Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-p2v8j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-01T12:39:51.684Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

13 - Evaluating Parapsychological Claims

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Ray Hyman
Affiliation:
University of Oregon
Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Henry L. Roediger III
Affiliation:
Washington University, St Louis
Diane F. Halpern
Affiliation:
Claremont McKenna College, California
Get access

Summary

Imagine that you have been assigned to evaluate a parapsychological claim. The assignment could be from a journal editor or from a media outlet. Or it could come from a granting agency or an instructor. You might be faced with such a task in your role as a referee for a professional journal. Or you might take on the assignment out of curiosity.

How would you proceed? Your first step might be to decide on the scope of the parapsychological claim. Are you dealing with a specific claim based on one experiment? Or is the claim based on evidence from a series of experiments? Perhaps you want to assess the status of the entire field of parapsychology.

What, indeed, makes a claim “parapsychological?” The term parapsychology was borrowed by J. B. Rhine to refer to what previously had been called psychical research. With the new label, Rhine wanted to promote an experimental science. Previous to Rhine, psychical research had focused on field investigations of haunted houses, tests of spiritualist mediums, surveys of premonitory dreams, and other dramatic, but controversial, paranormal claims. Rhine wanted parapsychology to focus on quantitative evidence obtained in controlled, laboratory settings. Beginning in the 1930s, parapsychologists started accumulating experimental evidence for the existence of extrasensory perception (ESP) and psychokinesis (PK). According to the glossary in the back of The Journal of Parapsychology, ESP is defined as “Experience of, or response to, a target object, state, event, or influence without sensory contact.” ESP includes telepathy, clairvoyance, and precognition.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Akers, C. (1984). Methodological criticisms of parapsychology. In Krippner, S. (Ed.), Advances in parapsychological research (vol. 4, pp. 112–164). Jefferson, NC: McFarland.Google Scholar
Bem, D. J. (1994). Response to Hyman. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 25–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bem, D. J., & Honorton, C. (1994). Does psi exist? Replicable evidence for an anomalous process of information transfer. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 4–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bem, D. J., Palmer, J., & Broughton, R. S. (2001). Updating the ganzfeld data base: A victim of its own success?Journal of Parapsychology, 65, 207–18.Google Scholar
Bierman, D. J. (2000). On the nature of anomalous phenomena: Another reality between the world of subjective consciousness and the objective world of physics? In Van, P. Loocke (Ed.), The physical nature of consciousness (pp. 269–292). New York: Benjamin.Google Scholar
Broad, C. D. (1953). Religion, philosophy and psychical research. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Hansel, C. E. M. (1966). ESP: A scientific evaluation. New York: Scribner's.Google Scholar
Honorton, C. (1985). Meta-analysis of psi ganzfeld research: A response to Hyman. Journal of Parapsychology, 49, 51–91.Google Scholar
Honorton, C., & Harper, S. (1974). Psi-mediated imagery and ideation in an experimental procedure for regulating perceptual input. Journal of the American Society for Psychical Research, 68, 156–168.Google Scholar
Honorton, C., Berger, R. E., Varvoglis, M. P., Quant, M., Derr, P., Schechter, E. I., et al. (1990). Psi communication in the ganzfeld: Experiments with an automated testing system and a comparison with a meta-analysis of earlier studies. Journal of Parapsychology, 54, 19–24.Google Scholar
Hyman, R. (1957). Review of Modern experiments in telepathy. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 52, 607–610. (Reprinted in Hyman, 1989).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyman, R. (1985a). A critical historical overview of parapsychology. In Kurtz, P. (Ed.), A skeptic's handbook of parapsychology (pp. 3–96). Buffalo, NY: Prometheus Books.Google Scholar
Hyman, R. (1985b). The ganzfeld psi experiment: A critical appraisal. Journal of Parapsychology, 49, 3–49.Google Scholar
Hyman, R. (1986). Parapsychological research: A tutorial review and critical appraisal. Proceedings of the IEEE, 74, 823–849.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyman, R. (1994). Anomaly or artifact? Comments on Bem and Honorton. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 19–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyman, R., & Honorton, C. (1986). A joint communiqué: The psi ganzfeld controversy. Journal of Parapsychology, 50, 351–364.Google Scholar
Markwick, B. (1985). The establishment of data manipulation in the Soal-Shackleton experiments. In Kurtz, P. (Ed.), A skeptic's handbook of parapsychology (pp. 287–311). Buffalo, NY: Prometheus Books.Google Scholar
Milton, J., & Wiseman, R. (1999). Does psi exist? Lack of replication of an anomalous process of information transfer. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 387–391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milton, J., & Wiseman, R. (2001). Does psi exist? Reply to Storm and Ertel (2001). Psychological Bulletin, 127, 434–438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Price, G. R. (1955). Science and the supernatural. Science, 122, 359–367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radin, D. I. (1997). The conscious universe. San Francisco: HarperEdge.Google Scholar
Soal, S. G., & Bateman, F. (1954). Modern experiments in telepathy (2nd ed.). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Storm, L., & Ertel, S. (2001). Does psi exist? Milton and Wiseman's (1999) meta-analysis of ganzfeld research. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 424–433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Utts, J. (1991). Replication and meta-analysis in parapsychology. Statistical Science, 6, 363–403 [Includes commentaries and Utts' rejoinder to commentators].CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×