Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-dfsvx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T03:49:05.673Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - Latent inhibition and habituation: evaluation of an associative analysis

from Current topics in latent inhibition research

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2010

Robert Lubow
Affiliation:
Tel-Aviv University
Ina Weiner
Affiliation:
Tel-Aviv University
Get access

Summary

Summary

Latent inhibition and habituation are behavioural phenomena that result from the same simple procedure: when an animal receives exposure to a stimulus, the unconditioned responses that were once provoked by that stimulus diminish or habituate, and subsequently the rate at which conditioned responding can be established to the stimulus is reduced. One parsimonious interpretation of these phenomena is that they reflect the operation of the same attentional process: a novel stimulus attracts attention, provokes responding and is rapidly learnt about; exposure to a stimulus produces a decline in attention and concomitant reductions in responding and learning. Here, we present an integrative review of recent research that provides support for a specific account of habituation and latent inhibition that is grounded in a formal analysis of the mnemonic processes involved in associative learning. This analysis also provides one interpretation of the role of the hippocampus, a structure traditionally associated with both mnemonic and attentional processes, in habituation and latent inhibition.

Introduction

Latent inhibition (LI) is a phenomenon in both senses of the word: it is a very simple and ubiquitous behavioural effect, and it has stimulated intense empirical and theoretical interest, and some controversy over a protracted period. After a faltering start (see Lubow, this volume), the observation first reported by Lubow and Moore (1959) has become a central concern for theoretical analyses of associative learning: once its generality had been established, LI became the territory of models of associative learning wherein variations in attention play a key role (e.g., Lubow, 1989; Mackintosh, 1975; Pearce & Hall, 1980).

Type
Chapter
Information
Latent Inhibition
Cognition, Neuroscience and Applications to Schizophrenia
, pp. 163 - 182
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bouton, M. E., & Swartzentruber, D. (1989). Slow reacquisition following extinction: context, encoding, and retrieval mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 15, 43–53.Google Scholar
Carr, A. F. (1974). Latent inhibition and overshadowing in conditioned emotional response conditioning with rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 86, 718–723.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dias, R., & Honey, R. C. (2002). Involvement of rat medial prefrontal cortex in novelty detection. Behavioral Neuroscience, 116, 498–503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donegan, N. H. (1981). Priming-produced facilitation or diminution of responding to a Pavlovian unconditioned stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 7, 295–312.Google ScholarPubMed
Dwyer, D. M., & Honey, R. C. (2007). The effects of habituation training on compound conditioning are not reversed by an associative activation treatment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 33, 185–190.Google Scholar
Gray, J. A. (1982). The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: An Enquiry into Functions of the Septo-hippocampal System. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Groves, P. M., & Thompson, R. F. (1970). Habituation: A dual-process theory. Psychological Review, 77, 419–450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, G. (1991). Perceptual and Associative Learning. Oxford, England: Clarendon.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, G., & Channell, S. (1985). Differential effects of contextual change on latent inhibition and on the habituation of an orienting response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 11, 470–481.Google Scholar
Hall, G., & Honey, R. C. (1989). Contextual effects in conditioning, latent inhibition and habituation: Associative and retrieval functions of contextual cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 15, 232–241.Google Scholar
Hall, G., & Schachtman, T. R. (1987). Differential effects of a retention interval on latent inhibition and habituation of an orienting response. Animal Learning and Behavior, 15, 76–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Han, J. -S., Gallagher, M., & Holland, P. C. (1995). Hippocampal lesions disrupt decrements but not increments in conditioned stimulus processing. Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 7323–7329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, R. D., & Kandel, E. R. (1984). Is there a cell biological alphabet for simple forms of learning?Psychological Review, 91, 375–391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holt, W., & Maren, S. (1999). Muscimol inactivation of the dorsal hippocampus impairs contextual retrieval of fear memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 9054–9062.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Honey, R. C., & Good, M. (1993). Selective hippocampal lesions abolish the contextual specificity of latent inhibition and conditioning. Behavioral Neuroscience, 107, 23–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Honey, R. C., & Good, M. (2000a). Associative modulation of the orienting response: Distinct effects revealed by hippocampal lesions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 26, 3–14.Google ScholarPubMed
Honey, R. C., & Good, M. (2000b). Associative components of recognition memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 10, 200–204CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Honey, R. C., Good, M., & Manser, K. L. (1998). Negative priming in associative learning: Evidence from a serial-habituation procedure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 24, 229–237.Google Scholar
Honey, R. C., Hall, G., & Bonardi, C. (1993). Negative priming in associative learning: Evidence from a serial-conditioning procedure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 19, 90–97.Google ScholarPubMed
Honey, R. C., Marshall, V. J., McGregor, A., Futter, J., & Good, M. (2007). Revisiting places passed: Sensitization of exploratory activity in rats with hippocampal lesions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 625–634.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Honey, R. C., Watt, A., & Good, M. (1998). Hippocampal lesions disrupt an associative mismatch process. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 2226–2230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horn, G., & Hill, R. M. (1964). Habituation of the response to sensory stimuli of neurones in the brain stem of rabbits. Nature, 202, 296–298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jordan, W. P., Strasser, H. C., & McHale, L. (2000). Contextual control of long-term habituation in rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 26, 323–339.Google ScholarPubMed
Kasprow, W. J., Catterson, D., Schachtman, T. R., & Miller, R. R. (1984). Attenuation of latent inhibition by postacquisition reminder. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36B, 53–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaye, H., & Mackintosh, N. J. (1990). A change of context can enhance performance of an aversive but not of an appetitive conditioned response. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 42B, 113–134.Google Scholar
Kaye, H., & Pearce, J. M. (1984). The strength of the orienting response during Pavlovian conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 10, 90–109.Google ScholarPubMed
Konorski, J. (1967). Integrative Activity of the Brain: An Interdisciplinary Approach. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kumaran, D., & Maguire, E. A. (2006). An unexpected sequence of events: Mismatch detection in the human hippocampus. PLoS Biology, 4, e424.Google Scholar
Lovibond, P. E., Preston, G. C., & Mackintosh, N. J. (1984). Context specificity of conditioning and latent inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 10, 360–375.Google Scholar
Lubow, R. E. (1973). Latent inhibition. Psychological Bulletin, 79, 398–407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lubow, R. E. (1989). Latent Inhibition and Conditioned Attention Theory. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lubow, R. E., & Moore, A. U. (1959). Latent inhibition: The effect of non-reinforced preexposure to the conditioned stimulus. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 52, 415–419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lubow, R. E., Rifkin, B., & Alek, M. (1976). The context effect: The relationship between stimulus preexposure and environmental preexposure determines subsequent learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2, 38–47.Google Scholar
Mackintosh, N. J. (1975). A theory of attention: Variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychological Review, 82, 276–298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mackintosh, N. J. (1987). Neurobiology, psychology, and habituation. Behaviour, Research and Therapy, 25, 81–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marlin, N. A., & Miller, R. R. (1981). Associations to contextual stimuli as a determinant of long-term habituation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 7, 313–333.Google ScholarPubMed
Marshall, V. J., McGregor, A., Good, M., & Honey, R. C. (2004). Hippocampal lesions modulate both associative and nonassociative priming. Behavioral Neuroscience, 118, 377–382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaren, I. P. L., Bennett, C., Plaisted, K., Aitken, M., & Mackintosh, N. J. (1994). Latent inhibition, context specificity, and context familiarity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 387–400.Google ScholarPubMed
McLaren, I. P. L., & Mackintosh, N. J. (2000). An elemental model of associative learning: I. Latent inhibition and perceptual learning. Animal Learning and Behavior, 28, 211–246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olton, D. S., Becker, J. T., & Handelmann, G. E. (1979). Hippocampus, space and memory. Behavioural Brain Sciences, 2, 313–365CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oswald, C. J. P., Yee, B. K., Bannerman, D. B., et al. (2002). The influence of selective lesions to components of the hippocampal system on the orienting response, habituation and latent inhibition. European Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 1983–1990.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned Reflexes. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pearce, J. M., & Hall, G. (1980). A model of Pavlovian learning: Variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not unconditioned stimuli. Psychological Review, 87, 532–552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rawlins, J. N. P. (1985). Associations across time: The hippocampus as a temporary memory store. Behavioural Brain Sciences, 8, 479–496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In Black, A. H. & Prokasy, W. F. (Eds.), Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, pp. 64–99.Google Scholar
Sokolov, E. N. (1963). Perception and the Conditioned Reflex. London: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Solomon, R. L., & Corbit, J. D. (1974). An opponent-process theory of motivation: I. Temporal dynamics of affect. Psychological Review, 81, 119–145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swan, J. A., & Pearce, J. M. (1988). The orienting response as an index of stimulus associability in rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 14, 292–301.Google ScholarPubMed
Vinogradova, O. S. (1975). Registration of information and the limbic system. In Horn, G. & Hinde, R. A. (Eds.), Short-term Changes in Neural Activity and Behavior. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 95–148.Google Scholar
Wagner, A. R. (1981). SOP: A model of automatic memory processing in animal behavior. In Spear, N. E. & Miller, R. R. (Eds.), Information Processing in Animals: Memory Mechanisms. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 5–48.Google Scholar
Wagner, A. R., & Brandon, S. E. (1989). Evolution of a structured connectionist model of Pavlovian conditioning. In Klein, S. B. & Mowrer, R. R. (Eds.), Contemporary Learning Theories: Pavlovian Conditioning and the Status of Traditional Learning Theory. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 149–190.Google Scholar
Wagner, A. R., & Brandon, S. E. (2001). A componential theory of Pavlovian conditioning. In Mowrer, R. R. & Klein, S. (Eds.), Contemporary Learning: Theory and Application. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 23–64.Google Scholar
Weiner, I. (1990). Neural substrates of latent inhibition: the switching model. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 442–461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiner, I., & Feldon, J. (1997). The switching model of latent inhibition: an update of neural substrates. Behavioral Brain Research, 88, 11–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wheeler, D. S., & Miller, R. R. (2007). Constrasting reduced overshadowing and blocking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 33, 349–359.Google Scholar
Wilson, P. N., Boumphrey, P., & Pearce, J. M. (1992). Restoration of the orienting response to a light by a change in its predictive accuracy. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44B, 17–36.Google Scholar
Wilson, P. N., & Pearce, J. M. (1988). Transfer of responding during serial conditioning in the rat. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40B, 259–278.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×