Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ttngx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-30T18:31:44.529Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 December 2013

Katja Liebal
Affiliation:
Freie Universität Berlin
Bridget M. Waller
Affiliation:
University of Portsmouth
Anne M. Burrows
Affiliation:
Duquesne University, Pittsburgh
Katie E. Slocombe
Affiliation:
University of York
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Primate Communication
A Multimodal Approach
, pp. 238 - 280
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2013

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abbott, D. H. (1984). Behavioral and physiological suppression of fertility in subordinate marmoset monkeys. American Journal of Primatology, 6(3), 169–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abbott, D. H., Barrett, J. and George, L. M. (1993). Comparative aspects of the social suppression of reproduction in female marmosets and tamarins. In Rylands, A. B. (ed.), Marmosets and Tamarins: Systematics, Behaviour, and Ecology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 152–163.Google Scholar
Abbott, D. H., Saltzman, W., Schultz-Darken, N. J. and Smith, T. E. (1997). Specific neuroendocrine mechanisms not involving generalized stress mediate social regulation of female reproduction in cooperatively breeding marmoset monkeys. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 807(1), 219–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aboitiz, F., García, R. R., Bosman, C. and Brunetti, E. (2006). Cortical memory mechanisms and language origins. Brain and Language, 98(1), 40–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aitken, P. G. (1981). Cortical control of conditioned and spontaneous vocal behavior in rhesus monkeys. Brain and Language, 13(1), 171–184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alcock, J. (1984). Animal Behavior. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.Google Scholar
Allison, T., Puce, A. and McCarthy, G. (2000). Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(7), 267–278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altmann, J. (1974). Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour, 49, 227–267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altmann, S. A. (1962). Social behavior of anthropoid primates: analysis of recent concepts. In Bliss, E. L. (ed.), Roots of Behavior. New York: Harper, pp. 277–285.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R., Kuroshima, H., Hattori, Y. and Fujita, K. (2010). Flexibility in the use of requesting gestures in squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). American Journal of Primatology, 72(8), 707–714.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J.R., Kuroshima, H., Kuwahata, H., Fujita, K., Vick, S.-J. (2001). Training squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) to deceive: acquisition and analysis of behavior toward cooperative and competitive trainers. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 115(3), 282–293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, J. R., Kuwahata, H. and Fujita, K. (2007). Gaze alternation during ‘pointing’ by squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus)?Animal Cognition, 10(2), 267–271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andrew, R. J. (1963). Evolution of facial expression. Science, 142(3595), 1034–1041.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ankel-Simons, F. (2000). Primate Anatomy (2nd edn). San Diego, CA: Academia Press.Google Scholar
Arbib, M. A. (2005). From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(2), 105–124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arbib, M. A., Liebal, K. and Pika, S. (2008). Primate vocalization, gesture, and the evolution of human language. Current Anthropology, 49(6), 1053–1076.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnold, K. and Zuberbühler, K. (2006). Language evolution: Semantic combinations in primate calls. Nature, 441(7091), 303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnold, K. and Zuberbühler, K. (2008). Meaningful call combinations in a non-human primate. Current Biology, 18(5), R202–R203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, K. and Zuberbühler, K. (2012). Call combinations in monkeys: Compositional or idiomatic expressions?Brain and Language, 120(3), 303–309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnold, K., Pohlner, Y. and Zuberbühler, K. (2008). A forest monkey’s alarm call series to predator models. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 62(4), 549–559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, K., Pohlner, Y. and Zuberbühler, K. (2011). Not words but meanings? Alarm calling behaviour in a forest guenon. In Sommer, V. and Ross, C. (eds), Primates of Gashaka, Vol. 35. New York: Springer, pp. 437–468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atsalis, S. (2008). A Natural History of the Brown Mouse Lemur. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Aujard, F. (1997). Effect of vomeronasal organ removal on male socio-sexual responses to female in a prosimian primate (Microcebus murinus). Physiology and Behavior, 62(5), 1003–1008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, L. A. and Teleki, G. (1976). Patterns of gibbon behavior on Hall’s Island, Bermuda: a preliminary ethogram for Hylobates lar. In D. M. Rumbaugh (ed.) Gibbon and Siamang, vol. 4, pp. 21–105.Google Scholar
Bamshad, M. and Albers, E. (1996). Neural circuitry controlling vasopressin-stimulated scent marking in Syrian hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus). The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 369(2), 252–263.3.0.CO;2-2>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bard, K. A. (1992). Intentional behavior and intentional communication in young free-ranging orangutans. Child Development, 63(5), 1186–1197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bard, K. A. (2003). Development of emotional expressions in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1000(1), 88–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bard, K. A. (2007). Neonatal imitation in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) tested with two paradigms. Animal Cognition, 10(2), 233–242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S. (1992). How monkeys do things with ‘words’. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15(1), 148–149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barraclough, N. E. and Perrett, D. I. (2011). From single cells to social perception. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1571), 1739–1752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barraclough, N. E., Xiao, D., Baker, C. I., Oram, M. W. and Perrett, D. I. (2005). Integration of visual and auditory information by superior temporal sulcus neurons responsive to the sight of actions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(3), 377–391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, J., Abbott, D. H. and George, L. M. (1993). Sensory cues and the suppression of reproduction in subordinate female marmoset monkeys, Callithrix jacchus. Journal of Reproduction and Fertility, 97(1), 301–310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartlett, T. Q. (2008). The Gibbons of Khao Yai: Seasonal Variation in Behavior and Ecology. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Barton, R. A. (2006). Olfactory evolution and behavioral ecology in primates. American Journal of Primatology, 68(6), 545–558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barton, R. A. and Harvey, P. H. (2000). Mosaic evolution of brain structure in mammals. Nature, 405(6790), 1055–1057.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L. and Volterra, V. (1979). The Emergence of Symbols: Cognition and Communication in Infancy. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Camaioni, L. and Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development, 21(3), 205–226.Google Scholar
Bauer, H. R. and Philip, M. M. (1983). Facial and vocal individual recognition in the common chimpanzee. The Psychological Record, 33(2), 161–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bearder, S. K. (1987). Lorises, bush babies, and tarsiers: Diverse societies in solitary foragers. In Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W. and Struhsaker, T. T. (eds), Primate Societies. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press, pp. 12–24.Google Scholar
Beauchamp, G. K. and Yamazaki, K. (2005). Individual differences and the chemical senses. Chemical Senses, 30(suppl. 1), i6–i9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bekoff, M. (1974). Social play and play-soliciting by infant canids. American Zoologist, 14(1), 323–340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belcher, A., Epple, G., Küderling, I. and Smith, A. B. (1988). Volatile components of scent material from cotton-top tamarin (Saguinus o. oedipus). Journal of Chemical Ecology, 14(5), 1367–1384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beletsky, L. D. (1983). Aggressive and pair bond maintenance songs of female red-winged blackbirds (Agelaius phoeniceus). Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 62(1), 47–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benga, O. (2005). Intentional communication and the anterior cingulate cortex. Interaction Studies, 6(2), 201–221.Google Scholar
Benz, J. J. (1993). Food-elicited vocalizations in golden lion tamarins: design-features for representational communication. Animal Behaviour, 45(3), 443–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benz, J. J., Leger, D. W. and French, J. A. (1992). Relation between food preference and food-elicited vocalizations in golden lion tamarins (Leontopithecus rosalia). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 106(2), 142–149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bergman, T. (2013). Speech-like vocalized lip-smacking in geladas. Current Biology, 23(7), R268–269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bering, J. M. (2004). A critical review of the ‘enculturation hypothesis’: the effects of human rearing on great ape social cognition. Animal Cognition, 7(4), 201–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berkson, G. and Becker, J. D. (1975). Facial expressions and social responsiveness of blind monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 84(5), 519.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bickerton, D. (1992). Language and Species. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Blaschke, M. and Ettlinger, G. (1987). Pointing as an act of social communication by monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 35(5), 1520–1523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloch, J. I. and Silcox, M. T. (2006). Cranial anatomy of the Paleocene plesiadapiform Carpolestes simpsoni (Mammalia, Primates) using ultra high-resolution X-ray computed tomography, and the relationships of plesiadapiforms to Euprimates. Journal of Human Evolution, 50(1), 1–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloch, J. I., Silcox, M. T., Boyer, D. M. and Sargis, E. J. (2007). New Paleocene skeletons and the relationship of plesiadapiforms to crown-clade primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(4), 1159–1164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bodamer, M. D. and Gardner, R. A. (2002). How cross-fostered chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) initiate and maintain conversations. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 116(1), 12–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolwig, N. (1964). Facial expression in primates with remarks on a parallel development in certain carnivores (a preliminary report on work in progress). Behaviour, 22(3/4), 167–192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonnie, K. E. and de Waal, F. B. M. (2006). Affiliation promotes the transmission of a social custom: handclasp grooming among captive chimpanzees. Primates, 47(1), 27–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bouchet, H., Blois-Heulin, C. and Lemasson, A. (2012). Age- and sex-specific patterns of vocal behavior in De Brazza’s monkeys (Cercopithecus neglectus). American Journal of Primatology, 74(1), 12–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bouchet, H., Blois-Heulin, C., Pellier, A. S., Zuberbühler, K. and Lemasson, A. (2012). Acoustic variability and individual distinctiveness in the vocal repertoire of red-capped mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126(1), 45–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bout, N. and Thierry, B. (2005). Peaceful meaning for the silent bared-teeth displays of mandrills. International Journal of Primatology, 26(6), 1215–1228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradbury, J. W. and Vehrencamp, S. L. (1998). Principles of Animal Communication. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.Google Scholar
Brentano, F. (1874 [1973]). Psychologie vom empirischen Standpunkt. Hamburg: Felix Meiner Verlag.Google Scholar
Bressem, J. (2008). Notating gestures: proposal for a form based notation system of coverbal gestures. Unpublished manuscript.
