Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-rvbq7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-11T11:42:22.208Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

15 - Children's Learning through Participation in Institutional Practice: A Model from the Perspective of Cultural-Historical Psychology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 August 2009

Mariane Hedegaard
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Copenhagen, Denmark
Bert van Oers
Affiliation:
Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam
Wim Wardekker
Affiliation:
Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam
Ed Elbers
Affiliation:
Universiteit Utrecht, The Netherlands
René van der Veer
Affiliation:
Universiteit Leiden
Get access

Summary

A child learns through participation with others in institutionalised activities. Learning takes place through the tasks and expectations that the child meets in these activities. But the child also contributes to the creation, change, and development of the activities and thereby to his own surroundings and learning conditions. The child is not only adapting to tasks and demands. Each person, whether a schoolchild, a teacher, or a parent, contributes to the activities in which he or she takes part and thereby, as a collective, creates the conditions for each other's activity.

In traditional classroom teaching, learning is seen as an individualised activity promoted by the teacher's lectures and formulation of questions and the students' answers and work with exercise material. Classmates are seen as interfering with the single child's learning process. In the more modern teaching types, where children can work together, teaching is still expected to relate to and measure each student's competence, and thereby learning is still conceptualised as a change in the child (Hedegaard, 2006). In these approaches, the product of the children's activity is not seen as important; what is conceptualised is the process of learning as change in the children's minds. This conceptualisation, by itself, gives a too-narrow perspective on learning activity, as I argue in this chapter.

Alternative approaches to teaching include those based on activity theory: Davydov's approach (1982, 1990) of “developmental teaching,” Lompscher's approach (1984, 1999) of “ascending from the abstract to the concrete,” and my own approach (Hedegaard, 2002) of “the double move in teaching.”

Type
Chapter
Information
The Transformation of Learning
Advances in Cultural-Historical Activity Theory
, pp. 294 - 318
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Davydov, V. V. (1982). Ausbildung der Lerntätigkeit [Development of learning activity]. In Davydov, V. V., Lompscher, J., & Markova, A. K. (Eds.), Ausbildung der Lerntätigkeit bei Schülern (pp. 37–44). Berlin: Volk und Wissen.Google Scholar
Davydov, V. V. (1990). Types of generalization in instruction: Logical and psychological problems in the structuring of school curricula (Soviet studies in mathematics education, Vol. 2) (Kilpatrick, J., Ed.; Teller, J., Trans.). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (Original work published 1972).Google Scholar
Davydov, V. V. (1999). What is real learning activity. In Hedegaard, M. & Lompscher, J. (Eds.), Learning activity and development (pp. 123–138). Aarhus: Aarhus University Press.Google Scholar
El'konin, D. B. (1999). Toward the problem of stages in the mental development of children. Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, 37 (6), 11–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hedegaard, M. (2002). Learning and child development. Aarhus: Aarhus University Press.Google Scholar
Hedegaard, M. (2006). Undervisning i klasser med mange b⊘rn fra indvandrerfamilier. In Elle, B., Nielsen, K., & Nissen, M. (Eds.), Pædagogisk psykologi [Pedagogical psychology] (pp. 123–145). Roskilde: Roskilde Universitetsforlag.Google Scholar
Hedegaard, M. (2007). The development of children's conceptual relation to the world, with focus on concept formation in pre-school children's activity. In Daniels, H., Cole, M., & Wertsch, J. (Eds.), The Cambridge Companion to Vygotsky (pp. 246–275). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hedegaard, M., & Chaiklin, S. (2005). Radical local teaching and learning. Aarhus: Aarhus University Press.Google Scholar
Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in practice: Mind, mathematics, and culture in everyday life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leontiev, A. N. (1972). The problem of activity in psychology. In Wertsch, J. W. (Ed.), Culture, communication and cognition: A Vygotskian perspective (pp. 37–69). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lompscher, J. (1984). Problems and results of experimental research on the formation of theoretical thinking through instruction. In Hedegaard, M., Hakkarainen, P., & Engeström, Y. (Eds.), Learning and teaching on a scientific basis (pp. 293–356). Aarhus: Aarhus University, Institute of Psychology.Google Scholar
Lompscher, J. (1999). Learning activity and its formation: Ascending from the abstract to the concrete. In Hedegaard, M. & Lompscher, J. (Eds.), Learning activity and development (pp. 139–168). Aarhus: Aarhus University Press.Google Scholar
Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Scribner, S. (1985). Vygotsky's use of history. In Wertsch, J. V. (Ed.), Culture, communication, and cognition: A Vygotskian perspective (pp. 119–145). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Scribner, S., & Stevens, J. (1989). Experimental studies on the relationship of school math and work math. Technical Paper Series No. 4. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University.Google Scholar
Wartofsky, M. (1979). Models – Representations and the scientific understanding. Dordrecht: D. Reidel.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1982). Om barnets psykiske udvikling [About the child's psychic development]. Copenhagen: Nyt Nordisk Forlag.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). Thinking and speech. In Rieber, R. W. & Carton, A. S. (Eds.), The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky: Vol. 1. Problems of general psychology (pp. 39–285). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1997a). The instrumental method in psychology. The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky: Vol. 3. Problems of the theory and history of psychology (pp. 85–89). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1997b). Educational psychology. Boca Raton, FL: St Lucie Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×