Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ndmmz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-25T08:25:48.961Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - Child Poverty and Brain Development

from Section A - Feelings, Fears, Stressors, and Coping

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 August 2016

Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Cornell University, New York
Susan T. Fiske
Affiliation:
Princeton University, New Jersey
Donald J. Foss
Affiliation:
University of Houston
Get access

Summary

What's a nice cognitive neuroscientist doing in a field like child poverty? Why would anybody work with ill-defined concepts such as socioeconomic status (SES) when they could be manipulating binocular cues to depth perception with precision? Allow me to explain, by way of presenting my current work and how I came to it.

The first decades of my career were spent working to understand the neural bases of vision and visual cognition, using behavioral research methods with normal participants and brain-damaged patients, as well as by using event-related potentials (ERPs) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to study normal brain function more directly. The 1980s and 1990s were rewarding times to be involved in such research, as neuroscience first grappled with fundamental questions about how humans perceive the world.

Being a cognitive neuroscientist in the early days of the field satisfied many of my intellectual needs and wants. I could engage with age-old questions of mind and brain in a productive new way, asking how we come to know the visual world and how our brains implement or enable our minds. Of course, the deepest philosophical readings of these questions will never be settled with experiments, but to me it was nevertheless exciting to work on various empirical issues concerning mind–brain relations.

Perverse as it may sound, I also enjoyed the conceptual messiness of early cognitive neuroscience. In a mature science, there is a theoretical framework that guides us to the next questions to ask and provides models of the kinds of explanations that could count as answers. There is also a stable of methods recognized as able to deliver relevant evidence. With a framework, questions, and methods in place, the path of scientific progress is relatively clear. But, at the risk of sounding flippant, where's the fun in that? The cognitive neuroscience of the 1980s offered few of the conveniences of a mature science, but plenty of messy intellectual fun. Coming up with questions, deciding what kinds of data were relevant to answering them, parsing the array of possible answers, and examining the theoretical assumptions that underlay these decisions – these were necessary everyday thought processes for us.

But, for all the intellectual joy of grappling with issues of mind and brain in a developing science, I did feel that one thing was missing from my work life: social relevance.

Type
Chapter
Information
Scientists Making a Difference
One Hundred Eminent Behavioral and Brain Scientists Talk about Their Most Important Contributions
, pp. 20 - 23
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Evans, G. W., & Kim, P. (2013). Childhood poverty, chronic stress, self‐regulation, and coping. Child Development Perspectives, 7(1), 43–48.Google Scholar
Farah, M. J., Shera, D. M., Savage, J. H., Betancourt, L., Giannetta, J. M., Brodsky, N. L., … & Hurt, H. (2006). Childhood poverty: Specific associations with neurocognitive development. Brain Research, 1110(1), 166–174.Google Scholar
Hackman, D. A., & Farah, M. J. (2009). Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(2), 65–73.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×