Brosch, M. and Scheich, H. (2003). Neural representation of sound patterns in the auditory cortex of monkeys. In Ghazanfar, A. A. (ed.), Primate Audition: Ethology and Neurobiology. Boca Raton: CRC Press, pp. 151–175.Google Scholar
Brown, C. H., Gomez, R. and Waser, P. M. (1995). Old World monkey vocalizations: adaptation to the local habitat?Animal Behaviour, 50(4), 945–961.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1981). Intention in the structure of action and interaction. In Lipsitt, L. (ed.), Advances in Infancy Research, Vol. 1. Norwood, NJ: Ablex, pp. 41–56.Google Scholar
Bullinger, A. F., Zimmermann, F., Kaminski, J. and Tomasello, M. (2011). Different social motives in the gestural communication of chimpanzees and human children. Developmental Science, 14(1), 58–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burling, R. (2005). The Talking Ape: How Language Evolved. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Burrows, A. M. (2008). The facial expression musculature in primates and its evolutionary significance. Bioessays, 30(3), 212–225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burrows, A. M. and Smith, T. D. (2003). Muscles of facial expression in Otolemur, with a comparison to Lemuroidea. Anatomical Record Part A: Discoveries in Molecular Cellular and Evolutionary Biology, 274(1), 827–836.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burrows, A. M., Diogo, R., Waller, B. M., Bonar, C. J. and Liebal, K. (2011). Evolution of the muscles of facial expression in a monogamous ape: Evaluating the relative influences of ecological and phylogenetic factors in hylobatids. The Anatomical Record: Advances in Integrative Anatomy and Evolutionary Biology, 294(4), 645–663.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burrows, A. M., Waller, B. M. and Parr, L. A. (2009). Facial musculature in the rhesus macaque (Macaca mulatta): evolutionary and functional contexts with comparisons to chimpanzees and humans. Journal of Anatomy, 215(3), 320–334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burrows, A. M., Waller, B. M., Parr, L. A. and Bonar, C. J. (2006). Muscles of facial expression in the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes): descriptive, comparative and phylogenetic contexts. Journal of Anatomy, 208(2), 153–167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butterworth, G. (1998). What is special about pointing in babies? In Simion, F. and Butterworth, G. (eds), The Development of Sensory, Motor and Cognitive Capacities in Early Infancy: From Perception to Cognition. East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press, pp. 171–187.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W. and Bates, L. A. (2006). Why are animals cognitive?Current Biology, 16(12), R445–448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caeiro, C., Waller, B. M., Zimmerman, E., Burrows, A. M. and Davila-Ross, M. (2013). OrangFACS: a muscle-based facial movement coding system for orangutans (Pongo spp.). International Journal of Primatology, 34(1), 115–129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caine, N. G., Addington, R. L. and Windfelder, T. L. (1995). Factors affecting the rates of food calls given by red-bellied tamarins. Animal Behaviour, 50(1), 53–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Call, J. and Tomasello, M. (1994). Production and comprehension of referential pointing by orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108(4), 307–317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Call, J. and Tomasello, M. (eds). (2007). The Gestural Communication of Apes and Monkeys. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Call, J., Agnetta, B. and Tomasello, M. (2000). Cues that chimpanzees do and do not use to find hidden objects. Animal Cognition, 3(1), 23–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calvert, G. A. (2001). Crossmodal processing in the human brain: Insights from functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, 11(12), 1110–1123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, M. W. and de Waal, F. B. M. (2011). Ingroup-outgroup bias in contagious yawning by chimpanzees supports link to empathy. PLoS One, 6(4), e18283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Candiotti, A., Zuberbühler, K. and Lemasson, A. (2012a). Convergence and divergence in Diana monkey vocalizations. Biology Letters, 8(3), 382–385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Candiotti, A., Zuberbühler, K. and Lemasson, A. (2012b). Context-related call combinations in female Diana monkeys. Animal Cognition, 15(3), 327–339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Candland, D. K., Blumer, E. S. and Mumford, M. D. (1980). Urine as a communicator in a New World primate, Saimiri sciureus. Animal Learning and Behavior, 8(3), 468–480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cantalupo, C. and Hopkins, W. D. (2001). Asymmetric Broca’s area in great apes: a region of the ape brain is uncannily similar to one linked with speech in humans. Nature, 414(6863), 505–505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, C. R. (1940) A Field Study in Siam of the Behavior and Social Relations of the Gibbon (Hylobates lar). New York: The Johns Hopkins Press pp. 1–212.Google Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K. and Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63(4), 1–174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cartmill, E. A. and Byrne, R. W. (2007). Orangutans modify their gestural signalling according to their audience’s comprehension. Current Biology, 17(15), 1345–1348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cartmill, E. A. and Byrne, R. W. (2010). Semantics of primate gestures: intentional meanings of orangutan gestures. Animal Cognition, 13(6), 793–804.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cartmill, M. (1972). Arboreal adaptations and the origin of the order Primates. In Tuttle, R. H. (ed.), The Functional and Evolutionary Biology of Primates. Chicago, IL: Aldine-Atherton, pp. 97–122.Google Scholar
Cartmill, M. (1974). Rethinking primate origins. Science, 184(4135), 436–443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cartmill, M. (1992). New views on primate origins. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 1(3), 105–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cäsar, C., Byrne, R., Young, R. J. and Zuberbühler, K. (2012a). The alarm call system of wild black-fronted titi monkeys, Callicebus nigrifrons. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 66(5), 653–667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cäsar, C., Byrne, R. W., Hoppitt, W., Young, R. J. and Zuberbühler, K. (2012b). Evidence for semantic communication in titi monkey alarm calls. Animal Behaviour, 84(2), 405–411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catani, M. and Jones, D. K. (2005). Perisylvian language networks of the human brain. Annals of Neurology, 57(1), 8–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chalcraft, V. J. and Gardner, R. A. (2005). Cross-fostered chimpanzees modulate signs of American Sign Language. Gesture, 5(1–2), 107–132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chandrasekaran, C., Lemus, L., Trubanova, A., Gondan, M. and Ghazanfar, A. A. (2011). Monkeys and humans share a common computation for face/voice integration. PLoS Computational Biology, 7(9), e1002165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chapman, C. A. (1990). Association patterns of spider monkeys: the influence of ecology and sex on social organization. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 26(6), 409–414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charles-Dominique, P. (1977). Ecology and Behaviour of Nocturnal Primates. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Charles-Dominique, P. (1978). Solitary and gregarious prosimians: evolution of social structure in primates. In Chivers, D. J. and Joysey, K. A. (eds), Recent Advances in Primatology, Vol. 3. London: Academic Press, pp. 139–149.Google Scholar
Cheney, D. L. (1981). Inter-group encounters among free-ranging vervet monkeys. Folia Primatologica, 35(2–3), 124–146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (1985). Vervet monkey alarm calls: manipulation through shared information?Behaviour, 94(1–2), 150–166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (1988). Assessment of meaning and the detection of unreliable signals by vervet monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 36(2), 477–486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (1990). How Monkeys See the World: Inside the Mind of Another Species. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (2005). Constraints and preadaptations in the earliest stages of language evolution. Linguistic Review, 22(2–4), 135–159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheney, D. L. and Wrangham, R. W. (1987). Predation. In Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W. and Struhsaker, T. T. (eds), Primate Societies. Chicago, IL, US: University of Chicago Press, pp. 227–239.Google Scholar
Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M. and Palombit, R. (1996). The function and mechanisms underlying baboon ‘contact’ barks. Animal Behaviour, 52(3), 507–518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chevalier-Skolnikoff, S. (1973). Facial expression of emotion in nonhuman primates. In Ekman, P. (ed.), Darwin and Facial Expression: A Century of Research in Review. Oxford: Academic Press, pp. 11–89.Google Scholar
Chevalier-Skolnikoff, S. (1974). Ontogenetic development of communication patterns in stumptail monkeys (Macaca speciosa). American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 40(1), 133–133.Google Scholar
Chivers, D. J. (1976). Communication within and between family groups of siamang (Symphalangus syndactylus). Behaviour, 57(1–2), 116–135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1966). Cartesian Linguistics: A Chapter in the History of Rationalist Thought. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Christiansen, M. H. and Kirby, S. (2003). Language evolution: consensus and controversies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(7), 300–307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, A. B. (1982). Scent marks as social signals in Galago crassicaudatus I. Sex and reproductive status as factors in signals and responses. Journal of Chemical Ecology, 8(8), 1133–1151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarke, E., Reichard, U. H. and Zuberbühler, K. (2006). The syntax and meaning of wild gibbon songs. PLoS One, 1(1), e73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clarke, P. M. R., Barrett, L. and Henzi, S. P. (2009). What role do olfactory cues play in chacma baboon mating?American Journal of Primatology, 71(6), 493–502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clay, Z. and Zuberbühler, K. (2009). Food-associated calling sequences in bonobos. Animal Behaviour, 77(6), 1387–1396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clay, Z. and Zuberbühler, K. (2011a). Bonobos extract meaning from call sequences. PLoS One, 6(4), e18786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clay, Z. and Zuberbühler, K. (2011b). The structure of bonobo copulation calls during reproductive and non-reproductive sex. Ethology, 117(12), 1158–1169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cleveland, J. and Snowdon, C. T. (1982). The complex vocal repertoire of the adult cotton-top tamarin (Saguinus oedipus oedipus). Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 58(3), 231–270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clutton-Brock, T. and Janson, C. (2012). Primate socioecology at the crossroads: past, present, and future. Evolutionary Anthropology, 21(4), 136–150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clutton-Brock, T. H. and Harvey, P. H. (1977). Primate ecology and social organization. Journal of Zoology, 183(1), 1–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clutton-Brock, T. H., Brotherton, P. N. M., O’Riain, M. J. et al. (2000). Individual contributions to babysitting in a cooperative mongoose, Suricata suricatta. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 267(1440), 301–305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, C. and Haun, D. B. M. (2013). The development of tag-based cooperation via a socially acquired trait. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(3), 230–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, E. (2012). The evolution of tag-based cooperation in humans. Current Anthropology, 53(5), 588–616.CrossRef
Coleman, S. W., Patricelli, G. L. and Borgia, G. (2004). Variable female preferences drive complex male displays. Nature, 428(6984), 742–745.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colishaw, G. (1992). Song function in gibbons. Behaviour, 121(1–2), 131–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colonnesi, C., Stams, G. J. J. M., Koster, I. and Noom, M. J. (2010). The relation between pointing and language development: a meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 30(4), 352–366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colquhoun, I. C. (2011). A review and interspecific comparison of nocturnal and cathemeral Strepsirrhine primate olfactory behavioural ecology. International Journal of Zoology, 2011, 1–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conroy, G. C. (1990). Primate Evolution. New York: W.W. Norton and Company.Google Scholar
Cooper, B. G. and Goller, F. (2004). Multimodal signals: enhancement and constraint of song motor patterns by visual display. Science, 303(5657), 544–546.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corballis, M. C. (1992). The Lopsided Brain: Evolution of the Generative Mind. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Corballis, M. C. (2002). From Hand to Mouth, the Origins of Language. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Corballis, M. C. (2003). From mouth to hand: gesture, speech, and the evolution of right-handedness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26(2), 199–208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cords, M. (1987). Forest guenos and patas monkeys: male-male competition in one-male groups. In Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W. and Struhsaker, T. T. (eds), Primate Societies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, pp. 98–111.Google Scholar
Cords, M. (2002). When are there influxes in blue monkey groups? In Glenn, M. E. and Cords, M. (eds), The Guenons: Diversity and Adaptation in African Monkeys. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers, pp. 189–201.Google Scholar
Crockford, C. and Boesch, C. (2003). Context-specific calls in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus: analysis of barks. Animal Behaviour, 66(1), 115–125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crockford, C. and Boesch, C. (2005). Call combinations in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour, 142(4), 397–421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crockford, C., Herbinger, I., Vigilant, L. and Boesch, C. (2004). Wild chimpanzees produce group-specific calls: a case for vocal learning?Ethology, 110(3), 221–243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M., Mundry, R. and Zuberbühler, K. (2012). Wild chimpanzees inform ignorant group members of danger. Current Biology, 22(2), 142–146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cussins, A. (1992). Content, embodiment and objectivity: the theory of cognitive trails. Mind, 101(404), 651–688.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C. (1872). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davila-Ross, M., Allcock, B., Thomas, C. and Bard, K. A. (2011). Aping expressions? Chimpanzees produce distinct laugh types when responding to laughter of others. Emotion, 11(5), 1013–1020.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davila-Ross, M., J Owren, M. and Zimmermann, E. (2009). Reconstructing the evolution of laughter in great apes and humans. Current Biology, 19(13), 1106–1111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawkins, R. and Krebs, J. R. (1978). Animal signals: information or manipulation. In Krebs, J. R. and Davies, N. B. (eds), Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications, pp. 282–309.Google Scholar
De Boer, B., Sandler, W. and Kirby, S. (2012). New perspectives on duality of patterning: Introduction to the special issue. Language and Cognition, 4(4), 251–259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Marco, A. and Visalberghi, E. (2007). Facial displays in young tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): appearance, meaning, context and target. Folia Primatologica, 78(2), 118–137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Saussure, F. (1983). Course in General Linguistics. La Salle, IL: Open Court.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. (1988). The communicative repertoire of captive bonobos (Pan paniscus), compared to that of chimpanzees. Behaviour, 106(3/4), 183–251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. (1989). Dominance ‘style’ and primate social organization. In Standen, V. and Foley, R. A. (eds), Comparative Socioecology: The Behavioural Ecology of Humans and Other Mammals. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 243–263.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. (1995). Sex as an alternative to aggression in the bonobo. In Abramson, P. R. and Pinkerton, S. D. (eds), Sexual Nature, Sexual Culture. Chicago, IL: University Press of Chicago, pp. 37–56.Google Scholar
Decety, J., Grezes, J., Costes, N. et al., (1997). Brain activity during observation of actions. Influence of action content and subject’s strategy. Brain, 120(10), 1763–1777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
delBarco-Trillo, J., Sacha, C. R., Dubay, G. R. and Drea, C. M. (2012). Eulemur, me lemur: the evolution of scent-signal complexity in a primate clade. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1597), 1909–1922.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1983). Intentional systems in cognitive ethology: the ‘Panglossian paradigm’ defended. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6(3), 343–390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1987). The Intentional Stance. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Deschner, T., Heistermann, M., Hodges, K. and Boesch, C. (2004). Female sexual swelling size, timing of ovulation, and male behavior in wild West African chimpanzees. Hormones and Behavior, 46(2), 204–215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeWitt, I. and Rauschecker, J. P. (2012). Phoneme and word recognition in the auditory ventral stream. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(8), E505–E514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Di Bitetti, M. S. (2003). Food-associated calls of tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella nigritus) are functionally referential signals. Behaviour, 140(5), 565–592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Di Fiore, A. and Campbell, C. J. (2007). The atelines: variation in ecology, behavior, and social organization. In Campbell, C. J., Fuentes, A., MacKinnon, K. C., Panger, M. and Bearder, S. K. (eds), Primates in Perspective. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 155–185.Google Scholar
di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V. and Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research, 91(1), 176–180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Digby, L. J. (1995). Social organization in a wild population of Callithrix jacchus: II. Intragroup social behavior. Primates, 36(3), 361–375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Digby, L. J., Ferrari, S. F. and Saltzman, W. (2007). Callitrichines: The role of competition in cooperatively breeding species. In Campbell, C. J., Fuentes, A., MacKinnon, K. C., Panger, M. and Bearder, S. K. (eds), Primates in Perspective. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 85–106.Google Scholar
Digweed, S. M., Fedigan, L. M. and Rendall, D. (2005). Variable specificity in the anti-predator vocalizations and behaviour of the white-faced capuchin, Cebus capucinus. Behaviour, 142(8), 997–1021.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diogo, R., Wood, B. A., Aziz, M. A. and Burrows, A. (2009). On the origin, homologies and evolution of primate facial muscles, with a particular focus on hominoids and a suggested unifying nomenclature for the facial muscles of the Mammalia. Journal of Anatomy, 215(3), 300–319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dittus, W. P. J. (1984). Toque macaque food calls: semantic communication concerning food distribution in the environment. Animal Behaviour, 32(2), 470–477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixson, A. F. (1983). Observations on the evolution and behavioral significance of ‘sexual skin’ in female primates. In Rosenblatt, J. S., Hinde, R. A., Beer, C. and Busnel, M.-C. (eds), Advances in the Study of Behavior Vol. 13. New York: Academic Press, pp. 63–106.Google Scholar
Dixson, A. F. (1998). Primate Sexuality: Comparative Studies of the Prosimians, Monkeys, Apes, and Human Beings. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dobson, S. D. (2009). Socioecological correlates of facial mobility in nonhuman anthropoids. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 139(3), 413–420.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dobson, S. D. (2012). Coevolution of facial expression and social tolerance in macaques. American Journal of Primatology, 74(3), 229–235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dobson, S. D. and Sherwood, C. C. (2011). Correlated evolution of brain regions involved in producing and processing facial expressions in anthropoid primates. Biology Letters, 7(1), 86–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dominy, N. J. and Lucas, P. W. (2001). Ecological importance of trichromatic vision to primates. Nature, 410(6826), 363–366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dominy, N. J., Ross, C. F. and Smith, T. D. (2004). Evolution of the special senses in primates: past, present, and future. The Anatomical Record Part A: Discoveries in Molecular, Cellular, and Evolutionary Biology, 281(1), 1078–1082.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dressnandt, J. and Jürgens, U. (1992). Brain stimulation-induced changes of phonation in the squirrel monkey. Experimental Brain Research, 89(3), 549–559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duchenne de Boulogne, G. B. (1990 [first published in French in 1862]). The Mechanisms of Human Facial Expression. Cuthbertson, R. A. (ed. and trans.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dulac, C. and Torello, A. T. (2003). Molecular detection of pheromone signals in mammals: from genes to behaviour. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(7), 551–562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1987). Habitat quality, population dynamics, and group composition in colobus monkeys (Colobus guereza). International Journal of Primatology, 8(4), 299–329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1988). Primate Social Systems. New York: Cornell University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1992). Neocortex size as a constraint on group size in primates. Journal of Human Evolution, 22(6), 469–493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993). Coevolution of neocortical size, group-size and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(4), 681–694.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. (1995). Neocortex size and group size in primates: a test of the hypothesis. Journal of Human Evolution, 28(3), 287–296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1996). Grooming, Gossip, and the Evolution of Language. London: Faber and Faber.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology, 6(5), 178–190.3.0.CO;2-8>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (2003). The social brain: mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective. Annual Review of Anthropology, 32, 163–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (2011). Evolutionary basis of the social brain. In Decety, J. and Cacioppo, J. J. (eds), Oxford Handbook of Social Neuroscience. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, pp. 28–38.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (2012). Bridging the bonding gap: the transition from primates to humans. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1597), 1837–1846.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. and Shultz, S. (2010). Bondedness and sociality. Behaviour, 147(7), 775–803.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egbert, A. L. and Stokes, A. W. (1976). The social behavior of brown bears on an Alaskan stream. In Pelton, M. R., Lentfer, J. W. and Folk, G. E. (eds), Bears: Their Biology and Management: Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on Bear Research and Management. Morges, Switzerland: International Union for the Conservation of nature and Natural Resources, pp. 41–56.Google Scholar
Eggert, F., Ferstl, R. and Müller-Ruchholtz, W. (1999). MHC and olfactory communication in humans. In Johnston, R. E., Müller-Schwarze, D. and Sorensen, P. W. (eds), Advances in Chemical Signals in Vertebrates. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers, pp. 181–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P. (1979). About brows: emotional and conversational signals. In von Cranach, M., Koppa, F., Lepenies, W. and Ploog, D. (eds), Human Ethology: Claims and Limits of a New Discipline. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 169–202.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1984). Expression and the nature of emotion. In Scherer, K. and Ekman, P. (eds), Approaches to Emotion. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 19–344.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6(3–4), 169–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P. (1994). Moods, emotions, and traits. In Ekman, P. and Davidson, R. J. (eds), The Nature of Emotion: Fundamental Questions. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 56–58.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. and Friesen, W. V. (1978). Facial Action Coding System: A Technique for the Measurement of Facial Movement. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Davidson, R. J. and Friesen, W. V. (1990). The Duchenne smile: emotional expression and brain physiology: II. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(2), 342–353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V. and Hager, J. C. (2002). The Facial Action Coding System (2nd edn). Salt Lake City, UT: Research Nexus.Google Scholar
Elliot Smith, G. (1927). The Evolution of Man. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Elowson, A. M. and Snowdon, C. T. (1994). Pygmy marmosets, Cebuella pygmaea, modify vocal structure in response to changed social environment. Animal Behaviour, 47(6), 1267–1277CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elowson, A. M., Tannenbaum, P. L. and Snowdon, C. T. (1991). Food-associated calls correlate with food preferences in cotton-top tamarins. Animal Behaviour, 42(6), 931–937.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Embick, D., Marantz, A., Miyashita, Y., O’Neil, W. and Sakai, K. L. (2000). A syntactic specialization for Broca’s area. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97(11), 6150–6154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emery, N. J. (2000). The eyes have it: The neuroethology, function and evolution of social gaze. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24(6), 581–604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emery, N. J., Seed, A. M., von Bayern, A. M. P. and Clayton, N. S. (2007). Cognitive adaptations of social bonding in birds. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362(1480), 489–505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engelhardt, A., Fischer, J., Neumann, C., Pfeifer, J. B. and Heistermann, M. (2012). Information content of female copulation calls in wild long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 66(1), 121–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engh, A. L., Hoffmeier, R. R., Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (2006). Who, me? Can baboons infer the target of vocalizations?Animal Behaviour, 71(2), 381–387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Enstam, K. L. and Isbell, L. A. (2007). The guenons (genus Cercopithecus) and their allies. Primates in Perspective. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 252–274.Google Scholar
Epple, G. (1986). Communication by chemical signals. In Mitchell, G. and Erwin, J. (eds), Comparative Primate Biology: Behavior, Conservation, and Ecology, Vol. 2. New York: Alan R. Liss, pp. 531–580.Google Scholar
Epple, G. and Katz, Y. (1984). Social influences on estrogen excretion and ovarian cyclicity in saddle back tamarins (Saguinus fuscicollis). American Journal of Primatology, 6(3), 215–227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epple, G., Belcher, A. M., Kuderling, I. et al. (1993). Making sense out of scents: species differences in scent glands, scent-marking behaviour, and scent-mark composition in the Callitrichidae. In Rylands, A. B. (ed.), Marmosets and Tamarins: Systematics, Behaviour, and Ecology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 123–151.Google Scholar
Evans, C. S. (1997). Referential signals. In Owings, D. H., Beacher, M. D. and Thompson, N. S. (eds), Perspectives in Ethology. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 99–143.Google Scholar
Evans, C. S. and Evans, L. (1999). Chicken food calls are functionally referential. Animal Behaviour, 58(2), 307–319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ey, E., Rahn, C., Hammerschmidt, K. and Fischer, J. (2009). Wild female olive baboons adapt their grunt vocalizations to environmental conditions. Ethology, 115(5), 493–503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fedurek, P. and Slocombe, K. E. (2011). Primate vocal communication: a useful tool for understanding human speech and language evolution?Human Biology, 83(2), 153–173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fedurek, P. and Slocombe, K. E. (2013). The social function of food-associated calls in male chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology.CrossRef
Fedurek, P., Schel, A. M. and Slocombe, K. E. (2013). The acoustic structure of pant-hooting facilitates chorusing. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, .CrossRef
Fernández-Carriba, S., Loeches, Á., Morcillo, A. and Hopkins, W. D. (2002). Asymmetry in facial expression of emotions by chimpanzees. Neuropsychologia, 40(9), 1523–1533.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferrari, P. F., Gallese, V., Rizzolatti, G. and Fogassi, L. (2003). Mirror neurons responding to the observation of ingestive and communicative mouth actions in the monkey ventral premotor cortex. European Journal of Neuroscience, 17(8), 1703–1714.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferrari, P. F., Paukner, A., Ionica, C. and Suomi, S. J. (2009). Reciprocal face-to-face communication between rhesus macaque mothers and their newborn infants. Current Biology, 19(20), 1768–1772.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferrari, P. F., Vanderwert, R. E., Paukner, A., Bower, S., Suomi, S. J. and Fox, N. A. (2012). Distinct EEG amplitude suppression to facial gestures as evidence for a mirror mechanism in newborn monkeys. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(5), 1165–1172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferrari, P. F., Visalberghi, E., Paukner, A., Fogassi, L., Ruggiero, A. and Suomi, S. J. (2006). Neonatal imitation in rhesus macaques. PLoS Biology, 4(9), e302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fichtel, C. and Kappeler, P. M. (2002). Anti-predator behavior of group-living Malagasy primates: mixed evidence for a referential alarm call system. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 51(3), 262–275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finlay, B. L. and Darlington, R. B. (1995). Linked regularities in the development and evolution of mammalian brains. Science, 268(5217), 1578–1584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Firestein, S. (2001). How the olfactory system makes sense of scents. Nature 413(6852), 211–218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischer, J. (1998). Barbary macaques categorize shrill barks into two call types. Animal Behaviour, 55(4), 799–807.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischer, J. (2003). Developmental modifications in the vocal behavior of nonhuman primates. In Ghazanfar, A. A. (ed.), Primate Audition: Ethology and Neurobiology. Boca Raton: CRC Press, pp. 109–125.Google Scholar
Fischer, J., Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (2000). Development of infant baboons’ responses to graded bark variants. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 267(1459), 2317–2321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fitch, W. T. (2000). The evolution of speech: a comparative review. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(7), 258–267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fitch, W. T. and Hauser, M. D. (1995). Vocal production in nonhuman primates: acoustics, physiology, and functional constraints on ‘honest’ advertisement. American Journal of Primatology, 37(3), 191–219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitch, W. T. and Reby, D. (2001). The descended larynx is not uniquely human. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 268(1477), 1669–1675.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flack, J. C. and de Waal, F. B. M. (2007). Context modulates signal meaning in primate communication. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(5), 1581–1586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fleagle, J. G. (1999). Primate Adaptation and Evolution (2nd edn). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Fogassi, L. and Ferrari, P. F. (2007). Mirror neurons and the evolution of embodied language. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(3), 136–141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Font, E. and Carazo, P. (2010). Animals in translation: Why there is meaning (but probably no message) in animal communication. Animal Behaviour, 80(2), e1–e6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fontaine, R. (1984). Imitative skills between birth and six months. Infant Behavior and Development, 7(3), 323–333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forrester, G. S. (2008). A multidimensional approach to investigations of behaviour: Revealing structure in animal communication signals. Animal Behaviour, 76(5), 1749–1760.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fouts, R. (1975). Capacities for language in great apes. In Tuttle, R. H. (ed.), Socioecology and Psychology of Primates. The Hague: Mouton, pp. 371–390.Google Scholar
Fox, G. J. (1977). Social Dynamics in Siamangs. Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, University of Wisconsin, Milwaukee, WI.Google Scholar
Fox, G. J. (1982). Potentials for pheromones in chimpanzee vaginal fatty acids. Folia Primatologica, 37(3–4), 255–266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeberg, T. M., Dunbar, R. I. M. and Ord, T. J. (2012). Social complexity as a proximate and ultimate factor in communicative complexity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1597), 1785–1801.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freiwald, W. and Tsao, D. (2011). Taking apart the neural machinery of face processing. In Calder, A. J., Rhodes, G., Johnston, M. H. and Haxby, J. V. (eds), The Oxford Handbook of Face Perception. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 707–718.Google Scholar
French, J. A. (1997). Proximate regulation of singular breeding in callitrichid primates. In Solomon, N. G. and French, J. A. (eds), Cooperative Breeding in Mammals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 34–75.Google Scholar
Fridlund, A. J. (1991). Sociality of solitary smiling: potentiation by an implicit audience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(2), 229–240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fridlund, A. J. (1992). Darwin’s anti-Darwinism in ‘The expression of the emotions in man and animals’. In Strongman, K. T. (ed.), International Review of Studies on Emotion, Vol. 2, New York: Wiley, pp. 117–137.Google Scholar
Friederici, A. D. and Alter, K. (2004). Lateralization of auditory language functions: a dynamic dual pathway model. Brain and Language, 89(2), 267–276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Furuichi, T. and Ihobe, H. (1994). Variation in male relationships in bonobos and chimpanzees. Behaviour, 130(3–4), 211–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galdikas, B. F. and Vasey, P. (1992). Why are orangutans so smart? Ecological and social hypotheses. In Burton, F. D. (ed.), Social Processes and Mental Abilities in Nonhuman Primates. Lewiston, New York: Edwin Mellen Press, pp. 183–224.Google Scholar
Galdikas, B. M. F. (1985). Orangutan sociality at Tanjung Puting. American Journal of Primatology, 9(2), 101–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L. and Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119(2), 593–609.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garber, P. A. (1988). Diet, foraging patterns, and resource defense in a mixed species troop of Saguinus mystax and Saguinus fuscicollis in Amazonian Peru. Behaviour, 105(112), 18–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garber, P. A. (1993). Feeding ecology and behavior of the genus Saguinus. In Rylands, A. B. (ed.), Marmosets and Tamarins: Systematics, Behaviour, and Ecology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 273–295.Google Scholar
Gardner, B. T. and Gardner, R. A. (1974). Comparing the early utterances of child and chimpanzee. In Pick, A. (ed.), Minnesota Symposia of Child Psychology, Vol. 8. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, pp. 3–23.Google Scholar
Gardner, R. A., Gardner, B. T. and Van Cantford, (eds) (1989). Teaching Sign Language to Chimpanzees. New York: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Gardner, R. A. and Gardner, B. T. (1969). Teaching sign language to a chimpanzee. Science, 165(3894), 664–672.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardner, R. A. and Gardner, B. T. (1971). Two-way communication with an infant chimpanzee. In Schrier, A. and Stollnitz, F. (eds), Behavior of Nonhuman Primates, Vol. 4. New York: Academic Press, pp. 117–184.Google Scholar
Geissmann, T. (1984). Inheritance of song parameters in the gibbon song, analyzed in 2 hybrid gibbons (Hylobates pileatus x H. lar). Folia Primatologica, 42(3–4), 216–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geissmann, T. (1999). Duet songs of the siamang, Hylobates syndactylus: II. Testing the pair-bonding hypothesis during a partner exchange. Behaviour, 136, 1005–1039.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gemba, H., Miki, N. and Sasaki, K. (1995). Cortical field potentials preceding vocalization and influences of cerebellar hemispherectomy upon them in monkeys. Brain Research, 697(1–2), 143–151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gentilucci, M. and Corballis, M. C. (2006). From manual gesture to speech: A gradual transition. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(7), 949–960.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Genty, E. and Byrne, R. W. (2010). Why do gorillas make sequences of gestures?Animal Cognition, 13(2), 287–301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Genty, E., Breuer, T., Hobaiter, C. and Byrne, R. W. (2009). Gestural communication of the gorilla (Gorilla gorilla): repertoire, intentionality and possible origins. Animal Cognition, 12(3), 527–546.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ghazanfar, A. A. and Hauser, M. D. (1999). The neuroethology of primate vocal communication: Substrates for the evolution of speech. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(10), 377–384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ghazanfar, A. A. and Logothetis, N. K. (2003). Neuroperception: facial expressions linked to monkey calls. Nature, 423(6943), 937–938.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ghazanfar, A. A. and Rendall, D. (2008). Evolution of human vocal production. Current Biology, 18(11), R457–R460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ghazanfar, A. A. and Schroeder, C. E. (2006). Is neocortex essentially multisensory?Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(6), 278–285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ghazanfar, A. A., Chandrasekaran, C. and Morrill, R. J. (2010). Dynamic, rhythmic facial expressions and the superior temporal sulcus of macaque monkeys: implications for the evolution of audiovisual speech. European Journal of Neuroscience, 31(10), 1807–1817.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ghazanfar, A. A., Maier, J. X., Hoffman, K. L. and Logothetis, N. K. (2005). Multisensory integration of dynamic faces and voices in rhesus monkey auditory cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25(20), 5004–5012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ghazanfar, A. A., Takahashi, D. Y., Mathur, N. and Fitch, W. (2012). Cineradiography of monkey lip-smacking reveals putative precursors of speech dynamics. Current Biology, 22(13), 1176–1182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gil-da-Costa, R., Martin, A., Lopes, M. A., Muñoz, M., Fritz, J. B. and Braun, A. R. (2006). Species-specific calls activate homologs of Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas in the macaque. Nature Neuroscience, 9(8), 1064–1070.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillette, R. G., Brown, R., Herman, P., Vernon, S. and Vernon, J. (1973). The auditory sensitivity of the lemur. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 38(2), 365–370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glowa, J. R. and Newman, J. D. (1986). Benactyzine increases alarm call rates in the squirrel monkey. Psychopharmacology, 90(4), 457–460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldfoot, D. A., Essock-Vitale, S. M., Asa, C. S., Thornton, J. E. and Leshner, A. I. (1978). Anosmia in male rhesus monkeys does not alter copulatory activity with cycling females. Science, 199(4333), 1095–1096.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2003). Beyond words: the importance of gesture to researchers and learners. Child Development, 71(1), 231–239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gómez, J. C. (1990). The emergence of intentional communication as a problem-solving strategy in the gorilla. In Parker, S. T. and Gibson, K. R. (eds), ‘Language’ and Intelligence in Monkeys and Apes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 333–355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gómez, J. C. (1996). Ostensive behavior in great apes: The role of eye contact. In Russon, A. E., Parker, S. T. and Bard, K. A. (eds.), Reaching into Thought: The Minds of the Great Apes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 131–151.Google Scholar
Gómez, J. C. (2007). Pointing behaviors in apes and human infants: a balanced interpretation. Child Development, 78(3), 729–734.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodall, J. (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior. Cambridge, MA: The Belknap Press of Harvard University.Google Scholar
Goodall, J., Bandora, A., Bergmann, E. et al. (1979). Intercommunity interactions in the chimpanzee population of the Gombe National Park. In Hamburg, D. and McCown, E. R. (eds), The Great Apes. Menlo Park, California: Benjamin/Cummings, pp. 13–53.Google Scholar
Goossens, B., Dekleva, M., Reader, S. M., Sterck, E. H. M. and Bolhuis, J. J. (2008). Gaze following in monkeys is modulated by observed facial expressions. Animal Behaviour, 75(5), 1673–1681.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gordon, S. D. and Uetz, G. W. (2011). Multimodal communication of wolf spiders on different substrates: evidence for behavioural plasticity. Animal Behaviour, 81(2), 367–375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gothard, K. M., Battaglia, F. P., Erickson, C. A., Spitler, K. M. and Amaral, D. G. (2007). Neural responses to facial expression and face identity in the monkey amygdala. Journal of Neurophysiology, 97(2), 1671–1683.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottlieb, G. (1971). Ontogenesis of sensory function in birds and mammals. In Tobach, E., Aronson, L. and Shaw, E. (eds), The Biopsychology of Development. New York: Academic Press, pp. 67–128.Google Scholar
Gould, J. L. and Gould, C. G. (1988). The Honey Bee. New York: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. and Lewontin, R. C. (1979). The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: a critique of the adaptationist programme. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 205(1161), 581–598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gouzoules, S., Gouzoules, H. and Marler, P. (1984). Rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) screams: representational signalling in the recruitment of agonistic aid. Animal Behaviour, 32(1), 182–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gouzoules, H., Gouzoules, S. and Marler, P. (1985). External reference in mammalian vocal communication. In Zivin, G. (ed.), The Development of Expressive Behavior: Biology–Environment Interactions. New York: Academic Press, pp. 77–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grafton, S. T., Arbib, M. A., Fadiga, L. and Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Localization of grasp representations in humans by positron emission tomography. 2. Observation compared with imagination. Experimental Brain Research, 112(1), 103–111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenfield, P. M. and Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (1990). Grammatical combination in Pan paniscus: processes of learning and invention in the evolution and development of language. In Parker, S. T. and Gibson, K. R. (eds), ‘Language’ and Intelligence in Monkeys and Apes. Comparative Developmental Perspectives. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 540–578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grice, H. P. (1957). Meaning. The Philosophical Review, 66(3), 377–388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gros-Louis, J. (2004a). Responses of white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus) to naturalistic and experimentally presented food-associated calls. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118(4), 396–402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gros-Louis, J. (2004b). The function of food-associated calls in white-faced capuchin monkeys, Cebus capucinus, from the perspective of the signaller. Animal Behaviour, 67(3), 431–440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gross, L. (2006). Evolution of neonatal imitation. PLoS Biology, 4(9), e311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groswasser, Z., Korn, C., Groswasser-Reider, I. and Solzi, P. (1988). Mutism associated with buccofacial apraxia and bihemispheric lesions. Brain and Language, 34(1), 157–168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groves, C. (2001). Primate Taxonomy. Washington DC: Smithsonian Institution Press.Google Scholar
Guilford, T. and Dawkins, M. S. (1991). Receiver psychology and the evolution of animal signals. Animal Behaviour, 42(1), 1–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gursky, S. (2003). Territoriality in the spectral tarsier, Tarsius spectrum. In Wright, P. C., Simons, E. L. and Gursky, S. L. (eds), Tarsiers: Past, Present and Future. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press, pp. 221–236.Google Scholar
Gustison, M. L., le Roux, A. and Bergman, T. J. (2012). Derived vocalizations of geladas (Theropithecus gelada) and the evolution of vocal complexity in primates. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1597), 1847–1859.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hadj-Bouziane, F., Bell, A. H., Knusten, T. A., Ungerleider, L. G. and Tootell, R. B. H. (2008). Perception of emotional expressions is independent of face selectivity in monkey inferior temporal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(14), 5591–5596.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hagey, L. R., Fry, B. G. and Fitch-Snyder, H. (2007). Talking defensively, a dual use for the brachial gland exudate of slow and pygmy lorises. In Gursky, S. L. and Nekaris, K. A. I. (eds), Primate Anti-predator Strategies. New York: Springer, pp. 253–272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halpern, M. (1987). The organization and function of the vomeronasal system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 10, 325–362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halpin, Z. T. (1980). Individual odors and individual recognition: review and commentary. Biology of Behaviour, 5(3), 233–248.Google Scholar
Hammerschmidt, K., Newman, J. D., Champoux, M. and Suomi, S. J. (2000). Changes in rhesus macaque ‘coo’ vocalizations during early development. Ethology, 106(10), 873–886.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hardus, M. E., Lameira, A. R., Singleton, I. et al. (2009a). A description of the orangutans, vocal and sound repertoire, with a focus on geographic variation. In Wich, S. A., Atmoki, S. S. U., Setia, T. M. and van Schaik, C. P. (eds), Orangutans: Geographical Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation, Vol. 1. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 49–65.Google Scholar
Hardus, M. E., Lameira, A. R., van Schaik, C. P. and Wich, S. A. (2009b). Tool use in wild orang-utans modifies sound production: a functionally deceptive innovation?Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276(1673), 3689–3694.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, B. and Tomasello, M. (2004). Chimpanzees are more skilful in competitive than in cooperative cognitive tasks. Animal Behaviour, 68(3), 571–581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hare, B., Brown, M., Williamson, C. and Tomasello, M. (2002). The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science, 298(5598), 1634–1636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harlow, H. F. and Harlow, M. (1966). Learning to love. American Scientist, 54(3), 244–272.Google ScholarPubMed
Harris, T. R. and Monfort, S. L. (2003). Behavioral and endocrine dynamics associated with infanticide in a black and white colobus monkey (Colobus guereza). American Journal of Primatology, 61(3), 135–142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hasselmo, M. E., Rolls, E. T. and Baylis, G. C. (1989). The role of expression and identity in the face-selective responses of neurons in the temporal visual cortex of the monkey. Behavioural Brain Research, 32(3), 203–218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hattori, Y., Kano, F. and Tomonaga, M. (2010). Differential sensitivity to conspecific and allospecific cues in chimpanzees and humans: A comparative eye-tracking study. Biology Letters, 6(5), 610–613.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hattori, Y., Kuroshima, H. and Fujita, K. (2010). Tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) show understanding of human attentional states when requesting food held by a human. Animal Cognition, 13(1), 87–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauser, M. D. (1989). Ontogenetic changes in the comprehension and production of vervet monkey (Cercopithecus aethiops) vocalizations. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 103(2), 149–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hauser, M. D. (1993). Right hemisphere dominance for the production of facial expression in monkeys. Science, 261(5120), 475–477.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauser, M. D. (1998). Functional referents and acoustic similarity: field playback experiments with rhesus monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 55(6), 1647–1658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauser, M. D. and Akre, K. (2001). Asymmetries in the timing of facial and vocal expressions by rhesus monkeys: implications for hemispheric specialization. Animal Behaviour, 61(2), 391–400.CrossRef
Hauser, M. D. and Marler, P. (1993). Food-associated calls in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): II. Costs and benefits of call production and suppression. Behavioral Ecology, 4(3), 206–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N. and Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve?Science, 298(5598), 1569–1579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauser, M. D., Teixidor, P., Field, L. and Flaherty, R. (1993). Food-elicited calls in chimpanzees: Effects of food quantity and divisibility. Animal Behaviour, 45(4), 817–819.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A. and Gobbini, M. I. (2000). The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(6), 223–233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A. and Gobbini, M. I. (2002). Human neural systems for face recognition and social communication. Biological Psychiatry, 51(1), 59–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, C. (1951). The Ape in our House. New York: Harper and Brothers.Google Scholar
Hayes, K. J. and Hayes, C. (1951). The intellectual development of a home-raised chimpanzee. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 95(2), 105–109.Google Scholar
Hayes, R. A., Morelli, T. L. and Wright, P. C. (2004). Anogenital gland secretions of Lemur catta and Propithecus verreauxi coquereli: A preliminary chemical examination. American Journal of Primatology, 63(2), 49–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hebets, E. A. and Papaj, D. R. (2005). Complex signal function: developing a framework of testable hypotheses. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 57(3), 197–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heesy, C. P. (2003). The evolution of orbit orientation in mammals and the function of the primate postorbital bar. Unpublished PhD dissertation, State University of New York at Stony Brook, Stony Brook, NY.Google Scholar
Heesy, C. P. and Ross, C. F. (2001). Evolution of activity patterns and chromatic vision in primates: morphometrics, genetics and cladistics. Journal of Human Evolution, 40(2), 111–149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heffner, H. E., Ravizza, R. J. and Masterton, B. (1969). Hearing in primitive mammals, IV: bushbaby (Galago senegalensis). Journal of Auditory Research, 9, 19–23.Google Scholar
Heffner, R. S. (2004). Primate hearing from a mammalian perspective. The Anatomical Record Part A: Discoveries in Molecular, Cellular, and Evolutionary Biology, 281A(1), 1111–1122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henglmüller, S. M. and Ladichm, F. (1999). Development of agonistic behaviour and vocalization in croaking gouramis. Journal of Fish Biology, 54(2), 380–395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hennessy, D. F., Owings, D. H., Rowe, M. P., Coss, R. G. and Leger, D. W. (1981). The information afforded by a variable signal: constraints on snake-elicited tail flagging by California ground squirrels. Behaviour, 78(3/4), 188–226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herbinger, I., Papworth, S., Boesch, C. and Zuberbühler, K. (2009). Vocal, gestural and locomotor responses of wild chimpanzees to familiar and unfamiliar intruders: a playback study. Animal Behaviour, 78(6), 1389–1396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hesler, N. and Fischer, J. (2007). Gestural communication in Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus): an overview. In Tomasello, M. and Call, J. (eds), The Gestural Communication of Apes and Monkeys. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 159–195.Google Scholar
Hewes, G. W. (1973). Primate communication and gestural origin of language. Current Anthropology, 14(1–2), 5–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heymann, E. W. (2006). The neglected sense: olfaction in primate behavior, ecology and evolution. American Journal of Primatology, 68(6), 519–524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hickok, G. and Poeppel, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(5), 393–402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, C. M. (1994). The role of female Diana monkeys, Cercopithecus diana, in territorial defence. Animal Behaviour, 47(2), 425–431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hinde, R. A. (1976). Interactions, relationships and social structure. Man, 11, 1–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobaiter, C. and Byrne, R. W. (2011a). The gestural repertoire of the wild chimpanzee. Animal Cognition, 14(5), 745–767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobaiter, C. and Byrne, R. W. (2011b). Serial gesturing by wild chimpanzees: its nature and function for communication. Animal Cognition, 14(6), 827–838.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hockett, C. F. (1960). The origin of speech. Scientific American, 203, 89–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hook-Costigan, M. A. and Rogers, L. J. (1998). Lateralized use of the mouth in production of vocalizations by marmosets. Neuropsychologia, 36(12), 1265–1273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoover, K. C. (2010). Smell with inspiration: The evolutionary significance of olfaction. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 143(S51), 63–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopkins, W. D. and Cantalupo, C. (2005). Individual and setting differences in the hand preferences of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): a critical analysis and some alternative explanations. Laterality, 10(1), 65–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopkins, W. D. and Leavens, D. A. (1998). Hand use and gestural communication in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 112(1), 95–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopkins, W. D. and Nir, T. M. (2010). Planum temporale surface area and grey matter asymmetries in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): the effect of handedness and comparison with findings in humans. Behavioural Brain Research, 208(2), 436–443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopkins, W. D., Cantalupo, C. and Taglialatela, J. (2007). Handedness is associated with asymmetries in gyrification of the cerebral cortex of chimpanzees. Cerebral Cortex, 17(8), 1750–1756.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopkins, W. D., Pika, S., Liebal, K. et al. (2012). Handedness for manual gestures in great apes. A meta-analysis. In Pika, S. and Liebal, K. (eds), Developments in Primate Gesture Research. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 93–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopkins, W. D., Taglialatela, J. P. and Leavens, D. A. (2011). Do chimpanzees have voluntary control of their facial expressions and vocalizations? In Vilain, A., Schwartz, J.-L., Abry, C. and Vauclair, J. (eds.), Primate Communication and Human Language: Vocalisation, Gestures, Imitation and Deixis in Humans and Non-humans. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 71–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopkins, W. D., Taglialatela, J. P. and Leavens, D. A. (2007). Chimpanzees differentially produce novel vocalizations to capture the attention of a human. Animal Behaviour, 73(2), 281–286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hostetter, A. B., Cantero, M. and Hopkins, W. D. (2001). Differential use of vocal and gestural communication by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in response to the attentional status of a human (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 115(4), 337–343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huber, E. (1931). Evolution of Facial Musculature and Facial Expression. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hurford, J. R. (2011). The Origins of Grammar: Language in the Light of Evolution II. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hurlbert, S. H. (1984). Pseudoreplication and the design of ecological field experiments. Ecological Monographs, 54(2), 187–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huxley, J. S. (1914). The courtship-habits of the great crested grebe (Podiceps cristatus); with an addition to the theory of sexual selection. Proceedings of the Zoological Society of London, 84(3), 491–562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isbell, L. A. (1991). Contest and scramble competition: patterns of female aggression and ranging behavior among primates. Behavioral Ecology, 2(2), 143–155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isbell, L. A. (1994). The vervets’ year of doom. Natural History, 103(8), 48–55.Google Scholar
Iverson, J. M. and Goldin-Meadow, S. (1998). Why people gesture when they speak. Nature, 396(6708), 228–230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jablonka, E., Ginsburg, S. and Dor, D. (2012). The co-evolution of language and emotions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1599), 2152–2159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacob, S., McClintock, M. K., Zelano, B. and Ober, C. (2002). Paternally inherited HLA alleles are associated with women’s choice of male odor. Nature Genetics, 30(2), 175–179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaeggi, A. V., Burkart, J. M. and van Schaik, C. P. (2010). On the psychology of cooperation in humans and other primates: combining the natural history and experimental evidence of prosocialityPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 365(1553), 2723–2735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Janson, C. H. (2000). Primate socio-ecology: the end of a golden age. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 9(2), 73–86.3.0.CO;2-X>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janson, C. H. and Chapman, C. A. (1999). Resources as determinants of primate community structure. In Fleagle, J. G., Janson, C. H. and Reed, K. E. (eds), Primate Communities. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 237–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jia, C., Chen, W. R. and Shepherd, G. M. (1999). Synaptic organization and neurotransmitters in the rat accessory olfactory bulb. Journal of Neurophysiology, 81(1), 345–355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnstone, R. A. (1996). Multiple displays in animal communication: ‘Backup signals’ and ‘multiple messages’. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 351(1337), 329–338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jolly, C. J. (2007). Baboons, mandrills, and mangabeys. Afro-papionin socioecology in a phylogentic perspective. In Campbell, C. J., Fuentes, A., MacKinnon, K. C., Panger, M. and Bearder, S. (Eds.), Primates in perspective. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 240–251.Google Scholar
Joly, O., Ramus, F., Pressnitzer, D., Vanduffel, W. and Orban, G. A. (2012). Interhemispheric differences in auditory processing revealed by fMRI in awake rhesus monkeys. Cerebral Cortex, 22(4), 838–853.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, C. B. and Van Cantford, T. E. (2007a). A schema for multimodal communication applied to male mantled howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata). Laboratory Primate Newsletter, 46(2), 6–9.Google Scholar
Jones, C. B. and Van Cantford, T. E. (2007b). Multimodal communication by male mantled howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata) in sexual contexts: a descriptive analysis. Folia Primatologica, 78(3), 166–185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jung-Beeman, M. (2005). Bilateral brain processes for comprehending natural language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(11), 512–518.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jürgens, U. (1976). Projections from the cortical larynx area in the squirrel monkey. Experimental Brain Research, 25(4), 401–411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jürgens, U. (2002). Neural pathways underlying vocal control. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26(2), 235–258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jürgens, U., Kirzinger, A. and von Cramon, D. (1982). The effects of deep-reaching lesions in the cortical face area on phonation. A combined case report and experimental monkey study. Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 18(1), 125–139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaas, J. H., Hackett, T. A. and Tramo, M. J. (1999). Auditory processing in primate cerebral cortex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 9(2), 164–170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaminski, J., Call, J. and Fischer, J. (2004). Word learning in a domestic dog: Evidence for ‘fast mapping’. Science, 304(5677), 1682–1683.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaminski, J., Call, J. and Tomasello, M. (2004). Body orientation and face orientation: two factors controlling apes’ begging behavior from humans. Animal Cognition, 7, 216–223.CrossRef
Kano, F., Call, J. and Tomonaga, M. (2012). Face and eye scanning in gorillas (Gorilla gorilla), orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), and humans (Homo sapiens): Unique eye-viewing patterns in humans among hominids. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126(4), 388–398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kano, T. (1992). The Last Ape: Pygmy Chimpanzee Behavior and Ecology. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Kaplan, G. and Rogers, L. J. (2002). Patterns of gazing in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). International Journal of Primatology, 23(3), 501–526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kappeler, P. M. (1997). Determinants of primate social organization: comparative evidence and new insights from Malagasy lemurs. Biological Reviews, 72(1), 111–151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kappeler, P. M. (1998). To whom it may concern: the transmission and function of chemical signals in Lemur catta. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 42(6), 411–421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kappeler, P. M. and van Schaik, C. P. (2002). Evolution of primate social systems. International Journal of Primatology, 23(4), 707–740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kawai, M., Ohsawa, H., Mori, U. and Dunbar, R. (1983). Social organization of gelada baboons: social units and definitions. Primates, 24(1), 13–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kellogg, W. N. and Kellogg, L. A. (1933). The Ape and the Child. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Kendon, A. (2004). Gesture: Visible Action as Utterance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keverne, E. B. (1999). The vomeronasal organ. Science, 286(5440), 716–720.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keysers, C. and Gazzola, V. (2009). Expanding the mirror: vicarious activity for actions, emotions and sensations. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 19(6), 666–671.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kimura, D. (1993). Neuromotor Mechanisms in Human Communication. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, A. J. and Palmer, A. R. (1985). Integration of visual and auditory information in bimodal neurons in the guinea-pig superior colliculus. Experimental Brain Research, 60(3), 492–500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, B. J. (1999). The Origin of Language: What Non-human Primates Can Tell Us. Santa Fe, NM: School of American Research Press.Google Scholar
King, B. J. (2004). The Dynamic Dance: Nonvocal Communication in African Great Apes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kinzey, W. G. (1981). The Titi Monkeys, Genus Callicebus. In Coimbra-Filho, A. F. and Mittermeier, R. A. (eds), Ecology and Behavior of Neotropical Primates, Vol. 1.Rio de Janeiro: Academia Brasileira de Ciências, pp. 241–276.Google Scholar
Kirchhof, J. and Hammerschmidt, K. (2006). Functionally referential alarm calls in tamarins (Saguinus fuscicollis and Saguinus mystax): evidence from playback experiments. Ethology, 112(4), 346–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirk, E. C. and Kay, R. F. (2004). The evolution of high visual acuity in the Anthropoidea. In Ross, C.F. and Kay, R.F. (eds), Anthropoid Origins. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers, pp. 539–602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirk, E. C., Cartmill, M., Kay, R. F. and Lemelin, P. (2003). Comment on ‘Grasping primate origins’. Science, 300(5620), 741.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirzinger, A. and Jürgens, U. (1985). The effects of brainstem lesions on vocalization in the squirrel monkey. Brain Research, 358(1–2), 150–162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kita, S. (2003). Pointing: Where Language, Culture and Cognition Meet. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kitzmann, C. D. and Caine, N. G. (2009). Marmoset (Callithrix geoffroyi) food-associated calls are functionally referential. Ethology, 115(5), 439–448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kleiman, D. G. (1977). Monogamy in mammals. Quarterly Review of Biology, 52(1), 39–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, J. (1986). Natural History of the Major Histocompatibility Complex. New York: John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
Knapp, L. A., Robson, J. and Waterhouse, J. S. (2006). Olfactory signals and the MHC: a review and a case study in Lemur catta. American Journal of Primatology, 68(6), 568–584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kobayashi, H. and Kohshima, S. (1997). Unique morphology of the human eye. Nature, 387(6635), 767–768.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kobayashi, H. and Kohshima, S. (2001). Unique morphology of the human eye and its adaptive meaning: Comparative studies on external morphology of the primate eye. Journal of Human Evolution, 40(5), 419–435.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koenig, A. (2002). Competition for resources and its behavioral consequences among female primates. International Journal of Primatology, 23(4), 759–783.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umilta, M. A. et al. (2002). Hearing sounds, understanding actions: action representation in mirror neurons. Science, 297(5582), 846–848.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kojima, S., Izumi, A. and Ceugniet, M. (2003). Identification of vocalizers by pant hoots, pant grunts and screams in a chimpanzee. Primates, 44(3), 225–230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krebs, J. R. and Davies, N. B. (1993). An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Kroodsma, D. E., Byers, B. E., Goodale, E., Johnson, S. and Liu, W. C. (2001). Pseudoreplication in playback experiments, revisited a decade later. Animal Behaviour, 61, 1029–1033.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kruuk, H. (1972). The Spotted Hyena: A Study of Predation and Social Behavior. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kumashiro, M., Ishibashi, H., Itakura, S., Iriki, A. (2002). Bidirectional communication between a Japanese monkey and a human through eye gaze and pointing. Current Psychology of Cognition, 21(1), 3–32.Google Scholar
Kummer, H. (1968). Social Organization of Hamadryas Baboons: A Field Study. Basel, Switzerland: Karger.Google Scholar
Kummer, H. (1997). In Quest of the Sacred Baboon: A Scientist’s Journey. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Kummer, H. and Kurt, F. (1965). A comparison of social behavior in captive and wild hamadryas baboons. In Vagtborg, H. (ed.) The Baboon in Medical Research, Vol. 1. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press, pp. 65–80.Google Scholar
Kuroda, S. (1980). Social behavior of the pygmy chimpanzees. Primates, 21(2), 181–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuypers, H. G. J. M. (1958). Corticobulbar connexions to the pons and lower brainstem in man. An anatomical study. Brain, 81, 364–388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laidre, M. E. (2008). Do captive mandrills invent new gestures?Animal Cognition, 11(2), 179–187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laidre, M. E. (2009). Informative breath: olfactory cues sought during social foraging among Old World monkeys (Mandrillus sphinx, M. leucophaeus and Papio anubis). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 123(1), 34–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laidre, M. E. (2011). Meaningful gesture in monkeys? Investigating whether mandrills create social culture. PLoS One, 6(2), e14610.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laporte, M. N. C. and Zuberbühler, K. (2010). Vocal greeting behaviour in wild chimpanzee females. Animal Behaviour, 80(3), 467–473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laporte, M. N. C. and Zuberbühler, K. (2011). The development of a greeting signal in wild chimpanzees. Developmental Science, 14(5), 1220–1234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laska, M., Seibt, A. and Weber, A. (2000). ‘Microsmatic’ primates revisited: Olfactory sensitivity in the squirrel monkey. Chemical Senses, 25(1), 47–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Le Gros Clark, W. E. (1959). The Antecedents of Man. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Leavens, D. A. (2004). Manual deixis in apes and humans. Interaction Studies, 5(3), 387–408.Google Scholar
Leavens, D. A. and Hopkins, W. D. (1998). Intentional communication by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): a cross-sectional study of the use of referential gestures. Developmental Psychology, 34(5), 813–822.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leavens, D. A. and Hopkins, W. D. (1999). The whole-hand point: the structure and function of pointing from a comparative perspective. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113(4), 417–425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leavens, D. A., Hopkins, W. D. and Bard, K. A. (1996). Indexical and referential pointing in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 110(4), 346–353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leavens, D. A., Hopkins, W. D. and Bard, K. A. (2005). Understanding the point of chimpanzee pointing epigenesis and ecological validity. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(4), 185–189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leavens, D. A., Hopkins, W. D. and Thomas, R. K. (2004). Referential communication by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118(1), 48–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leavens, D. A., Hostetter, A. B., Wesley, M. J. and Hopkins, W. D. (2004). Tactical use of unimodal and bimodal communication by chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Animal Behaviour, 67(3), 467–476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leavens, D. A., Russell, J. L. and Hopkins, W. D. (2005). Intentionality as measured in the persistence and elaboration of communication by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Child Development, 76(1), 291–306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leavens, D. A., Russell, J. L. and Hopkins, W. D. (2010). Multimodal communication by captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Animal Cognition, 13(1), 33–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leitten, L., Jensvold, M. L. A., Fouts, R. S. and Wallin, J. M. (2012). Contingency in requests of signing chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Interaction Studies, 13(2), 147–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leliveld, L. M. C., Scheumann, M. and Zimmermann, E. (2010). Effects of caller characteristics on auditory laterality in an early primate (Microcebus murinus). PLoS One, 5(2), e9031.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lemasson, A., Gandon, E. and Hausberger, M. (2010a). Attention to elders' voice in non-human primates. Biology Letters, 6, 325–328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lemasson, A., Koda, H., Kato, A. et al. (2010). Influence of sound specificity and familiarity on Japanese macaques’ (Macaca fuscata) auditory laterality. Behavioural Brain Research, 208(1), 286–289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberman, A. M., Harris, K. S., Hoffman, H. S. and Griffith, B. C. (1957). The discrimination of speech sounds within and across phoneme boundaries. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54(5), 358–368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liebal, K. and Call, J. (2012). The origins of non-human primates’ manual gestures. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1585), 118–128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liebal, K., Behne, T., Carpenter, M. and Tomasello, M. (2009). Infants use shared experience to interpret pointing gestures. Developmental Science, 12(2), 264–271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liebal, K., Bressem, J. and Müller, C. (2010). Simultaneous structures of gestures in non-human primates. Paper presented at the 3rd Conference of the International Society of Gesture Studies.
Liebal, K., Call, J. and Tomasello, M. (2004a). Use of gesture sequences in chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology, 64(4), 377–396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liebal, K., Pika, S., Call, J. and Tomasello, M. (2004b). To move or not to move: how apes adjust to the attentional state of others. Interaction Studies, 5(2), 199–219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liebal, K., Pika, S. and Tomasello, M. (2004c). Social communication in siamangs (Symphalangus syndactylus): use of gestures and facial expressions. Primates, 45(1), 41–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liebal, K., Pika, S. and Tomasello, M. (2006). Gestural communication of orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Gesture, 6(1), 1–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, P. (1984). The Biology and Evolution of Language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P. (2012). Vocal tract anatomy and the neural bases of talking. Journal of Phonetics, 40(4), 608–622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, P., Klatt, D. H. and Wilson, W. H. (1969). Vocal tract limitations on the vowel repertoires of rhesus monkey and other nonhuman primates. Science, 164(3884), 1185–1187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liotti, M., Gay, C. T. and Fox, P. T. (1994). Functional imaging and language: evidence from positron emission tomography. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology: Official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society, 11(2), 175–190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liszkowski, U., Brown, P., Callaghan, T., Takada, A. and de Vos, C. (2012). A prelinguistic gestural universal of human communication. Cognitive Science, 36(4), 698–713.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Henning, A., Striano, T. and Tomasello, M. (2004). Twelve-month-olds point to share attention and interest. Developmental Science, 7(3), 297–307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Striano, T. and Tomasello, M. (2006). 12- and 18-month-olds point to provide information for others. Journal of Cognition and Development, 7(2), 173–187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M. and Tomasello, M. (2007). Reference and attitude in infant pointing. Journal of Child Language, 34(1), 1–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liszkowski, U., Schaefer, M., Carpenter, M. and Tomasello, M. (2009). Prelinguistic Infants, but not chimpanzees, communicate about absent entities. Psychological Science, 20(5), 654–660.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Losin, E. A. R., Russell, J. L., Freeman, H., Meguerditchian, A. and Hopkins, W. D. (2008). Left hemisphere specialization for oro-facial movements of learned vocal signals by captive chimpanzees. PLoS One, 3(6), e2529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luckett, W. P. (1976). Cladistic relationships among primate higher categories: Evidence of the fetal membranes and placenta. Folia Primatologica, 25(4), 245–276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luef, E. M. and Liebal, K. (2012). Infant-directed communication in lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla): Do older animals scaffold communicative competence in infants?American Journal of Primatology, 74(9), 841–852.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lüthe, L., Häusler, U. and Jürgens, U. (2000). Neuronal activity in the medulla oblongata during vocalization. A single-unit recording study in the squirrel monkey. Behavioural Brain Research, 116(2), 197–210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyn, H., Greenfield, P. M., Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Gillespie-Lynch, K. and Hopkins, W. D. (2011). Nonhuman primates do declare! A comparison of declarative symbol and gesture use in two children, two bonobos, and a chimpanzee. Language and Communication, 31(1), 63–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ma, W. and Klemm, W. R. (1997). Variations of equine urinary volatile compounds during the oestrous cycle. Veterinary Research Communications, 21(6), 437–446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macedonia, J. M. (1990). What is communicated in the antipredator calls of lemurs: evidence from playback experiments with ringtailed and ruffed lemurs. Ethology, 86(3), 177–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macedonia, J. M. and Evans, C. S. (1993). Essay on contemporary issues in ethology: variation among mammalian alarm call systems and the problem of meaning in animal signals. Ethology, 93(3), 177–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Machlis, L., Dodd, P. W. D. and Fentress, J. C. (1985). The pooling fallacy: problems arising when individuals contribute more than one observation to the data set. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 68(3), 201–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maciej, P., Fischer, J. and Hammerschmidt, K. (2011). Transmission characteristics of primate vocalizations: implications for acoustic analyses. PLoS One, 6(8), e23015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacKay, D. M. (1972). Formal analysis of communicative processes. In Hinde, R. A. (ed.), Non-verbal Communication. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 3–26.Google Scholar
MacKinnon, J. (1974). The behaviour and ecology of wild orang-utans (Pongo pygmaeus). Animal Behaviour, 22(1), 3–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacNeilage, P. F., Studdert-Kennedy, M. G. and Lindblom, B. (1987). Primate handedness reconsidered. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 10(2), 247–303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maestripieri, D. (1996a). Gestural communication and its cognitive implications in pigtail macaques (Macaca nemestrina). Behaviour, 133(13–14), 997–1022.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maestripieri, D. (1996b). Social communication among captive stump-tailed macaques (Macaca arctoides). International Journal of Primatology, 17(5), 785–802.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maestripieri, D. (1997). Gestural communication in macaques: Usage and meaning of nonvocal signals. Evolution of Communication, 1(2), 193–222.Google Scholar
Maestripieri, D. (1999). Primate social organization, gestural repertoire size, and communication dynamics: a comparative study of macaques. In King, B. J. (ed.), The Evolution of Language: Assessing the Evidence from Nonhuman Primates. Santa Fe, NM: School of American Research, pp. 55–77.Google Scholar
Maille, A., Engelhart, L., Bourjade, M. and Blois-Heulin, C. (2012). To beg, or not to beg? That is the question: mangabeys modify their production of requesting gestures in response to human’s attentional states. PLoS One, 7(7), e41197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mann, J. (2000). Unraveling the dynamics of social life: long term studies and observational methods. In Mann, J., Connor, R. C., Tyack, P. L. and Whitehead, H. (eds), Cetacean Societies: Field Studies of Dolphins and Whales. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press, pp. 45–64.Google Scholar
Manser, M. B., Seyfarth, R. M. and Cheney, D. L. (2002). Suricate alarm calls signal predator class and urgency. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(2), 55–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mariani, C., Spinnler, H., Sterzi, R. and Vallar, G. (1980). Bilateral perisylvian softenings: bilateral anterior opercular syndrome (Foix–Chavany–Marie syndrome). Journal of Neurology, 223(4), 269–284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marler, P. (1965). Communication in monkeys and apes. In DeVore, I. (ed.), Primate Behavior: Field Studies of Monkeys and Apes. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, pp. 544–584.Google Scholar
Marler, P. (1976). Social organization, communication and graded signals: the chimpanzee and the gorilla. In Bateson, P. P. G. and Hinde, R. A. (eds), Growing Points. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 239–280.Google Scholar
Marler, P. (1977). The structure of animal communication sounds. In Bullock, T. (ed.), Recognition of complex acoustic signals. Berlin: Dahlem Konferenzen, pp. 17–35.Google Scholar
Marler, P., Evans, C. S. and Hauser, M. D. (1992). Animal signals: Motivational, referential, or both? In Papoušek, H. and Jürgens, U. (Eds.), Nonverbal Vocal Communication: Comparative and Developmental Approaches. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 66–86.Google Scholar
Marshall, A. J., Wrangham, R. W. and Arcadi, A. C. (1999). Does learning affect the structure of vocalizations in chimpanzees?Animal Behaviour, 58(4), 825–830.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, P. and Bateson, P. (2007). Measuring Behaviour: An Introductory Guide (3rd edn). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, R. D. (1973). A review of the behaviour and ecology of the lesser mouse lemur (Microcebus murinus J.F. Miller, 1777). In Michael, R. P. and Crook, J. H. (eds), Comparative Ecology and Behaviour of Primates. London: Academic Press, pp. 1–68.Google Scholar
Martin, R. D. (1990). Primate Origins and Evolution: A Phylogenetic Reconstruction. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Martin, R. D. and Bearder, S. K. (1979). Radio bush baby. Natural History, 88, 76–81.Google Scholar
Mason, W. A. (1960). The effects of social restriction on the behavior of rhesus monkeys: I. Free social behavior. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 53(6), 582–589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masterton, B., Heffner, H. and Ravizza, R. (1969). The evolution of human hearing. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 45, 966–985.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matsumoto-Oda, A., Oda, R., Hayashi, Y. et al. (2003). Vaginal fatty acids produced by chimpanzees during menstrual cycles. Folia Primatologica, 74(2), 75–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matsumura, S. (1999). The evolution of ‘egalitarian’ and ‘despotic’ social systems among macaques. Primates, 40(1), 23–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayr, E. (1961). Cause and effect in biology. Science, 134(3489), 1501–1506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayr, E. (1963). Animal Species and Evolution. Cambridge, MA: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayr, E. (1969). Principles of Systematic Zoology. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
McComb, K. and Semple, S. (2005). Coevolution of vocal communication and sociality in primates. Biology Letters, 1(4), 381–385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDowell, J. (1994). Mind and World. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
McGrew, W. C. and Tutin, C. E. G. (1978). Evidence for a social custom in wild chimpanzees?Man, 13, 234–251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGurk, H. and MacDonald, J. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature, 264(5588), 746–748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNeill, D. (2002). Gesture and language dialectic. Acta Linguistica Hafniensia, 34(1), 7–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mech, L. D., Adams, L. G., Meier, T. J., Burch, J. W. and Dale, B. W. (1998). The Wolves of Denali. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Meguerditchian, A. and Vauclair, J. (2006). Baboons communicate with their right hand. Behavioural Brain Research, 171(1), 170–174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meguerditchian, A., Gardner, M. J., Schapiro, S. J. and Hopkins, W. D. (2012). The sound of one-hand clapping: handedness and perisylvian neural correlates of a communicative gesture in chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279(1735), 1959–1966.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meguerditchian, A., Molesti, S. and Vauclair, J. (2011). Right-handedness predominance in 162 baboons (Papio anubis) for gestural communication: Consistency across time and groups. Behavioral Neuroscience, 125(4), 653–660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meguerditchian, A., Vauclair, J. and Hopkins, W. D. (2010). Captive chimpanzees use their right hand to communicate with each other: implications for the origin of the cerebral substrate for language. Cortex, 46(1), 40–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mehu, M., Grammer, K. and Dunbar, R. I. M. (2007). Smiles when sharing. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28(6), 415–422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meredith, M. (1991). Sensory processing in the main and accessory olfactory systems: comparisons and contrasts. The Journal of Steroid Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, 39(4), 601–614.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meredith, M. A. and Stein, B. E. (1983). Interactions among converging sensory inputs in the superior colliculus. Science, 221(4608), 389–391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meredith, M. A. and Stein, B. E. (1986). Spatial factors determine the activity of multisensory neurons in cat superior colliculus. Brain Research, 365(2), 350–354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meunier, H., Prieur, J. and Vauclair, J. (2013). Olive baboons communicate intentionally by pointing. Animal Cognition, 16(2), 155–163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michael, R. P. and Zumpe, D. (1982). Influence of olfactory signals on the reproductive behaviour of social groups of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Journal of Endocrinology, 95(2), 189–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Micheletta, J., Engelhardt, A., Matthews, L. E. E., Agil, M. and Waller, B. M. (2013). Multicomponent and multimodal lipsmacking in crested macaques (Macaca nigra). American Journal of Primatology, 75(7), 763–773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michelsen, A., Andersen, B. B., Storm, J., Kirchner, W. H. and Lindauer, M. (1992). How honeybees perceive communication dances, studied by means of a mechanical model. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 30(3), 143–150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miklósi, Á. and Soproni, K. (2006). A comparative analysis of animals’ understanding of the human pointing gesture. Animal Cognition, 9(2), 81–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miles, H. L. (1983). Apes and language: the search for communicative competence. In de Luce, J. and Wilder, H. T. (eds), Language in Primates. Perspectives and Implications. New York: Springer, pp. 43–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miles, H. L. (1990). The cognitive foundations for reference in a signing orangutan. In Parker, S. T. and Gibson, K. R. (eds), Language and Intelligence in Monkeys and Apes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 511–539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, L. E. and Treves, A. (2007). Predation on primates: Past studies, current challenges, and directions for the future. In Campbell, C. J., Fuentes, A., MacKinnon, J., Panger, M. and Bearder, S. K. (eds), Primates in Perspective. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 525–542.Google Scholar
Miller, R. E., Caul, W. F. and Mirsky, I. A. (1967). Communication of affects between feral and socially isolated monkeys. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7(3), 231–239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milton, K. (1975). Urine-rubbing behaviour in the mantled howler monkey Alouatta palliata. Folia Primatologica, 23(1–2), 105–112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitani, J. C. (1988). Male gibbon (Hylobates agilis) singing behavior: natural history, song variations and function. Ethology, 79(3), 177–194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitani, J. C. and Brandt, K. L. (1994). Social factors influence the acoustic variability in the long-distance calls of male chimpanzees. Ethology, 96(3), 233–252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitani, J. C. and Gros-Louis, J. (1998). Chorusing and call convergence in chimpanzees: tests of three hypotheses. Behaviour, 135, 1041–1064.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitani, J. C. and Marler, P. (1989). A phonological analysis of male gibbon singing behavior. Behaviour, 109, 20–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitani, J. C. and Nishida, T. (1993). Contexts and social correlates of long-distance calling by male chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 45, 735–746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitani, J. C., Hasegawa, T., Gros-Louis, J., Marler, P. and Byrne, R. (1992). Dialects in wild chimpanzees?American Journal of Primatology, 27(4), 233–243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitani, J. C., