Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-v5vhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-22T06:58:27.320Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

15 - Religion and Prejudice

from Part II - Prejudice in Specific Domains

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 November 2016

Ben K. L. Ng
Affiliation:
University of Kentucky
Will M. Gervais
Affiliation:
University of Kentucky
Chris G. Sibley
Affiliation:
University of Auckland
Fiona Kate Barlow
Affiliation:
University of Queensland
Get access

Summary

From a religious point of view, if God had thought homosexuality is a sin, he would not have created gay people.

Howard Dean, Former Governor of Vermont

If a man has sexual relations with a man as one does with a woman, both of them have done what is detestable. They are to be put to death; their blood will be on their own heads.

Leviticus 20:13, New International Version

A woman helps feed and clothe poor children in the slums of India: How likely is it that her religion motivated her to do so? A man votes against marriage equality for gay men and lesbians: How likely is it that his religion motivated him to do so? As Allport (1954) noted more than half a century ago, religion has a paradoxical relationship with prejudice. In his words, “[religion] makes and unmakes prejudice.” In the more than six decades since Allport made this observation, social psychologists have tried to pin down the precise relationship between religion and prejudice.

Early work in this area attempted to fractionate religion into distinct components that respectively predicted more and less prejudice. In this view, some “core” element of religion may predict tolerance, while other aspects of religion (or nonreligious constructs that tend to nonetheless correlate with religiosity) might engender outgroup enmity. This tradition has a long history, indeed, as William James (1902) noted:

The plain fact is that men's minds are built, as has been often said, in water-tight compartments. Religious after a fashion, they yet have many other things in them beside their religion, and unholy entanglements and associations inevitably obtain. The basenesses so commonly charged to religion's account are thus, almost all of them, not chargeable at all to religion proper, but rather to religion's wicked practical partner, the spirit of corporate dominion. And the bigotries are most of them in their turn chargeable to religion's wicked intellectual partner, the spirit of dogmatic dominion, the passion for laying down the law in the form of an absolutely closed-in theoretic system.

