Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-22dnz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T12:03:53.617Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Evaluating Theories

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Simon Dennis
Affiliation:
University of Adelaide
Walter Kintsch
Affiliation:
University of Colorado
Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Henry L. Roediger III
Affiliation:
Washington University, St Louis
Diane F. Halpern
Affiliation:
Claremont McKenna College, California
Get access

Summary

All theories are false (Popper, 1959). So in one sense evaluating theories is a straightforward matter. However, some theories are more false than others. Furthermore, some theories have characteristics that tend to promote the advance of scientific knowledge. In this chapter, we examine what some of those characteristics are and how one goes about the process of identifying and building useful theories.

A theory is a concise statement about how we believe the world to be. Theories organize observations of the world and allow researchers to make predictions about what will happen in the future under certain conditions. Science is about the testing of theories, and the data that we collect as scientists should either implicitly or explicitly bear on theory.

There is, however, a great difference between theories in the hard sciences and theories in the soft sciences in their formal rigor. Formal theories are well established and incredibly successful in physics, but they play a lesser role in biology, and even less in psychology, where theories are often stated in verbal form. This has certainly been true historically, but some scientists, especially physicists, as well as laypeople, construe this fact to mean that formal theories are restricted to the hard sciences, particularly physics, while formalization is unattainable in the soft sciences. There is absolutely no reason to think so. Indeed, this is a pernicious idea that would permanently relegate psychology to second-class status.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Akaike, H. (1973). Information theory and an extension of the maximum likelihood principle. In Petrox, B. N. & Caski, F. (Eds.), Second international symposium on information theory (pp. 267–281). Budapest, Hungary: Akadémiai Kiadṍ.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1990). The adaptive character of thought. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R., & Lebiere, C. (1998). The atomic components of thought. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R., & Schooler, L. J. (1991). Reflections of the environment in memory. Psychological Science, 2, 396–408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bozdogan, H. (1990). On the information-based measure of covariance complexity and its application to the evaluation of multivariate linear models. Communications in Statistics: Theory and Methods, 19, 221–278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broadbent, D. (1987). Simple models for experimental situations. In Morris, P. (Ed.), Modeling cognition (pp. 169–185). London: Wiley.Google Scholar
Brown, S. C., & Craik, F. I. M. (2000). Encoding and retrieval of information. In Tulving, E. and Craik, F. I. M. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 93–108). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bush, V. (1945). Science – The endless frontier, appendix 3. (Report of the Committee on Science and the Public Welfare). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.Google Scholar
Cary, M., & Reder, L. M. (2003). A dual-process account of the list-length and strength-based mirror effects in recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 231–248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. (1994). The earth is round (p <.05). American Psychologist, 49, 997–1003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennis, S., & Humphreys, M. S. (2001). A context noise model of episodic word recognition. Psychological Review, 108, 452–477.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, G. N., & Mulkay, (1984). Opening Pandora's box: A sociological analysis of scientists' discourse. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gillund, G., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1984). A retrieval model for both recognition and recall. Psychological Review, 91, 1–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gronlund, S. D., & Elam, L. E. (1994). List-length effect: Recognition accuracy and variance of underlying distributions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 1355–1369.Google Scholar
Hintzman, D. L. (1984). MINERVA2: A simulation model of human memory. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 16, 96–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hintzman, D. L. (1991). Why are formal models useful in psychology? In Hockley, W. E. and Lewandowsky, S. (Eds.), Relating theory and data: Essays on human memory in honor of Bennet B. Murdock (pp. 39–56). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Humphreys, M. S., Bain, J. D., & Pike, R. (1989). Different ways to cue a coherent memory system: A theory for episodic, semantic and procedural tasks. Psychological Review, 96, 208–233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kass, R. E., & Raftery, A. E. (1995). Bayes factors. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 90, 773–795.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W. (1977). Memory and Cognition. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kuhn, T. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato's problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104, 211–240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landauer, T. K., Laham, D., & Foltz, P. W. (2000). The Intelligent Essay Assessor. IEEE Intelligent Systems, 15(5), 27–31.Google Scholar
Leahey, T. H., & Harris, R J. (1985). Human learning. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Murdock, B. B. Jr. (1982). A theory for the storage and retrieval of items and associative information. Psychological Review, 89, 609–626.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myung, I. J., Balasubramanian, V., & Pitt, M. A. (2000). Counting probability distributions: Differential geometry and model selection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 97, 11,170–11,175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myung, I. J., Kim, C., & Pitt, M. A. (2000). Towards an explanation of the power law artifact: Insights from response surface analysis. Memory & Cognition, 28, 832–840.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myung, I. J., & Pitt, M. A. (1997). Applying Occam's razor in modeling cognition: A Bayesian approach. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 79–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Navarro, D. J., & Lee, M. D. (in press). Common and distinctive features in stimulus representation: A modified version of the contrast model. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.Google Scholar
Navarro, D. J., Myung, I. J., Pitt, M. A., & Kim, W. (2003). Global model analysis by landscaping. In R. Alterman & D. Kirsh (Eds.), Proceedings of the 25th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Newell, A. (1973). You can't play twenty questions with nature and win. In Chase, W. C. (Ed.), Visual information processing (pp. 283–308). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Newell, A. (1990). Unified theories of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Pitt, M. A., & Myung, I. J. (2002). When a good fit can be bad. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 421–425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pitt, M. A., Myung, I. J., & Zhang, S. (2002). Towards a method of selecting amoung computational models of cognition. Psychological Review, 109, 472–491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Popper, K. R. (1959). The logic of scientific discovery. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Ratcliff, R., Clarke, S., & Shiffrin, R. (1990). The list strength effect: I. Data and discussion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 163–178.Google ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, R. A. (1970). Reduction in the effectiveness of reinforcement after prior excitatory conditioning. Learning and Motivation, 1, 372–381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rissanen, J. (1996). Fisher information and stochastic complexity. IEEE Transactions in Information Theory, 42, 40–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, S., & Pashler, H. (2000). How persuasive is a good fit? A comment on theory testing. Psychological Review, 107, 358–367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwarz, G. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. The Annals of Statistics, 6, 461–464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stokes, D. E. (1997). Pasteur's quadrant: Basic science and technological innovation. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press.Google Scholar
Stone, M. (1974). Cross-validatory choice and assessment of statistical predictions (with discussion). Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B, 36, 111–147.Google Scholar
Wagenmakers, E. J., Ratcliff, R.Gomez, P., & Iverson, G. J. (2004). Assessing model mimicry using the parametric bootstrap. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 48, 28–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watkins, M. J. (1990). Mediationism and the obfuscation of memory. American Psychologist, 45, 328–335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilkinson, L., and the Task Force on Statistical Inference APA Board of Scientific Affairs. (1999). Statistical methods in psychology journals: Guidelines and explanations. American Psychologist, 54, 594–604.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×