Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-vfjqv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T22:04:01.101Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

26 - Levels of Processing in Human Memory

from Section B - Learning and Memory

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 August 2016

Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Cornell University, New York
Susan T. Fiske
Affiliation:
Princeton University, New Jersey
Donald J. Foss
Affiliation:
University of Houston
Get access

Summary

Throughout my professional career as a cognitive psychologist, I have been interested in the topics of memory, attention, perception, and thinking – how best to characterize them, how they relate to each other, and how they change over a person's lifespan. In everyday life, these mental activities are usually considered to be rather different from each other – remembering meeting someone a month ago seems different from seeing the person in front of you – and this separation is often echoed in psychology textbooks, in which perception, memory, and decision-making are treated in different chapters. Much of the current thinking in cognitive psychology has reacted against this commonsense view, however, suggesting instead that these areas of study are better regarded as closely related and interacting aspects of one general processing system.

This latter position was one starting point for the formulation of the levels of processing (LOP) framework for memory research proposed by Robert Lockhart and myself in 1972. The LOP article with Lockhart, plus a later empirical article with Endel Tulving in 1975, are my most-cited pieces of published research, and may therefore be regarded as my best-known scientific contributions to cognitive psychology. Additionally, the general ideas in which the LOP framework was embedded – for example, that remembering should be regarded as an activity of mind rather than a collection of structural “memory traces” waiting to be revived – have always been central to my thinking about memory. Thus, the LOP paper and its spinoffs have been the starting point for much of the work that my lab has produced over the years.

In the 1960s, ideas about learning and memory were changing from the belief that the formation of associations between two mental events was the crucial element, to concepts derived from information-processing theories. From this latter point of view, the brain/mind was regarded as a highly sophisticated computer, processing sensory information from the environment, performing computations on that information, and finally translating the products into relevant actions. To accomplish these operations efficiently, the proposed system needed a variety of memory stores, holding information of different qualitative types either temporarily, while it was processed, or relatively permanently, in the case of learned knowledge.

Type
Chapter
Information
Scientists Making a Difference
One Hundred Eminent Behavioral and Brain Scientists Talk about Their Most Important Contributions
, pp. 128 - 131
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Craik, F. I. M. (2002). Levels of processing: Past, present … and future? Memory, 10, 305–318.Google Scholar
Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671–684.Google Scholar
Craik, F. I. M., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 268–294.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×