Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T10:38:53.851Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - Evaluating Experimental Research

Critical Issues

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Henry L. Roediger III
Affiliation:
Washington University in St. Louis
David P. McCabe
Affiliation:
Washington University in St. Louis
Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Henry L. Roediger III
Affiliation:
Washington University, St Louis
Diane F. Halpern
Affiliation:
Claremont McKenna College, California
Get access

Summary

[T]he application of the experimental method to the problem of mind is the great outstanding event in the study of the mind, an event to which no other is comparable.

The author of this quote is Edwin G. Boring (1886–1968), one of the great psychologists of the 20th century and author of A History of Experimental Psychology (1929; the quote comes from p. 659). Contemporary psychologists take “the psychology experiment” as a given, but it is actually a relatively recent cultural invention. Although fascination with human behavior is doubtless as old as the emergence of Homo sapiens, the application of experimental methods to the study of the human mind and behavior is only 150 or so years old. Scientific methods, with heavy reliance on experimental technique, arose in Western civilization during the time of the Renaissance, when great insights and modes of thoughts from the ancient Greek, Roman, and Arab civilizations were rediscovered. The 17th century witnessed the great discoveries of Kepler, Galileo, and Newton in the physical world. Interest in chemistry and biology arose after the early development of physics. Experimental physiology arose as a discipline in the late 1700s and early 1800s. Still, despite great advances in these fields and despite the fact that scientists of the day usually conducted research in many different fields, no one at that time performed experiments studying humans or their mental life. The first physiologists and anatomists mostly contented themselves with the study of corpses.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bacon, F. The new organon. (L. Jardine & M. Silverthorne, trans.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. (Original work published in 1620).
Balota, D. A., Cortese, M. J., Duchek, J. M., Adams, D. A., Roediger, H. L., McDermott, K. B., & Yerys, B. E. (1999). Veridical and false memories in healthy older adults and in dementia of the Alzheimers type. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 16, 361–384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Banaji, M. R., & Crowder, R. G. (1989). The bankruptcy of everyday memory. American Psychologist, 44, 1185–1193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boring, E. G. (1929). History of experimental psychology. London: Century/Random House.Google Scholar
Brainerd, C. J. & Reyna, V. F. (2005). The science of false memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clancy, S. A., McNally, R. J., Schacter, D. L., Lenzenweger, M. F., & Pitman, R. K. (2002). Memory distortion in people reporting abduction by aliens. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 455–461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal instructions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 17–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freyd, J. J., & Gleaves, D. H. (1996). “Remembering” words not presented in lists: Relevance to the current recovered/false memory controversy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 811–813.Google Scholar
Gates, A. I. (1917). Recitation as a factor in memorizing. Archives of Psychology, 26, 1–104.Google Scholar
Jenkins, J. J. (1979). Four points to remember: A tetrahedral model of memory experiments. In Cermak, L. S. & Craik, F. I. M. (Eds.), Levels of processing in human memory (pp. 429–446). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kantowitz, B. H., Roediger, H. L., & Elmes, D. (2005). Experimental psychology (8th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (1999). When true memories suppress false memories: Effects of aging. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 16, 399–415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mather, M., Henkel, L. A., & Johnson, M. K. (1997). Evaluating characteristics of false memories: Remember/know and memory characteristics questionnaire compared. Memory & Cognition, 25, 826–837.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCabe, D. P., Presmanes, A. G., Robertson, C. L., & Smith, A. D. (2004). Item-specific processing reduces false memories. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 1074–1079.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCabe, D. P., & Smith, A. D. (in press). The distinctiveness heuristic in false recognition and false recall. Memory.Google Scholar
McDermott, K. B. (1996). The persistence of false memories in list recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 212–230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDermott, K. B., & Watson, J. M. (2001). The rise and fall of false recall: The impact of presentation duration. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 160–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mook, D. G. (1983). In defense of external invalidity. American Psychologist, 38, 379–387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. New YorkNorton.Google Scholar
Robinson, K. J., & Roediger, H. L. (1997). Associative processes in false recall and false recognition. Psychological Science, 8, 231–237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roediger, H. L., Balota, D. A., & Watson, J. M. (2001). Spreading activation and the arousal of false memories. In Roediger, H. L., Nairne, J. S., Neath, I., & Surprenant, A. M. (Eds.), The nature of remembering: Essays in honor of Robert G. Crowder (pp. 95–115). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roediger, H. L., & Gallo, D. A. (2002). Processes affecting accuracy and distortion in memory: An overview. In Eisen, M. L., Goodman, G. S., & Quas, J. A. (Eds.), Memory and suggestibility in the forensic interview (pp. 3–28). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Roediger, H. L., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006). Test-enhanced learning: Taking memory tests improves long-term retention. Psychological Science, 17, 249–255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803–814.Google Scholar
Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1996). False perceptions of false memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 814–816.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Verfaellie, M., & Pradere, D. (1996). The neuropsychology of memory illusions: False recall and recognition in amnesic patients. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 319–334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thapar, A., & McDermott, K. B. (2001). False recall and false recognition induced by presentation of associated words: Effects of retention interval and level of processing. Memory & Cognition, 29, 424–432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tulving, E. (1967). The effects of presentation and recall of material in free-recall learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 175–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology, 26, 1–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Underwood, B. J. (1965). False recognition produced by implicit verbal responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, 122–129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×