This search for “religion proper” and the unfortunate “wicked practical partners” of religiosity dominated research on religion and prejudice for decades.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alexander, R. D. (1979). Darwinism and human affairs. Seattle: University of Washington Press.
Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Allport, G. W. (1966). The religious context of prejudice. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 447–457.
Allport, G. W., & Ross, J. M. (1967). Personal religious orientation and prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5(4), 432.Google Scholar
Altemeyer, B. (1988). Enemies of freedom: Understanding right-wing authoritarianism. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Altemeyer, B., & Hunsberger, B. (1992). Authoritarianism, religious fundamentalism, quest, and prejudice. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 2(2), 113–133.Google Scholar
Altemeyer, B., & Hunsberger, B. (2005). Fundamentalism and authoritarianism. In Paloutzian, R. F. & Park, C. L. (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of religion and spirituality (pp. 378–393). New York: Guilford Press.
AtheistBerlin, . (2012). USPS Discrimination Against Atheism? from www.atheistberlin.com/atheist/?study
Atran, S. (2003). Genesis of suicide terrorism. Science, 299(5612), 1534–1539.Google Scholar
Atran, S., & Norenzayan, A. (2004). Religion's evolutionary landscape: Counterintuition, commitment, compassion, communion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(06), 713–730.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Barrett, J. L. (2000). Exploring the natural foundations of religion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(1), 29–34.Google Scholar
Barrett, J. L. (2004). Why would anyone believe in God?: Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press.
Barrett, J. L., & Keil, F. C. (1996). Conceptualizing a nonnatural entity: Anthropomorphism in God concepts. Cognitive Psychology, 31(3), 219–247.Google Scholar
Barrett, J. L., & Nyhof, M. A. (2001). Spreading non-natural concepts: The role of intuitive conceptual structures in memory and transmission of cultural materials. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 1(1), 69–100.Google Scholar
Bassett, R. L., Hodak, E., Allen, J., Bartos, D., Grastorf, J., Sittig, L., & Strong, J. (2000). Homonegative Christians: Loving the sinner but hating the sin. Journal of Psychology and Christianity, 19(3), 258–269.Google Scholar
Batson, C. D. (1976). Religion as prosocial: Agent or double agent? Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 29–45.
Batson, C. D., Schoenrade, P., & Ventis, W. L. (1993). Religion and the individual: A social-psychological perspective. New York: Oxford University Press.
Batson, C. D., & Ventis, W. L. (1982). The religious experience: A social-psychological perspective. New York: Oxford University Press.
Bering, J. M., & Johnson, D. D. (2005). “O Lord … You perceive my thoughts from afar”: Recursiveness and the evolution of supernatural agency. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 5(1), 118–142.Google Scholar
Blogowska, J., & Saroglou, V. (2011). Religious fundamentalism and limited prosociality as a function of the target. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 50(1), 44–60.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (2007). Religion is natural. Developmental Science, 10(1), 147–151.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of religious thought. New York: Basic Books.
Boyer, P. (2003). Religious thought and behaviour as by-products of brain function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 119–124.Google Scholar
Brandt, M. J., & Reyna, C. (2010). The role of prejudice and the need for closure in religious fundamentalism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 715–725.Google Scholar
Brandt, M. J., & Reyna, C. (2014). To love or hate thy neighbor: The role of authoritarianism and traditionalism in explaining the link between fundamentalism and racial prejudice. Political Psychology, 35(2), 207–223.Google Scholar
Brewer, M. B., & Brown, R. J. (1998). Intergroup relations. In Gilbert, D. T., Fiske, S. T., & Lindzey, G. (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (ed., pp. 554–594). Boston: McGraw-Hill.
Choi, J.-K., & Bowles, S. (2007). The coevolution of parochial altruism and war. Science, 318(5850), 636–640.Google Scholar
Cottrell, C. A., & Neuberg, S. L. (2005). Different emotional reactions to different groups: A sociofunctional threat-based approach to “prejudice.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(5), 770.Google Scholar
Cottrell, C. A., Richards, D. A., & Nichols, A. L. (2010). Predicting policy attitudes from general prejudice versus specific intergroup emotions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(2), 247–254.Google Scholar
De Botton, A. (2012). Religion for atheists: A non-believer's guide to the uses of religion. London: Penguin.
Donahue, M. J. (1985). Intrinsic and extrinsic religiousness: The empirical research. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 418–423.
Duckitt, J. (2001). A dual-process cognitive-motivational theory of ideology and prejudice. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 41–113.Google Scholar
Duckitt, J., Wagner, C., Du Plessis, I., & Birum, I. (2002). The psychological bases of ideology and prejudice: Testing a dual process model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(1), 75.Google Scholar
Duckitt, J. (2006). Differential effects of right wing authoritarianism and social dominance orientation on outgroup attitudes and their mediation by threat from and competitiveness to outgroups. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(5), 684–696.Google Scholar
Duckitt, J., & Sibley, C. G. (2007). Right wing authoritarianism, social dominance orientation and the dimensions of generalized prejudice. European Journal of Personality, 21(2), 113–130.Google Scholar
Duckitt, J., & Sibley, C. G. (2010). Personality, ideology, prejudice, and politics: A dual-process motivational model. Journal of Personality, 78(6), 1861–1894.Google Scholar
Edgell, P., Gerteis, J., & Hartmann, D. (2006). Atheists as “other”: Moral boundaries and cultural membership in American society. American Sociological Review, 71(2), 211–234.Google Scholar
FBI. (2011). FBI Releases 2011 Hate Crime Statistics [Press release]. Retrieved from www.fbi.gov/news/pressrel/press-releases/fbi-releases-2011-hate-crime-statistics
Fessler, D. M., & Navarrete, C. D. (2004). Third-party attitudes toward sibling incest: Evidence for Westermarck's hypotheses. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25(5), 277–294.Google Scholar
Finlay, B., & Walther, C. S. (2003). The relation of religious affiliation, service attendance, and other factors to homophobic attitudes among university students. Review of Religious Research, 370–393.
Fisher, R. D., Derison, D., Polley, C. F., Cadman, J., & Johnston, D. (1994). Religiousness, religious orientation, and attitudes towards gays and lesbians. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24(7), 614–630.Google Scholar
Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 358(1431), 459–473. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1218Google Scholar
Gallup. (2009a). In U.S., religious prejudice stronger against Muslims. Retrieved from www.gallup.com/poll/125312/religious-prejudice-stronger-against-muslims.aspx
Gallup. (2009b). Religious perceptions in America with an in-depth analysis of U.S. attitudes toward Muslims and Islam. Retrieved from www.clubmadrid.org/img/secciones/SSP_MWF_WorldReligion_Report_en-US_final.pdf
Gervais, W. M. (2013a). Religious cognition. In Saroglou, V. (Ed.), Religion, personality, and social behavior (pp. 71–95). New York: Psychology Press.
Gervais, W. M. (2013b). Perceiving minds and gods: How mind perception enables, constrains, and is triggered by belief in gods. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(4), 380–394.Google Scholar
Gervais, W. M. (2014). Everything is permitted? People intuitively judge immorality as representative of atheists. PloS One, 9(4), e92302.Google Scholar
Gervais, W. M., & Norenzayan, A. (2012). Reminders of secular authority reduce believers’ distrust of atheists. Psychological Science, 23, 483–491.Google Scholar
Gervais, W. M., Shariff, A. F., & Norenzayan, A. (2011). Do you believe in atheists? Distrust is central to anti-atheist prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(6), 1189.Google Scholar
Gervais, W. M., Willard, A. K., Norenzayan, A., & Henrich, J. (2011). The cultural transmission of faith: Why innate intuitions are necessary, but insufficient, to explain religious belief. Religion, 41(3), 389–410.Google Scholar
Ginges, J., Atran, S., Medin, D., & Shikaki, K. (2007). Sacred bounds on rational resolution of violent political conflict. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(18), 7357–7360.Google Scholar
Ginges, J., Hansen, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2009). Religion and support for suicide attacks. Psychological Science, 20(2), 224–230.Google Scholar
Graham, J., & Haidt, J. (2010). Beyond beliefs: Religions bind individuals into moral communities. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14(1), 140–150. doi: 10.1177/1088868309353415Google Scholar
Guthrie, S. (1993). Faces in the clouds. New York: Oxford University Press.
Haidt, J., & Graham, J. (2007). When morality opposes justice: Conservatives have moral intuitions that liberals may not recognize. Social Justice Research, 20(1), 98–116.Google Scholar
Haidt, J., McCauley, C., & Rozin, P. (1994). Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: A scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors. Personality and Individual Differences, 16(5), 701–713.Google Scholar
Henrich, J. (2009). The evolution of costly displays, cooperation and religion: Credibility enhancing displays and their implications for cultural evolution. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30(4), 244–260.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22(3), 165–196.Google Scholar
Herek, G. M. (1987). Religious orientation and prejudice: A comparison of racial and sexual attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13(1), 34–44.Google Scholar
Herek, G. M., & Capitanio, J. P. (1996). “Some of my best friends”: Intergroup contact, concealable stigma, and heterosexuals’ attitudes toward gay men and lesbians. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 412–424.Google Scholar
Hitchens, C. (2008). God is not great: How religion poisons everything. New York: Twelve.
Hoge, R., & Carroll, J. W. (1973). Religiosity and prejudice in northern and southern churches. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 181–197.
Hood, R. W., Hill, P. C., & Williamson, W. P. (2005). The psychology of religious fundamentalism. New York: Guilford Press.
Hunsberger, B. (1996). Religious fundamentalism, right-wing authoritarianism, and hostility toward homosexuals in non-Christian religious groups. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 6(1), 39–49.Google Scholar
Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D. A., & Bloom, P. (2009). Conservatives are more easily disgusted than liberals. Cognition and Emotion, 23(4), 714–725.Google Scholar
Insko, C. A., Pinkley, R. L., Harring, K., & Holton, B. (1987). Minimal conditions for real groups: Mere categorization or competitive between category behavior. Representative Research in Social Psychology, 17, 5–36.Google Scholar
Irons, W. (2001). Religion as a hard-to-fake sign of commitment. Evolution and the capacity for commitment, 292–309.
James, W. (1902). The varieties of religious experience. New York: Random House.
Johnson, D. D. (2005). God's punishment and public goods. Human Nature, 16(4), 410–446.Google Scholar
Johnson, D. D., & Bering, J. M. (2006). Hand of God, mind of man: Punishment and cognition in the evolution of cooperation. Evolutionary Psychology, 4(1), 219–233.Google Scholar
Johnson, D. D., & Krüger, O. (2004). The good of wrath: Supernatural punishment and the evolution of cooperation. Political Theology, 5(2), 159–176.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. K., Labouff, J. P., Rowatt, W. C., Patock-Peckham, J. A., & Carlisle, R. D. (2012). Facets of right-wing authoritarianism mediate the relationship between religious fundamentalism and attitudes toward Arabs and African Americans. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 51(1), 128–142.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. K., Rowatt, W. C., & LaBouff, J. P. (2012). Religiosity and prejudice revisited: In-group favoritism, out-group derogation, or both? Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 4(2), 154–168. doi: DOI 10.1037/a0025107Google Scholar
Jones, J. M. (2007). Some Americans reluctant to vote for Mormon, 72-year-old presidential candidates; Strong support for black, women, Catholic candidates. Retrieved from www.gallup.com/poll/26611/some-americans-reluctant-vote-mormon-72yearold-presidential-candidates.aspx
Katz, M. H. (2005). The study of Islamic ritual and the meaning of wuḍūʾ. Der Islam, 82(1), 106–145.Google Scholar
Kay, A. C., Gaucher, D., Napier, J. L., Callan, M. J., & Laurin, K. (2008). God and the government: Testing a compensatory control mechanism for the support of external systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(1), 18.Google Scholar
Kay, A. C., Shepherd, S., Blatz, C. W., Chua, S. N., & Galinsky, A. D. (2010). For God (or) country: The hydraulic relation between government instability and belief in religious sources of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(5), 725.Google Scholar
Kelemen, D. (1999a). Function, goals and intention: Children's teleological reasoning about objects. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(12), 461–468.Google Scholar
Kelemen, D. (1999b). Why are rocks pointy? Children's preference for teleological explanations of the natural world. Developmental Psychology, 35(6), 1440.Google Scholar
Kelemen, D. (2004). Are children “intuitive theists”? Reasoning about purpose and design in nature. Psychological Science, 15(5), 295–301.Google Scholar
Kelemen, D., & Rosset, E. (2009). The human function compunction: Teleological explanation in adults. Cognition, 111(1), 138–143.Google Scholar
Kelemen, D., Rottman, J., & Seston, R. (2013). Professional physical scientists display tenacious teleological tendencies: Purpose-based reasoning as a cognitive default. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(4), 1074.Google Scholar
Krebs, D. L., & Denton, K. (1997). Social illusions and self-deception: The evolution of biases in person perception. Evolutionary Social Psychology, 21–48.
Kurzban, R., & Leary, M. R. (2001). Evolutionary origins of stigmatization: The functions of social exclusion. Psychological Bulletin, 127(2), 187.Google Scholar
Kurzban, R., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2001). Can race be erased? Coalitional computation and social categorization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(26), 15387–15392.Google Scholar
Laythe, B., Finkel, D. G., Bringle, R. G., & Kirkpatrick, L. A. (2002). Religious fundamentalism as a predictor of prejudice: A two-component model. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 41(4), 623–635.Google Scholar
Lieberman, D., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2007). The architecture of human kin detection. Nature, 445(7129), 727–731.Google Scholar
Matthews, M., & Levin, S. (2012). Testing a dual process model of prejudice: Assessment of group threat perceptions and emotions. Motivation and Emotion, 36(4), 564–574.Google Scholar
Mavor, K. I., Louis, W. R., & Laythe, B. (2011). Religion, prejudice, and authoritarianism: Is RWA a boon or bane to the psychology of religion? Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 50(1), 22–43.Google Scholar
McFarland, S. G. (1989). Religious orientations and the targets of discrimination. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 324–336.
Neuberg, S. L., & Cottrell, C. A. (2008). Managing the threats and opportunities afforded by human sociality. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 12(1), 63.Google Scholar
Neuberg, S. L., Kenrick, D. T., & Schaller, M. (2010). Evolutionary social psychology. In Fiske, S. T., Gilbert, D. T., & Lindzey, G. (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (ed., Vol. 2, pp. 761–796). New York: Wiley.
Neuberg, S. L., Kenrick, D. T., & Schaller, M. (2011). Human threat management systems: Self-protection and disease avoidance. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(4), 1042–1051.Google Scholar
Neuberg, S. L., Smith, D. M., & Asher, T. (2000). Why people stigmatize: Toward a biocultural framework. In Heatherton, T., Kleck, R., Hebl, M. & Hull, J. (Eds.), The social psychology of stigma (pp. 31–61). New York: Guilford Press.
Norenzayan, A., & Gervais, W. M. (2012). The cultural evolution of religion. Creating consilience: Integrating the sciences and the humanities, 243–265.
Norenzayan, A., & Gervais, W. M. (2013). The origins of religious disbelief. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(1), 20–25.Google Scholar
Norenzayan, A., & Gervais, W. M. (2015). Secular rule of law erodes believers’ political intolerance of atheists. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 5(1), 3–14.Google Scholar
Norenzayan, A., Shariff, A., Gervais, W., Willard, A., Slingerland, E., & Henrich, J. (2016). The cultural evolution of prosocial religions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39.Google Scholar
Norenzayan, A., & Shariff, A. F. (2008). The origin and evolution of religious prosociality. Science, 322(5898), 58–62.Google Scholar
Pargament, K. I. (2001). The psychology of religion and coping: Theory, research, practice. New York: Guilford Press.
Pew, . (2007). Chapter 3. Views of religion and morality. Retrieved from www.pewglobal.org/2007/10/04/chapter-3-views-of-religion-and-morality/
Pew, . (2010). Public remains conflicted over Islam. Retrieved from www.pewforum.org/files/2010/08/Islam-mosque-full-report.pdf
Pew, . (2014). For 2016 hopefuls, Washington experience could do more harm than good. Retrieved from www.people-press.org/2014/05/19/for-2016-hopefuls-washington-experience-could-do-more-harm-than-good/
Pew, . (2015). America's changing religious landscape. Retrieved from www.pewforum.org/2015/05/12/americas-changing-religious-landscape/
Phelan, J. C., Link, B. G., & Dovidio, J. F. (2008). Stigma and prejudice: One animal or two? Social Science & Medicine, 67(3), 358–367.Google Scholar
Preston, J. L., & Ritter, R. S. (2013). Different effects of religion and God on prosociality with the ingroup and outgroup. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 1471–1483.Google Scholar
Preston, J. L., Ritter, R. S., & Ivan Hernandez, J. (2010). Principles of religious prosociality: A review and reformulation. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4(8), 574–590.Google Scholar
PRRI. (2010). Survey | Less than 1-in-5 give America's places of worship high marks on handling issue of homosexuality. Retrieved from http://publicreligion.org/research/2010/10/less-than-1-in-5-give-americas-places-of-worship-high-marks-on-handling-issue-of-homosexuality/#.VVuo7PlViko
Radcliffe, C. (2013). UN campaign reveals shocking, depressing gay Google auto-complete function. Retrieved from www.huffingtonpost.com/charles-radcliffe/google-auto-complete-gays_b_4153399.html
Raiya, H. A., Pargament, K. I., Mahoney, A., & Trevino, K. (2008). When Muslims are perceived as a religious threat: Examining the connection between desecration, religious coping, and anti-Muslim attitudes. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 30(4), 311–325.Google Scholar
Rendell, L., Fogarty, L., Hoppitt, W. J. E., Morgan, T. J. H., Webster, M. M., & Laland, K. N. (2011). Cognitive culture: Theoretical and empirical insights into social learning strategies. Trends in Cognitive Science, 15(2), 68–76.Google Scholar
Robert, B., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Rothschild, Z. K., Abdollahi, A., & Pyszczynski, T. (2009). Does peace have a prayer? The effect of mortality salience, compassionate values, and religious fundamentalism on hostility toward out-groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(4), 816–827.Google Scholar
Rowatt, W. C., Franklin, L. M., & Cotton, M. (2005). Patterns and personality correlates of implicit and explicit attitudes toward Christians and Muslims. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 44(1), 29–43.Google Scholar
Rowatt, W. C., Tsang, J. A., Kelly, J., LaMartina, B., McCullers, M., & McKinley, A. (2006). Associations between religious personality dimensions and implicit homosexual prejudice. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 45(3), 397–406.Google Scholar
Rozin, P., Millman, L., & Nemeroff, C. (1986). Operation of the laws of sympathetic magic in disgust and other domains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(4), 703.Google Scholar
Saroglou, V. (2011). Believing, bonding, behaving, and belonging: The Big Four religious dimensions and cultural variation. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42(8), 1320–1340.Google Scholar
Saroglou, V., & Galand, P. (2004). Identities, values, and religion: A study among Muslim, other immigrant, and native Belgian young adults after the 9/11 attacks. Identity, 4(2), 97–132.Google Scholar
Saroglou, V., Pichon, I., Trompette, L., Verschueren, M., & Dernelle, R. (2005). Prosocial behavior and religion: New evidence based on projective measures and peer ratings. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 44(3), 323–348.Google Scholar
Schaller, M., & Neuberg, S. L. (2008). Intergroup prejudices and intergroup conflicts. In Crawford, C. & Krebs, D. L. (Eds.), Foundations of evolutionary psychology: Ideas, issues, and applications (pp. 399–412). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Schaller, M., Park, J., & Faulkner, J. (2003). Prehistoric dangers and contemporary prejudices. European Review of Social Psychology, 14(1), 105–137.Google Scholar
Shen, M. J., Yelderman, L. A., Haggard, M. C., & Rowatt, W. C. (2013). Disentangling the belief in God and cognitive rigidity/flexibility components of religiosity to predict racial and value-violating prejudice: A Post-Critical Belief Scale analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 54(3), 389–395.Google Scholar
Sheriff, M. (1966). Group conflict and cooperation: London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Shweder, R., Much, N., Mahapatra, M., & Park, L. (1997). The “Big Three” of morality (autonomy, community, divinity) and the “Big Three” explanations of suffering. In Brandt, A. & Rozin, P. (Eds.), Morality and health (pp. 119–169). New York: Routledge.
Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (2001). Social dominance: An intergroup theory of social hierarchy and oppression. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Sides, J., & Gross, K. (2013). Stereotypes of Muslims and support for the war on terror. The Journal of Politics, 75(03), 583–598.Google Scholar
Simpson, J. A. (2007). Psychological foundations of trust. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(5), 264–268. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00517.xGoogle Scholar
Sober, E., & Wilson, D. S. (1999). Unto others: The evolution and psychology of unselfish behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Sosis, R. (2006). Religious behaviors, badges, and bans: Signaling theory and the evolution of religion, in McNamara, P. (Ed.), Where God and science meet: How brain and evolutionary studies alter our understanding of religion, 1, 61–86, Bridgeport, CT: Praeger Publishing.
Sosis, R., & Alcorta, C. (2003). Signaling, solidarity, and the sacred: The evolution of religious behavior. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 12(6), 264–274.Google Scholar
Sperber, D., & Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining culture. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.
Tajfel, H., Billig, M. G., Bundy, R. P., & Flament, C. (1971). Social categorization and intergroup behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1(2), 149–178.Google Scholar
Tan, J. H., & Vogel, C. (2008). Religion and trust: An experimental study. Journal of Economic Psychology, 29(6), 832–848.Google Scholar
Terrizzi, J. A., Shook, N. J., & Ventis, W. L. (2010). Disgust: A predictor of social conservatism and prejudicial attitudes toward homosexuals. Personality and Individual Differences, 49(6), 587–592.Google Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1988). The evolution of war and its cognitive foundations. Institute for Evolutionary Studies Technical Report, 88(1), 1–15.Google Scholar
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1983). Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: The conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychological Review, 90(4), 293–315. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.90.4.293Google Scholar
Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., & Griskevicius, V. (2009). Microbes, mating, and morality: Individual differences in three functional domains of disgust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(1), 103–122. doi: 10.1037/a0015474Google Scholar
van der Slik, F. W., & Konig, R. P. (2006). RESEARCH: Orthodox, humanitarian, and science-inspired belief in relation to prejudice against Jews, Muslims, and ethnic minorities: The content of one's belief does matter. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 16(2), 113–126.Google Scholar
Voas, D. (2007). Surveys of behaviour, beliefs and affiliation: Micro-quantitative. In Beckford, J. & Demerath, N. III (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of the sociology of religion (pp. 144–166). London: Sage.
Whitley, B. E. Jr. (2009). Religiosity and attitudes toward lesbians and gay men: A meta-analysis. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 19(1), 21–38.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. S., & Sober, E. (1994). Reintroducing group selection to the human behavioral sciences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17(04), 585–608.Google Scholar
Zuckerman, P. (2007). Atheism: Contemporary numbers and patterns. In Martin, M. (Ed.), The Cambridge companion to atheism (pp. 47–66). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×