonen-Yaacovi, G., de Souza, L. C., Levy, R., Urbanski, M., Josse, G., & Volle, E. (2013). Rostral and caudal prefrontal contribution to creativity: A meta-analysis of functional imaging data. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodwin, F. K., & Jamison, K. R. (2007). Manic-depressive illness: Bipolar disorders and recurrent depression (Vol. 2). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gottlieb, J., Cohanpour, M., Li, Y., Singletary, N., & Zabeh, E. (2020). Curiosity, information demand and attentional priority. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 35, 8391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottlieb, J., Lopes, M., & Oudeyer, P.-Y. (2016). Motivated cognition: Neural and computational mechanisms of curiosity, attention, and intrinsic motivation. In Recent developments in neuroscience research on human motivation. Emerald.Google Scholar
Gottlieb, J., Oudeyer, P. Y., Lopes, M., & Baranes, A. (2013). Information-seeking, curiosity, and attention: Computational and neural mechanisms. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(11), 585593. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.TICS.2013.09.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goulden, N., Khusnulina, A., Davis, N. J., Bracewell, R. M., Bokde, A. L., McNulty, J. P., & Mullins, P. G. (2014). The salience network is responsible for switching between the default mode network and the central executive network: Replication from DCM. NeuroImage, 99, 180190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grant, A. M., & Berry, J. W. (2011). The necessity of others is the mother of invention: Intrinsic and prosocial motivations, perspective taking, and creativity. Academy of Management Journal, 54(1), 7396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, K., Anderson, S., Chen, E. E., Kelly, J. M., Christian, M. S., Patrick, J., … Lewis, K. (2019). “Forward flow”: A new measure to quantify free thought and predict creativity. American Psychologist, 74(5), 539554. https://doi.org/10.1037/AMP0000391CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, M. E., Araujo, D. B., Zedelius, C. M., & Schooler, J. W. (2019). Is perception the missing link between creativity, curiosity and schizotypy? Evidence from spontaneous eye-movements and responses to auditory oddball stimuli. NeuroImage, 202, 116125. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2019.116125CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, M. E., Zedelius, C. M., & Schooler, J. W. (2020). Cultivating an understanding of curiosity as a seed for creativity. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 35, 7782.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gruber, M. J., Gelman, B. D., & Ranganath, C. (2014). States of curiosity modulate hippocampus-dependent learning via the dopaminergic circuit. Neuron, 84(2), 486496. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEURON.2014.08.060CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gruzelier, J. H. (2014). EEG-neurofeedback for optimising performance. II: Creativity, the performing arts and ecological validity. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 44, 142158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guilford, J. P. (1956). The structure of intellect. Psychological Bulletin, 53(4), 267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Gusnard, D. A., & Raichle, M. E. (2001). Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(10), 685694.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hagtvedt, L. P., Dossinger, K., Harrison, S. H., & Huang, L. (2019). Curiosity made the cat more creative: Specific curiosity as a driver of creativity. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 150, 113. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.OBHDP.2018.10.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harada, T. (2020). The effects of risk-taking, exploitation, and exploration on creativity. PLoS ONE, 15(7), e0235698. https://doi.org/10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0235698CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardy, J. H., Ness, A. M., & Mecca, J. (2017). Outside the box: Epistemic curiosity as a predictor of creative problem solving and creative performance. Personality and Individual Differences, 104, 230237. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PAID.2016.08.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harlow, H. F., Harlow, M. K., & Meyer, D. R. (1950). Learning motivated by a manipulation drive. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40(2), 228234. https://doi.org/10.1037/H0056906CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herz, N., Baror, S., & Bar, M. (2020). Overarching states of mind. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(3), 184199. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.TICS.2019.12.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hills, T. T., Todd, P. M., Lazer, D., Redish, A. D., Couzin, I. D., Bateson, M., … Wolfe, J. W. (2015). Exploration versus exploitation in space, mind, and society. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(1), 4654. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.TICS.2014.10.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hommel, B. (2015). Between persistence and flexibility: The Yin and Yang of action control. Advances in Motivation Science, 2, 3367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hsee, C. K., & Ruan, B. (2016). The Pandora effect: The power and peril of curiosity. Psychological Science, 27(5), 659666. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616631733CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, F., Tang, S., Sun, P., & Luo, J. (2018). Neural correlates of novelty and appropriateness processing in externally induced constraint relaxation. NeuroImage, 172, 381389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ivancovsky, T., Kleinmintz, O., Lee, J., Kurman, J., & Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2018). The neural underpinnings of cross-cultural differences in creativity. Human Brain Mapping, 39(11), 44934508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jauk, E. (2019). A bio-psycho-behavioral model of creativity. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 27, 16. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.COBEHA.2018.08.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jepma, M., Verdonschot, R. G., van Steenbergen, H., Rombouts, S. A. R., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2012). Neural mechanisms underlying the induction and relief of perceptual curiosity. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 6, 5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, S. L., Murray, G., Fredrickson, B., Youngstrom, E. A., Hinshaw, S., Bass, J. M., … Salloum, I. (2012). Creativity and bipolar disorder: Touched by fire or burning with questions? Clinical Psychology Review, 32(1), 112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joy, S. (2004). Innovation motivation: The need to be different. Creativity Research Journal, 16(2–3), 313330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jung, R. E., Grazioplene, R., Caprihan, A., Chavez, R. S., & Haier, R. J. (2010). White matter integrity, creativity, and psychopathology: Disentangling constructs with diffusion tensor imaging. PLoS ONE, 5(3), e9818.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jung, R. E., Mead, B. S., Carrasco, J., & Flores, R. A. (2013). The structure of creative cognition in the human brain. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kafkas, A., & Montaldi, D. (2018). How do memory systems detect and respond to novelty?. Neuroscience Letters, 680, 6068.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. Macmillan.Google Scholar
Kang, M. J., Hsu, M., Krajbich, I. M., Loewenstein, G., McClure, S. M., Wang, J. T. Y., & Camerer, C. F. (2009). The wick in the candle of learning: Epistemic curiosity activates reward circuitry and enhances memory. Psychological Science, 20(8), 963973. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02402.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karuza, E. A., Thompson-Schill, S. L., & Bassett, D. S. (2016). Local patterns to global architectures: Influences of network topology on human learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(8), 629640. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.TICS.2016.06.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karwowski, M. (2012). Did curiosity kill the cat? Relationship between trait curiosity, creative self-efficacy and creative personal identity. Europe's Journal of Psychology, 8(4), 547558. https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v8i4.513Google Scholar
Karwowski, M., & Lebuda, I. (2016). The big five, the huge two, and creative self-beliefs: A meta-analysis. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 10(2), 214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kashdan, T. B., Farmer, A. S., Adams, L. M., Ferssizidis, P., McKnight, P. E., & Nezlek, J. B. (2013). Distinguishing healthy adults from people with social anxiety disorder: Evidence for the value of experiential avoidance and positive emotions in everyday social interactions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122(3), 645655. https://doi.org/10.1037/A0032733CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kashdan, T. B., & Silvia, P. J. (2009). Curiosity and interest: The benefits of thriving on novelty and challenge. In Lopez, C. R. & Snyder, S. J. (Eds.), Oxford handbook of positive psychology (pp. 367374). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C., & Beghetto, R. A. (2009). Beyond big and little: The four C model of creativity. Review of General Psychology, 13(1), 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenett, Y. N. (2018). Investigating creativity from a semantic network perspective. In Kapoula, Z., Volle, E., Renoult, J., & Andreatta, M. (Eds.), Exploring transdisciplinarity in art and sciences (pp. 4975). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-76054-4_3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenett, Y. N., Anaki, D., & Faust, M. (2014). Investigating the structure of semantic networks in low and high creative persons. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8(June), 407. https://doi.org/10.3389/FNHUM.2014.00407/ABSTRACTCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kidd, C., & Hayden, B. Y. (2015). The psychology and neuroscience of curiosity. Neuron, 88(3), 449460. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEURON.2015.09.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleinhans, N., Akshoomoff, N., & Delis, D. C. (2005). Executive functions in autism and Asperger's disorder: Flexibility, fluency, and inhibition. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27(3), 379401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleinmintz, O. M., Ivancovsky, T., & Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2019). The twofold model of creativity: The neural underpinnings of the generation and evaluation of creative ideas. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 27. 131138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.11.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kobayashi, K., Ravaioli, S., Baranès, A., Woodford, M., & Gottlieb, J. (2019). Diverse motives for human curiosity. Nature Human Behavior, 3(6), 587595. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0589-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kornhuber, H. H., & Deecke, L. (1965). Changes in the brain potential in voluntary movements and passive movements in man: Readiness potential and reafferent potentials. Pflugers Archiv fur die Gesamte Physiologie des Menschen und der Tiere, 284, 117. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14341490CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koutstaal, W., Kedrick, K., & Gonzalez-Brito, J. (2022). Capturing, clarifying, and consolidating the curiosity–creativity connection. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krajbich, I., Lu, D., Camerer, C., & Rangel, A. (2012). The attentional drift-diffusion model extends to simple purchasing decisions. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kreitler, S., Zigler, E., & Kreitler, H. (1984). Curiosity and demographic factors as determinants of children's probability-learning strategies. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 145(1), 6175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kucyi, A., Hove, M. J., Esterman, M., Matthew Hutchison, R., & Valera, E. M. (2017). Dynamic brain network correlates of spontaneous fluctuations in attention. Cerebral Cortex, 27(3), 18311840. https://doi.org/10.1093/CERCOR/BHW029Google ScholarPubMed
Lau, J. K. L., Ozono, H., Kuratomi, K., Komiya, A., & Murayama, K. (2020). Shared striatal activity in decisions to satisfy curiosity and hunger at the risk of electric shocks. Nature Human Behaviour 4(5), 531543. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-0848-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laureiro-Martínez, D., Brusoni, S., & Zollo, M. (2010). The neuroscientific foundations of the exploration–exploitation dilemma. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 3(2), 95. https://doi.org/10.1037/A0018495CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lauriola, M., Litman, J. A., Mussel, P., de Santis, R., Crowson, H. M., & Hoffman, R. R. (2015). Epistemic curiosity and self-regulation. Personality and Individual Differences, 83, 202207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lavie, D., Stettner, U., & Tushman, M. L. (2010). Exploration and exploitation within and across organizations. The Academy of Management Annals, 4(1), 19416067. https://doi.org/10.1080/19416521003691287CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, M. W. (2000). Exploring paradox: Toward a more comprehensive guide. The Academy of Management Review, H25(4), 760776. https://doi.org/10.5465/AMR.2000.3707712CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, P. A., Knoblich, G., & Poe, G. (2018). How memory replay in sleep boosts creative problem-solving. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(6), 491503. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.TICS.2018.03.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, W., Li, X., Huang, L., Kong, X., Yang, W., Wei, D., … Liu, J. (2015). Brain structure links trait creativity to openness to experience. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(2), 191198. https://doi.org/10.1093/SCAN/NSU041CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, Y., Huo, T., Zhuang, K., Song, L., Wang, X., Ren, Z., … Qiu, J. (2019). Functional connectivity mediates the relationship between self-efficacy and curiosity. Neuroscience Letters, 711, 134442. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuletCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, Y., Kenett, Y. N., Hu, W., & Beaty, R. E. (2021). Flexible semantic network structure supports the production of creative metaphor. Creativity Research Journal, 33(3), 209223. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2021.1879508CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ligneul, R., Mermillod, M., & Morisseau, T. (2018). From relief to surprise: Dual control of epistemic curiosity in the human brain. NeuroImage, 181, 490500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Litchfield, R. C. (2008). Brainstorming reconsidered: A goal-based view. Academy of Management Review, 33(3), 649668.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Litman, J. (2005). Curiosity and the pleasures of learning: Wanting and liking new information. Cognition & Emotion, 19(6), 793814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Litman, J. A. (2008). Interest and deprivation factors of epistemic curiosity. Personality and Individual Differences, 44(7), 15851595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Litman, J. A., & Jimerson, T. L. (2004). The measurement of curiosity as a feeling of deprivation. Journal of Personality Assessment, 82(2), 147157. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa8202_3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Litman, J. A., & Spielberger, C. D. (2003). Measuring epistemic curiosity and its diversive and specific components. Journal of Personality Assessment, 80(1), 7586. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327752JPA8001_16CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loewenstein, G. (1994). The psychology of curiosity: A review and reinterpretation. Psychological Bulletin, 116(1), 75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lu, J. G., Akinola, M., & Mason, M. F. (2017). “Switching On” creativity: Task switching can increase creativity by reducing cognitive fixation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 139, 6375. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.OBHDP.2017.01.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lucchiari, C., Sala, P. M., & Vanutelli, M. E. (2018). Promoting creativity through transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). A critical review. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 12, 167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ludwig, A. M. (1992). Creative achievement and psychopathology: Comparison among professions. Journal of Psychotherapy, 46, 330356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lynn, M., & Snyder, C. R. (2002) Uniqueness seeking. In Snyder, C. R. & Lopez, S. J. (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 395410). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Madjar, N., Greenberg, E., & Chen, Z. (2011). Factors for radical creativity, incremental creativity, and routine, noncreative performance. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 96(4), 730743. https://doi.org/10.1037/A0022416CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madore, K. P., Addis, D. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2015). Creativity and memory: Effects of an episodic-specificity induction on divergent thinking. Psychological Science, 26(9), 14611468. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615591863CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madore, K. P., Thakral, P. P., Beaty, R. E., Addis, D. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2019). Neural mechanisms of episodic retrieval support divergent creative thinking. Cerebral Cortex, 29(1), 150166. https://doi.org/10.1093/CERCOR/BHX312CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
March, J. G. (1991). Exploration and exploitation in organizational learning. Organization Science, 2(1), 7187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marron, T. R., & Faust, M. (2019). Measuring spontaneous processes in creativity research. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 27, 6470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marron, T. R., Lerner, Y., Berant, E., Kinreich, S., Shapira-Lichter, I., Hendler, T., & Faust, M. (2018). Chain free association, creativity, and the default mode network. Neuropsychologia, 118, 4058. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA.2018.03.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martindale, C. (1999). Biological bases of creativity. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 137152). Cambridge University Press. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1998-08125-007Google Scholar
Martín, C. T., Ric, Á., & Hristovski, R. (2015). Creativity and emergence of specific dance movements using instructional constraints. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 9(1), 6574. https://doi.org/10.1037/A0038706CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mason, M. F., Norton, M. I., van Horn, J. D., Wegner, D. M., Grafton, S. T., & Macrae, C. N. (2007). Wandering minds: The default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science, 315(5810), 393395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maw, W. H., & Maw, E. W. (1970). Nature of creativity in high- and low-curiosity boys. Developmental Psychology, 2(3), 325329. https://doi.org/10.1037/H0029189CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayseless, N., Aharon-Peretz, J., & Shamay-Tsoory, S. (2014). Unleashing creativity: The role of left temporoparietal regions in evaluating and inhibiting the generation of creative ideas. Neuropsychologia, 64, 157168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayseless, N., Eran, A., & Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2015). Generating original ideas: The neural underpinning of originality. NeuroImage, 116, 232239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCrae, R. R. (1996). Social consequences of experiential openness. Psychological Bulletin, 120(3), 323. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.120.3.323CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mednick, S. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69(3), 220232. https://doi.org/10.1037/H0048850CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mehlhorn, K., Newell, B. R., Todd, P. M., Lee, M. D., Morgan, K., Braithwaite, V. A., … Gonzalez, C. (2015). Unpacking the exploration–exploitation tradeoff: A synthesis of human and animal literatures. Decision, 2(3), 191215. https://doi.org/10.1037/DEC0000033CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mendelsohn, G. A., & Griswold, B. B. (1964). Differential use of incidental stimuli in problem solving as a function of creativity. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 68(4), 431436. https://doi.org/10.1037/H0040166CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menon, V. (2015). Salience network. In Toga, A. W. (Ed.), Brain mapping: An encyclopedic reference (Vol. 2, pp. 597611). Academic Press, Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menon, V., & Uddin, L. Q. (2010). Saliency, switching, attention and control: A network model of insula function. Brain Structure and Function, 214(5), 655667.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, B. L., Boone, K., Cummings, J. L., Read, S. L., & Mishkin, F. (2000). Functional correlates of musical and visual ability in frontotemporal dementia. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 176(5), 458463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, B. L., Ponton, M., Benson, D. F., Cummings, J. L., & Mena, I. (1996). Enhanced artistic creativity with temporal lobe degeneration. The Lancet, 348(9043), 17441745.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miron-Spektor, E., & Erez, M. (2017). Looking at creativity through a paradox lens. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Miron-Spektor, E., Gino, F., & Argote, L. (2011). Paradoxical frames and creative sparks: Enhancing individual creativity through conflict and integration. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 116(2), 229240. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.OBHDP.2011.03.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 814. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411429458CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mok, L. W. (2014). The interplay between spontaneous and controlled processing in creative cognition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 663.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, M. W., & Leung, K. (2010). Creativity east and west: Perspectives and parallels. Management and Organization Review, 6(3), 313327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moutard, C., Dehaene, S., & Malach, R. (2015). Spontaneous fluctuations and non-linear ignitions: Two dynamic faces of cortical recurrent loops. Neuron, 88(1), 194206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mueller, J. S., Melwani, S., & Goncalo, J. A. (2012). The bias against creativity: Why people desire but reject creative ideas. Psychological Science, 23(1), 1317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mumford, M. D., Baughman, W. A., Supinski, E. P., & Maher, M. A. (1996). Process-based measures of creative problem-solving skills: II. Information encoding. Creativity Research Journal, 9(1), 7788. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj0901_7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murayama, K. (2022). A reward-learning framework of knowledge acquisition: An integrated account of curiosity, interest, and intrinsic–extrinsic rewards. Psychological Review, 129(1), 175198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, M., Runco, M. A., Acar, S., & Reiter-Palmon, R. (2013). Reanalysis of genetic data and rethinking dopamine's relationship with creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 25(1), 147148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murty, V. P., Ballard, I. C., Macduffie, K. E., Krebs, R. M., & Adcock, R. A. (2013). Hippocampal networks habituate as novelty accumulates. Learning & Memory, 20(4), 229235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109(3), 504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noordewier, M. K., & van Dijk, E. (2016). Interest in complex novelty. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 38(2), 98110. https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2016.1153474CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norman, Y., Raccah, O., Liu, S., Parvizi, J., & Malach, R. (2021). Hippocampal ripples and their coordinated dialogue with the default mode network during recent and remote recollection. Neuron, 109(17), 27672780.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., de Greck, M., Bermpohl, F., Dobrowolny, H., & Panksepp, J. (2006). Self-referential processing in our brain – A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. NeuroImage, 31(1), 440457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noy, N., Bickel, S., Zion-Golumbic, E., Harel, M., Golan, T., Davidesco, I., … Malach, R. (2015). Ignition's glow: Ultra-fast spread of global cortical activity accompanying local “ignitions” in visual cortex during conscious visual perception. Consciousness and Cognition, 35, 206224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nusbaum, E. C., & Silvia, P. J. (2011). Are intelligence and creativity really so different?: Fluid intelligence, executive processes, and strategy use in divergent thinking. Intelligence, 39(1), 3645. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.INTELL.2010.11.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oh, Y., Chesebrough, C., Erickson, B., Zhang, F., & Kounios, J. (2020). An insight-related neural reward signal. NeuroImage, 214, 116757.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oudeyer, P. Y., Kaplan, F., & Hafner, V. V. (2007). Intrinsic motivation systems for autonomous mental development. IEEE Transactions on Evolutionary Computation, 11(2), 265286. https://doi.org/10.1109/TEVC.2006.890271CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perchtold, C. M., Papousek, I., Koschutnig, K., Rominger, C., Weber, H., Weiss, E. M., & Fink, A. (2018). Affective creativity meets classic creativity in the scanner. Human Brain Mapping, 39(1), 393406.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peterson, W., Birdsall, T., & Fox, W. (1954). The theory of signal detectability. Transactions of the IRE Professional Group on Information Theory, 4(4), 171212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Picciuto, E., & Carruthers, P. (2014). The origins of creativity. In Paul, E. & Kaufman, S. B. (Eds.), The philosophy of creativity: New essays (pp. 199223). Oxford University Press. https://www.academia.edu/5584283/The_Origins_of_CreativityCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pomarol-Clotet, E., Oh, T., Laws, K. R., & McKenna, P. J. (2008). Semantic priming in schizophrenia: Systematic review and meta-analysis. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 192(2), 9297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Post, F. (1994). Creativity and psychopathology a study of 291 world-famous men. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 165(1), 2234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Puryear, J. S., Kettler, T., & Rinn, A. N. (2017). Relationships of personality to differential conceptions of creativity: A systematic review. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 11(1), 5968. https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000079CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., & Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98(2), 676682.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ranganath, C., & Rainer, G. (2003). Neural mechanisms for detecting and remembering novel events. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(3), 193202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rawlings, D. (1985). Psychoticism, creativity and dichotic shadowing. Personality and Individual Differences, 6(6), 737742. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(85)90084-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Redgrave, P., Gurney, K., & Reynolds, J. (2008). What is reinforced by phasic dopamine signals? Brain Research Reviews, 58(2), 322339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Risko, E. F., Anderson, N. C., Lanthier, S., & Kingstone, A. (2012). Curious eyes: Individual differences in personality predict eye movement behavior in scene-viewing. Cognition, 122(1), 8690. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.COGNITION.2011.08.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rothenberg, A. (2001). Bipolar illness, creativity, and treatment. Psychiatric Quarterly, 72(2), 131147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubenstein, D. J. (2000). Stimulating children's creativity and curiosity: Does content and medium matter? The Journal of Creative Behavior, 34(1), 117. https://doi.org/10.1002/J.2162-6057.2000.TB01199.XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A. (2015). Meta-creativity: Being creative about creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 27(3), 295298. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2015.1065134CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A., & Acar, S. (2012). Divergent thinking as an indicator of creative potential. Creativity Research Journal, 24(1), 6675. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2012.652929CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A., & Basadur, M. (1993). Assessing ideational and evaluative skills and creative styles and attitudes. Creativity and Innovation Management, 2(3), 166173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruscio, A., & Amabile, T. M. (1999). Effects of instructional style on problem-solving creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 12(4), 251266. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj1204_3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: Classic definitions and new directions. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25(1), 5467.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schad, J., Lewis, M. W., Raisch, S., & Smith, W. K. (2016). Paradox research in management science: Looking back to move forward. The Academy of Management Annals, 10(1), 564. https://doi.org/10.5465/19416520.2016.1162422CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (2001). Appraisal considered as a process of multilevel sequential checking. In Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 92120). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schomaker, J., & Meeter, M. (2015). Short- and long-lasting consequences of novelty, deviance and surprise on brain and cognition. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 55, 268279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schutte, N. S., & Malouff, J. M. (2019). A meta-analysis of the relationship between curiosity and creativity. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 54(4), 940947.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schutte, N. S., & Malouff, J. M. (2020). Connections between curiosity, flow and creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 152, 109555. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PAID.2019.109555CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartenbeck, P., FitzGerald, T., Dolan, R. J., & Friston, K. (2013). Exploration, novelty, surprise, and free energy minimization. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(October), 710. https://doi.org/10.3389/FPSYG.2013.00710/BIBTEXCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seeley, W. W., Matthews, B. R., Crawford, R. K., Gorno-Tempini, M. L., Foti, D., Mackenzie, I. R., & Miller, B. L. (2008). Unravelling Boléro: Progressive aphasia, transmodal creativity and the right posterior neocortex. Brain, 131(1), 3949.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silvia, P. J. (2005). What is interesting? Exploring the appraisal structure of interest. Emotion, 5(1), 89102. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.89CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silvia, P. J. (2006). Exploring the psychology of interest. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195158557.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silvia, P. J., & Christensen, A. P. (2020). Looking up at the curious personality: Individual differences in curiosity and openness to experience. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 35, 16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silvia, P. J., & Sanders, C. E. (2010). Why are smart people curious? Fluid intelligence, openness to experience, and interest. Learning and Individual Differences, 20(3), 242245. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.LINDIF.2010.01.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2003). Scientific creativity as constrained stochastic behavior: The integration of product, person, and process perspectives. Psychological Bulletin, 129(4), 475494. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.4.475CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simpson Steele, J. (2015). Becoming creative practitioners: Elementary teachers tackle artful approaches to writing instruction. Teaching Education, 27(1), 7287. https://doi.org/10.1080/10476210.2015.1037829CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, W., & Lewis, M. (2011). Toward a theory of paradox: A dynamic equilibrium model of organizing. The Academy of Management Review, 36(2), 381403. https://doi.org/10.5465/AMR.2009.0223Google Scholar
Sowden, P. T., Pringle, A., & Gabora, L. (2015). The shifting sands of creative thinking: Connections to dual-process theory. Thinking & Reasoning, 21(1), 4060.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (1999). Who is rational? Studies of individual differences in reasoning. Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stawarczyk, D., Bezdek, M. A., & Zacks, J. M. (2021). Event representations and predictive processing: The role of the midline default network core. Topics in Cognitive Science, 13(1), 164186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steele, L. M., Hardy, J. H., Day, E. A., Watts, L. L., & Mumford, M. D. (2021). Navigating creative paradoxes: Exploration and exploitation effort drive novelty and usefulness. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 15(1), 149164. https://doi.org/10.1037/ACA0000236CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stein, M. I. (1953). Creativity and culture. The Journal of Psychology, 36(2), 311322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1992). Creativity: Its nature and assessment. School Psychology International, 13(3), 243253. https://doi.org/10.1177/0143034392133004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1996). Investing in creativity. American Psychologist, 51(7), 677.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1999). The concept of creativity: Prospects and paradigms. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 315). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sugiura, M., Shah, N. J., Zilles, K., & Fink, G. R. (2005). Cortical representations of personally familiar objects and places: Functional organization of the human posterior cingulate cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(2), 183198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Sassa, Y., Hashizume, H., Sekiguchi, A., Fukushima, A., & Kawashima, R. (2010). Regional gray matter volume of dopaminergic system associate with creativity: Evidence from voxel-based morphometry. NeuroImage, 51(2), 578585.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tanner, W. P. Jr, & Swets, J. A. (1954). A decision-making theory of visual detection. Psychological Review, 61(6), 401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tian, X., Silva, A. C., & Liu, C. (2021). The brain circuits and dynamics of curiosity-driven behavior in naturally curious marmosets. Cerebral Cortex, 31(9), 42204232. https://doi.org/10.1093/CERCOR/BHAB080CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tulving, E., & Kroll, N. (1995). Novelty assessment in the brain and long-term memory encoding. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2(3), 387390.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uddin, L. Q. (2015). Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(1), 5561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Lieshout, L. L. F., de Lange, F. P., & Cools, R. (2019). Motives underlying human curiosity. Nature Human Behaviour 3(6), 550551. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0565-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Lieshout, L. L. F., Vandenbroucke, A. R. E., Müller, N. C. J., Cools, R., & de Lange, F. P. (2018). Induction and relief of curiosity elicit parietal and frontal activity. Journal of Neuroscience, 38(10), 25792588. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2816-17.2018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vartanian, O., Martindale, C., & Kwiatkowski, J. (2007). Creative potential, attention, and speed of information processing. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(6), 14701480. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PAID.2007.04.027CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vidler, D. C., & Karan, V. E. (1975). A study of curiosity, divergent thinking, and test-anxiety. The Journal of Psychology, 90(2), 237243. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.1975.9915781CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volle, E. (2018). Associative and controlled cognition in divergent thinking: Theoretical, experimental, neuroimaging evidence, and new directions. In Jung, R. E. & Vartanian, O. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of the neuroscience of creativity (pp. 333360). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316556238.020CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, H. A., & Shah, P. (2006). Uninhibited imaginations: Creativity in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Personality and Individual Differences, 40(6), 11211131. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PAID.2005.11.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, H. A., & Shah, P. (2016). Scope of semantic activation and innovative thinking in college students with ADHD. Creativity Research Journal, 28(3), 275282. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2016.1195655CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wittmann, B. C., Bunzeck, N., Dolan, R. J., & Düzel, E. (2007). Anticipation of novelty recruits reward system and hippocampus while promoting recollection. NeuroImage, 38(1), 194202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wright, J. H., & Beck, A. T. (1983). Cognitive therapy of depression: Theory and practice. Psychiatric Services, 34(12), 11191127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, T. Q., Miller, Z. A., Adhimoolam, B., Zackey, D. D., Khan, B. K., Ketelle, R., … Miller, B. L. (2015). Verbal creativity in semantic variant primary progressive aphasia. Neurocase, 21(1), 7378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xia, C., Touroutoglou, A., Quigley, K. S., Feldman Barrett, L., & Dickerson, B. C. (2017). Salience network connectivity modulates skin conductance responses in predicting arousal experience. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 29(5), 827836.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zabelina, D., Saporta, A., & Beeman, M. (2016). Flexible or leaky attention in creative people? Distinct patterns of attention for different types of creative thinking. Memory and Cognition, 44(3), 488498. https://doi.org/10.3758/S13421-015-0569-4/TABLES/2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zabelina, D. L. (2018). Attention and creativity. In Jung, R. E. & Vartanian, O. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of the neuroscience of creativity (pp. 161179). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316556238.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zabelina, D. L., & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2016). Dynamic network interactions supporting internally-oriented cognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 40, 8693.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zabelina, D. L., & Ganis, G. (2018). Creativity and cognitive control: Behavioral and ERP evidence that divergent thinking, but not real-life creative achievement, relates to better cognitive control. Neuropsychologia, 118, 2028. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA.2018.02.014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zabelina, D. L., & Robinson, M. D. (2010). Creativity as flexible cognitive control. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4(3), 136. https://doi.org/10.1037/A0017379CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, W., Sjoerds, Z., & Hommel, B. (2020). Metacontrol of human creativity: The neurocognitive mechanisms of convergent and divergent thinking. NeuroImage, 210, 116572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed