Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-767nl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-12T11:36:46.775Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

1 - A short history of latent inhibition research

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2010

Robert Lubow
Affiliation:
Tel-Aviv University
Ina Weiner
Affiliation:
Tel-Aviv University
Get access

Summary

The first latent inhibition (LI) paper was published 50 years ago (Lubow & Moore,1959), and the present book marks that anniversary. As such, it offers a convenient time for providing a historical perspective to a phenomenon that was born by accident, barely survived the first several post-parturitional years, and yet developed into a flourishing research enterprise, with activities cutting across such diverse fields as learning theory, schizophrenia, and even creativity. Indeed, in the weekly episodes of “Prison Break” on American TV, the concept of LI has even reached prime-time television.

In spite of the relatively widespread use of the LI paradigm in the laboratory, and, in particular, because of its adoption in research areas that are far removed from its origins, the present editors felt that there was a need to acquaint the larger audience with both the history and recent advances in LI research and theory.

Before describing the serendipitous discovery of LI, this apparently simple, yet ubiquitous, phenomenon requires a definition and a description of its adaptive function. Specifically, LI is a name for the decrease in the ability to acquire or express a new association to a stimulus that has previously received passive, non-reinforced preexposures, as compared to a stimulus that is either novel (not preexposed) or one that has been reinforced or attended. Importantly, LI is not a process. It is an effect that, as will be seen, is in search of a process that generates it.

Type
Chapter
Information
Latent Inhibition
Cognition, Neuroscience and Applications to Schizophrenia
, pp. 1 - 20
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ackil, J. E., Mellgren, R. L., Halgren, C., & Frommer, G. P. (1969). Effects of CS preexposure on avoidance learning in rats with hippocampal lesions. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 69, 739–747.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baruch, I., Hemsley, D. R., & Gray, J. A. (1988a). Differential performance of acute and chronic schizophrenics in a latent inhibition task. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 176, 698–600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baruch, I., Hemsley, D. R., & Gray, J. A. (1988b). Latent inhibition and “psychotic proneness” in normal subjects. Personality and Individual Differences, 9, 777–783.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Binder, E. B., Gross, R. E., Nemeroff, C. B., & Kilts, C. D. (2002). Effects of neurotensin receptor antagonism on latent inhibition in Sprague-Dawley rats. Psychopharmacology, 161, 288–295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boulenguez, P., Peters, S. L., Mitchell, S. N., Chauveau, J., Gray, , , J. A., & Joseph, M. J. (1998). Dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens and latent inhibition in the rat following microinjections of a 5-HT1B agonist into the dorsal subiculum: implications for schizophrenia. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 12, 258–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bouton, M. E. (1993). Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian conditioning. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 80–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruno, , K. J., Freet, C. S., Twining, R. C., et al. (2007). Abnormal latent inhibition and impulsivity in coloboma mice, a model of ADHD. Neurobiology of Disease, 25, 206–216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burton, H. A., & Toga, A. W. (1982). Successive discrimination performance improves with increasing numbers of stimulus preexposures in septal rats. Behavioral and Neural Biology, 34, 141–151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caldarone, B. J., Duman, C. H., & Picciotto, M. R. (2000). Fear conditioning and latent inhibition in mice lacking the high affinity subclass of nicotinic acetylcholine receptors in the brain. Neuropharmacology, 39, 2779–2784.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chacto, C., & Lubow, R. E. (1967). Classical conditioning and latent inhibition in the white rat. Psychonomic Science, 9, 135–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chandra, S. B. C., Hosler, J. S., & Smith, B. H. (2000). Heritable variation for latent inhibition and its correlation with reversal learning in honeybees (Apis mellifera). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 114, 73–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clapcote, S. J., Lipina, T. V., Millar, K. J., et al. (2007). Behavioral phenotypes of Disc1 missense mutations in mice. Neuron, 54, 387–402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, E., Sereni, N., Kaplan, O., et al. (2004). The relation between latent inhibition and symptom-types in young schizophrenics. Behavioural Brain Research, 149, 113–122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conti, L. H., Palmer, A. A., Vanella, J. J., & Printz, M. P. (2001). Latent inhibition and conditioning in rat strains which show differential prepulse inhibition. Behavior Genetics, 31, 325–333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casa, G., & Lubow, R. E. (2001). Latent inhibition with a response time measure from a within-subject design: effects of number of preexposures, masking task, context change, and delay. Neuropsychology, 15, 244–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denniston, J. C., Savastano, H. I., & Miller, R. R. (2001). The extended comparator hypothesis: learning by contiguity, responding by relative strength. In Mowrer, R. R. & Klein, S. B. (Eds.), Handbook of Contemporary Learning Theories. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 65–117.Google Scholar
Ely, T. D., Gross, R. F., Lambert, P. D., & Kilts, C. D. (1997). The neuroanatomical substrates of latent inhibition: a behavioral genetic quantitative cytochrome oxidase histochemical study. Society for Neuroscience, 23, 1840.Google Scholar
Escobar, M., Oberling, P., & Miller, R. R. (2002). Associative deficit accounts of disrupted latent inhibition and blocking in schizophrenia. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 203–216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, L., Gray, N. S., & Snowden, R. J. (2007). A new continuous within-participant latent inhibition task: examining associations with schizotypy dimensions, smoking and gender. Biological Psychology, 74, 365–373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Filsinger, I., Zimmermann, M., Kirsch, P., et al. (2004). Latent inhibition in schizophrenic patients and healthy controls: an event-related fMRI study. Journal of Psychophysiology, 18, 241 (abstract).Google Scholar
Gaisler-Salomon, , I., Diamant, L., Rubin, C., & Weiner, I. (2008). Abnormally persistent latent inhibition induced by MK801 is reversed by risperidone and by positive modulators of NMDA: differential efficacy depending on the stage of the task at which they are administered. Psychopharmacology, 196, 255–267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaisler-Salomon, , I., & Weiner, I. (2003). Systemic administration of MK-801 produces an abnormally persistent latent inhibition which is reversed by clozapine but not haloperidol. Psychopharmacology, 166, 333–342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gal, G., Schiller, D., & Weiner, I. (2005). Latent inhibition is disrupted by nucleus accumbens shell lesion but is abnormally persistent following entire nucleus accumbens lesion: the neural site controlling the expression and disruption of the stimulus preexposure effect. Behavioural Brain Research, 162, 246–255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerdjikov, T. V., Rudolph, U., Keist, R., et al. (2008). Hippocampal alpha 5 subunit-containing GABAA receptors are involved in the development of the latent inhibition effect. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 89, 87–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gould, T. J., & Lewis, M. C. (2005). Coantagonism of glutamate receptors and nicotinic acetylcholinergic receptors disrupts fear conditioning and latent inhibition of fear conditioning. Learning & Memory, 12, 389–398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gould, T. J., & Wehner, J. M. (1999). Genetic influences on latent inhibition. Behavioral Neuroscience, 113, 1291–1296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guterman, Y., Josiassen, R., Bashore, R. E., Johnson, M., & Lubow, R. E. (1996). Latent inhibition effects reflected in event-related brain potentials in healthy controls and schizophrenics. Schizophrenia Research, 20, 315–326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, J. A. (1998). Integrating schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 24, 249–266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, J. A., Feldon, J., Rawlins, J. N. P., Hemsley, D. R., & Smith, A. D. (1991). The neuropsychology of schizophrenia. Behavioral Brain Science, 14, 1–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, J. A., & McNaughton, N. (1983). Comparison between the behavioral effects of septal and hippocampal lesions: a review. Neuroscience & Biobehavior Reviews, 7, 119–188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, N. S., Fernandez, M., Williams, J., Ruddle, R. A., & Snowden, R. J. (2002). Which schizotypal dimensions abolish latent inhibition? British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 41, 271–284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, G. (1991). Perceptual and Associative Learning. Oxford: Clarendon Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, G., & Channell, S. (1986). Context specificity of latent inhibition in taste aversion learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38B, 121–139.Google Scholar
Harrell, , A. V., & Allan, A. M. (2003). Improvements in hippocampal-dependent learning and decremental attention in 5-HT3 receptor overexpressing mice. Learning & Memory, 10, 410–419.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of Behavior. NewYork: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1952). A Behavior System. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
James, W. (1950). The Principles of Psychology, vol. 1. New York: Dover (Original published in 1890).Google Scholar
Kaplan, O., Dar, R., Rosenthal, L., et al. (2006). Obsessive-compulsive disorder patients display enhanced latent inhibition on a visual search task. Behavior Research and Therapy, 44, 1137–1145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kathmann, N., Recum, S., Haag, C., & Engel, R. R. (2000). Electrophysiological evidence for reduced latent inhibition in schizophrenic patients. Schizophrenia Research, 45, 103–114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaye, H., & Pearce, J. M. (1987). Hippocampal lesions attenuate latent inhibition of a CS and of a neutral stimulus. Psychobiology, 15, 293–299.Google Scholar
Kilts, C., Scibilia, R., & Dunn, L. (1990). Selective breeding for distinct stimulus filtering/selective attention behavioral phenotypes. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 16, 189.11.Google Scholar
Kline, L., Decena, E., Hitzemann, R., & McCaughran, J. (1998). Acoustic startle, prepulse inhibition, locomotion, and latent inhibition in the neuroleptic responsive (NR) and neuroleptic nonresponsive (NNR) lines of mice. Psychopharmacology, 139, 322–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, M. C., & Gould, T. J. (2007a). Reversible inactivation of the entorhinal cortex disrupts the establishment and expression of latent inhibition of cued fear conditioning in C57BL/6 mice. Hippocampus, 17, 462–470.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, M. C., & Gould, T. J. (2007b). Signal transduction mechanisms within the entorhinal cortex that support latent inhibition of cued fear conditioning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 88, 359–368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lovibond, P. F., Preston, G. C., & Mackintosh, N. (1984). Context specificity of conditioning, extinction, and latent inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 10, 360–375.Google Scholar
Lipina, T., Weiss, K., & Roder, J. (2007). The Ampakine CX546 restores the prepulse inhibition and latent inhibition deficits in mGluR5-deficient mice. Neuropsychopharmacology, 32, 745–756.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lubow, R. E. (1965). Latent inhibition: the effects of frequency non-reinforced preexposure to the CS. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 60, 454–457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lubow, R. E. (1973). Latent inhibition. Psychological Bulletin, 79, 398–407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lubow, R. E. (1989). Latent Inhibition and Conditioned Attention Theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lubow, R. E. (1998). Latent inhibition and behavioral pathology: some effects of stimulus preexposure. In O'Donohue, W. (Ed.), Learning and Behavior Therapy. Boston: Allyn and Bacon, pp. 107–121.Google Scholar
Lubow, R. E. (2005). The construct validity of the animal-latent inhibition model of selective attention deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 31, 139–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lubow, R. E., Alek, M., & Rifkin, B. (1976). The context effect: the relationship between stimulus preexposure and environmental preexposure determines subsequent learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2, 38–47.Google Scholar
Lubow, R. E., Braunstein-Bercovitz, H., Blumenthal, O., Kaplan, O., & Toren, P. (2005). Latent inhibition and asymmetrical visual-spatial attention in children with ADHD. Child Neuropsychology, 11, 445–457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lubow, R. E., Dressler, R., & Kaplan, O. (1999). Visual search and latent inhibition in de novo Parkinson patients. Neuropsychology, 13, 415–423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lubow, R. E., Feldon, J., & Weiner, I. (1982). An animal model of attention. In Levy, A. & Spiegelstein, M. Y. (Eds.), Behavioral Models and the Analysis of Drug Action. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Lubow, R. E., & Gewirtz, J. C. (1995). Latent inhibition in humans: data, theory, and implications for schizophrenia, Psychological Bulletin, 117, 87–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lubow, R. E., & Kaplan, O. (2005). The visual search analog of latent inhibition: implications for theories of irrelevant stimulus processing in normal and schizophrenic groups. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 224–243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lubow, R. E., Markman, R. E., & Allen, J. (1968). Latent inhibition and classical conditioning of the rabbit pinna response. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 66, 688–694.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lubow, R. E., & Moore, A. U. (1959). Latent inhibition: the effect of non-reinforced preexposure to the conditioned stimulus. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 52, 415–419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lubow, R. E., Schnur, P., & Rifkin, B. (1976). Latent inhibition and conditioned attention theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2, 163–174.Google Scholar
Lubow, R. E., & Siebert, L. (1969). Latent inhibition within the CER paradigm. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 69, 136–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lubow, R. E., Weiner, I., & Schnur, P. (1981). Conditioned attention theory. In Bower, G. H. (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, vol. 15. New York: Academic Press, pp. 1–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mackintosh, N. J. (1975). A theory of attention: variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychological Review, 82, 276–298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLaren, I. P. L., & Mackintosh, N. J. (2000). An elemental model of associative learning: I. Latent inhibition and perceptual learning. Animal Learning & Behavior, 28, 211–246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, U., Feldon, J., Schedlowski, M., & Yee, B. K. (2006a). Immunological stress at the maternal-foetal interface: a link between neurodevelopment and adult psychopathology. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 20, 378–388.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, U., Schwendener, S., Feldon, J., & Yee, B. K. (2006b). Prenatal and postnatal maternal contributions in the infection model of schizophrenia. Experimental Brain Research, 173, 243–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, R. R., Kasprow, W. J., & Schachtman, T. R. (1986). Retrieval variability: sources and consequences. American Journal of Psychology, 99, 145–218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyakawa, T., Leiter, L. M., Gerber, D. J., et al. (2003). Conditional calcineurin knockout mice exhibit multiple abnormal behaviors related to schizophrenia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 8987–8992.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, J. W., Goodell, N. A., & Solomon, P. R. (1976). Central cholinergic blockade by scopolamine and habituation, classical conditioning, and latent inhibition of rabbit's nictitating membrane response. Physiological Psychology, 4, 395–399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moser, P. C., Hitchcock, J. M., Lister, S., & Moran, P. M. (2000). The pharmacology of latent inhibition as an animal model of schizophrenia. Brain Research Reviews, 33, 275–307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned Reflexes. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pearce, J. M., & Hall, G. (1980). A model of Pavlovian learning: variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not unconditioned stimuli. Psychological Review, 87, 532–552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterschmitt, Y., Meyer, F., & Louilot, A. (2007). Neonatal functional blockade of the entorhinal cortex results in disruption of accumbal dopaminergic responses observed in latent inhibition paradigm in adult rats. European Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 2504–2513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pothuizen, H. H. J., Jongen-Relo, A. L., Feldon, J., & Yee, B. K. (2006). Latent inhibition of conditioned taste aversion is not disrupted, but can be enhanced, by selective nucleus accumbens shell lesions in rats. Neuroscience, 137, 1119–1130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reiss, S., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). CS habituation produces “latent inhibition” effect but no active “conditioned inhibition”. Learning and Motivation, 3, 237–245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. (1971). Summation and retardation tests of latent inhibition. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 75, 77–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In Black, A. H. & Prokasy, W. F. (Eds.), Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, pp. 64–99.Google Scholar
Rimer, M., Barrett, D. W., Maldonado, M. A., Vock, V. M., & Gonzalez-Lima, F. (2005). Neuregulin-1 immunoglobulin-like domain mutant mice: clozapine sensitivity and impaired latent inhibition. Neuroreport, 16, 271–275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savostjanov, A. N., & Ilyuchenok, R. Y. (1999). Spectra and correlations of human brain electrical activity (EEG) in the range of the alpha-rhythm frequency during manifestation of latent inhibition. Zhurnal Vysshei Nervnoi Deyatelnosti Imeni I Pavlova, 49, 610–619.Google Scholar
Schmajuk, N. A. (2002). Latent Inhibition and its Neural Substrates. Norwell, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmajuk, N. A., Lam, Y. -W., & Gray, J. A. (1996). Latent inhibition: a neural network approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Brhavior Processes, 22, 321–349.Google ScholarPubMed
Schnur, P., & Ksir, C. J. (1969). Latent inhibition in human eyelid conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 80, 388–389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Serra, A. M., Jones, S. H., Toone, B., & Gray, J. A. (2001). Impaired associative learning in chronic schizophrenics and their first-degree relatives: a study of latent inhibition and the Kamin blocking effect. Schizophrenia Research, 48, 273–289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sherrington, C. S. (1906). The Integrative Action of the Nervous System. London: Constable.Google Scholar
Smith, S. E. P., Li, J., Garbett, K., Minics, K., & Patterson, P. H. (2007). Maternal immune activation alters fetal brain development through interleukin-6. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 10695–10702.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sokolov, E. N. (1963). Perception and the Conditioned Reflex. New York: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Solomon, P. R., Crider, A., Winkelman, J. W., et al. (1981). Disrupted latent inhibition in the rat with chronic amphetamine or haloperidol-induced supersensitivity: relationship to schizophrenic attention disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 16, 519–538.Google ScholarPubMed
Solomon, P. R., Kiney, C. A., & Scott, D. S. (1978). Disruption of latent inhibition following systemic administration of parachlorophenylanine (PCPA). Physiology and Behavior, 20, 265–271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solomon, P. R., & Moore, J. W. (1975). Latent inhibition and stimulus generalization of classically conditioned nictitating membrane response in rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) following dorsal hippocampal ablation. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 89, 1192–1203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solomon, P. R., & Staton, D. M. (1982). Differential effects of microinjections of d-amphetamine into the nucleus accumbens or the caudate putamen on the rat's ability to ignore an irrelevant stimulus. Biological Psychiatry, 17, 743–756.Google ScholarPubMed
Spence, K. W. (1966). Cognitive and drive factors in the extinction of the conditioned eye blink in human subjects. Psychological Review, 73, 445–458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutherland, N. S., & Mackintosh, N. J. (1971). Mechanisms of Animal Discrimination Learning. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Swerdlow, N. R., Hartston, H. J., & Hartman, P. L. (1999). Enhanced visual latent inhibition in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 45, 482–488.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swerdlow, N. R., Magulac, M., Filion, D., & Zinner, S. (1996). Visuospatial priming and latent inhibition in children and adults with Tourette's disorder. Neuropsychology, 10, 485–494.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swerdlow, N. R., Stephany, N., Wasserman, L. C., Talledo, J., Sharp, R., Auerbach, P. P. (2003). Dopamine agonists disrupt visual latent inhibition in normal males using a within-subject design. Psychopharmacology, 169, 314–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1932). Purposive Behavior in Animals and Men. NewYork: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C., & Honzik, H. C. (1930). Introduction and removal of reward, and maze performance of rats. University of California Publications in Psychology, 4, 257–275.Google Scholar
Tsaltas, E., Preston, G. C., Rawlins, J. N. P., Winocur, G., & Gray, J. A. (1984). Dorsal bundle lesions do not affect latent inhibition of conditioned suppression. Psychopharmacology, 84, 549–555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tzschentke, T. M. (2001). Pharmacology and behavioral pharmacology of the mesocortical dopamine system. Progress in Neurobiology, 63, 241–320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagner, A. R. (1976). Priming in STM: an information processing mechanism for self-generated or retrieval-generated depression in performance. In Tighe, T. & Leaton, R. N. (Eds.), Habituation: Perspectives from Child Development, Animal Behavior, and Neurophysiology. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 95–128.Google Scholar
Wagner, A. R., & Rescorla, R. A. (1972). Inhibition in Pavlovian conditioning: application of a theory. In Boakes, R. A. & Halliday, M. S. (Eds.), Inhibition and Learning. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Wang, H., Ng, K., Hayes, D., et al. (2004). Decreased amphetamine-induced locomotion and improved latent inhibition in mice mutant for the M5 muscarinic receptor gene found in the human 15q schizophrenia region. Neuropsychopharmacology, 29, 2126–2139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiner, I. (1979). The effects of arousal on latent inhibition: Tests of conditioned attention theory. Unpublished Master's thesis, Tel Aviv University.
Weiner, I. (1983). The effects of amphetamine on latent inhibition: Tests of the animal amphetamine model of schizophrenia using selected learning paradigms. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Tel Aviv University.Google Scholar
Weiner, I. (1990). The neural substrates of latent inhibition. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 442–461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiner, I. (2003). The “two-headed” latent inhibition model of schizophrenia: modeling positive and negative symptoms and their treatment. Psychopharmacology, 169, 257–297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiner, I., & Feldon, J. (1987). Facilitation of latent inhibition by haloperidol. Psychopharmacology, 91, 248–253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiner, I., Feldon, J., & Katz, Y. (1987). Facilitation of the expression but not the acquisition of latent inhibition by haloperidol in rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 26, 241–246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, I., Gaisler, I., Schiller, D., et al. (2000). Screening of antipsychotic drugs in animal models. Drug Development Research, 50, 235–249.3.0.CO;2-R>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, I., Lubow, R. E., & Feldon, J. (1981). Chronic amphetamine and latent inhibition. Behavioral Brain Research, 2, 285–286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, I., Lubow, R. E., & Feldon, J. (1984). Abolition of the expression but not the acquisition of latent inhibition by chronic amphetamine in rats. Psychopharmacology, 83, 194–199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, I., Lubow, R. E., & Feldon, J. (1988). Disruption of latent inhibition by acute administration of low doses of amphetamine. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 30, 871–878.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, A. M. J., Kumari, V., Mehrotra, R., et al. (2005). Disruption of learned irrelevance in acute schizophrenia in a novel continuous within-subject paradigm suitable for fMRI. Behavioural Brain Research, 156, 277–288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×