Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-9q27g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-17T03:40:38.315Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

30 - Serendipity in Research: Origins of the DRM False Memory Paradigm

from Section B - Learning and Memory

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 August 2016

Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Cornell University, New York
Susan T. Fiske
Affiliation:
Princeton University, New Jersey
Donald J. Foss
Affiliation:
University of Houston
Get access

Summary

The editors have given the authors a difficult task: Pick out one of our “most important contributions to research” and write about it. Just the first part had me stumped: Most important in what way? I decided to subvert the question into telling about my most-cited article (which may or may not be the most important). Citations at least indicate whether others have found the work useful, one possible meaning of importance.

I write about what has come to be called the Deese–Roediger–McDermott (DRM) paradigm, owing to a suggestion by Endel Tulving. And Tulving is responsible in more ways than one. I had introduced him for a talk at Rice University in 1993 while I was on the faculty. The talk was on “the brain's proclivity for primacy” or why first (and new) events are well remembered. After my introduction, I sat off to the side. The talk was exciting, and the audience stayed long afterwards, asking questions. As moderator, I thought maybe they were going on too long, so I was about to pull the plug. Happily, I waited. Someone asked Tulving a question (the person and the question are lost to me), and during his reply Tulving said: “Your comment reminds me of an experiment by James Deese from years ago in which he presented lists of words, asked people to recall them in any order, and found they often intruded a related word in their recall” (my reconstruction of his answer, of course, not his exact words).

I wrote down “Deese experiment” on a note card. Later, I asked Tulving what year it might have been. He said “probably 1959.” He and I talked about how unusual that finding was, because free recall of words is often surprisingly error free.

The note got buried in a pile on my desk. A couple of months later I found it while cleaning up. This was before all the journals were online, so sometime later I trekked to the library and copied two Deese papers from 1959. One was the correct one (it had rarely been cited). I discovered that Deese had created lists of related words, words that were associates to a key, seed word. For example, he took the top associates to the word sleep (bed, rest, awake, tired, dream, etc.) for one of his lists.

Type
Chapter
Information
Scientists Making a Difference
One Hundred Eminent Behavioral and Brain Scientists Talk about Their Most Important Contributions
, pp. 144 - 147
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Gallo, D. A. (2006). Associative illusions of memory: False memory research in DRM and related tasks. New York: Psychology Press.
McDermott, K. B. (2007). Inducing false memories through associated lists: A window onto everyday false memories? In Nairne, J. S. (ed.), The foundations of remembering: Essays in honor of Henry L. Roediger, III. New York: Psychology Press, 2007.
Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 21, 803–814.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×