Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-4hhp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-26T12:54:45.911Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Dual-process moral judgment beyond fast and slow

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 July 2023

Joshua D. Greene*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Center for Brain Science, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA jgreene@wjh.harvard.edu https://www.joshua-greene.net

Abstract

De Neys makes a compelling case that the sacrificial moral dilemmas do not elicit competing “fast and slow” processes. But are there even two processes? Or just two intuitions? There remains strong evidence, most notably from lesion studies, that sacrificial dilemmas engage distinct cognitive processes generating conflicting emotional and rational responses. The dual-process theory gets much right, but needs revision.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bago, B., & De Neys, W. (2019). The intuitive greater good: Testing the corrective dual process model of moral cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(10), 1782.10.1037/xge0000533CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bago, B., Kovacs, M., Protzko, J., Nagy, T., Kekecs, Z., Palfi, B., … Matibag, C. J. (2022). Situational factors shape moral judgements in the trolley dilemma in eastern, southern and western countries in a culturally diverse sample. Nature Human Behaviour, 6(6), 880895.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balleine, B. W., & O'Doherty, J. P. (2010). Human and rodent homologies in action control: Corticostriatal determinants of goal-directed and habitual action. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(1), 4869.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron, J., & Gürçay, B. (2017). A meta-analysis of response-time tests of the sequential two-systems model of moral judgment. Memory & Cognition, 45(4), 566575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ciaramelli, E., Muccioli, M., Làdavas, E., & Di Pellegrino, G. (2007). Selective deficit in personal moral judgment following damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2(2), 8492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cova, F., Strickland, B., Abatista, A., Allard, A., Andow, J., Attie, M., … Zhou, X. (2021). Estimating the reproducibility of experimental philosophy. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 12(1), 944.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craver, C. F., Keven, N., Kwan, D., Kurczek, J., Duff, M. C., & Rosenbaum, R. S. (2016). Moral judgment in episodic amnesia. Hippocampus, 26(8), 975979.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crockett, M. J. (2013). Models of morality. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(8), 363366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cushman, F. (2013). Action, outcome, and value: A dual-system framework for morality. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17(3), 273292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cushman, F., Young, L., & Hauser, M. (2006). The role of conscious reasoning and intuition in moral judgment: Testing three principles of harm. Psychological Science, 17(12), 10821089.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greene, J. (2013). Moral tribes: Emotion, reason, and the gap between us and them. Penguin.Google Scholar
Greene, J. D. (2014). Beyond point-and-shoot morality: Why cognitive (neuro) science matters for ethics. Ethics, 124(4), 695726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greene, J. D. (2017). The rat-a-gorical imperative: Moral intuition and the limits of affective learning. Cognition, 167, 6677.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greene, J. D., Cushman, F. A., Stewart, L. E., Lowenberg, K., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2009). Pushing moral buttons: The interaction between personal force and intention in moral judgment. Cognition, 111(3), 364371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greene, J. D., Morelli, S. A., Lowenberg, K., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2008). Cognitive load selectively interferes with utilitarian moral judgment. Cognition, 107(3), 11441154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greene, J. D., Nystrom, L. E., Engell, A. D., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment. Neuron, 44(2), 389400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science (New York, N.Y.), 293(5537), 21052108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gürçay, B., & Baron, J. (2017). Challenges for the sequential two-system model of moral judgement. Thinking & Reasoning, 23(1), 4980.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58(9), 697.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. Macmillan.Google Scholar
Koenigs, M., Young, L., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Cushman, F., Hauser, M., & Damasio, A. (2007). Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements. Nature, 446(7138), 908911.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koop, G. J. (2013). An assessment of the temporal dynamics of moral decisions. Judgment and Decision Making, 8(5), 527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCormick, C., Rosenthal, C. R., Miller, T. D., & Maguire, E. A. (2016). Hippocampal damage increases deontological responses during moral decision making. Journal of Neuroscience, 36(48), 1215712167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, R., & Cushman, F. (2013). Aversive for me, wrong for you: First-person behavioral aversions underlie the moral condemnation of harm. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7(10), 707718.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moretto, G., Làdavas, E., Mattioli, F., & Di Pellegrino, G. (2010). A psychophysiological investigation of moral judgment after ventromedial prefrontal damage. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(8), 18881899.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patil, I., Zucchelli, M. M., Kool, W., Campbell, S., Fornasier, F., Calò, M., … Cushman, F. (2021). Reasoning supports utilitarian resolutions to moral dilemmas across diverse measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 120(2), 443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosas, A., Bermúdez, J. P., & Aguilar-Pardo, D. (2019). Decision conflict drives reaction times and utilitarian responses in sacrificial dilemmas. Judgment & Decision Making, 14(5), 555564.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shenhav, A., & Greene, J. D. (2014). Integrative moral judgment: Dissociating the roles of the amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(13), 47414749.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suter, R. S., & Hertwig, R. (2011). Time and moral judgment. Cognition, 119(3), 454458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutton, R. S., & Barto, A. G. (2018). Reinforcement learning: An introduction. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Thomas, B. C., Croft, K. E., & Tranel, D. (2011). Harming kin to save strangers: Further evidence for abnormally utilitarian moral judgments after ventromedial prefrontal damage. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(9), 21862196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomson, J. J. (1985). The trolley problem. Yale Law Journal, 94(6), 13951415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tinghög, G., Andersson, D., Bonn, C., Johannesson, M., Kirchler, M., Koppel, L., & Västfjäll, D. (2016). Intuition and moral decision-making – The effect of time pressure and cognitive load on moral judgment and altruistic behavior. PLoS ONE, 11(10), e0164012.10.1371/journal.pone.0164012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trémolière, B., De Neys, W., & Bonnefon, J. F. (2012). Mortality salience and morality: Thinking about death makes people less utilitarian. Cognition, 124(3), 379384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Honk, J., Terburg, D., Montoya, E. R., Grafman, J., Stein, D. J., & Morgan, B. (2022). Breakdown of utilitarian moral judgement after basolateral amygdala damage. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 119(31), e2119072119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verfaellie, M., Hunsberger, R., & Keane, M. M. (2021). Episodic processes in moral decisions: Evidence from medial temporal lobe amnesia. Hippocampus, 31(6), 569579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed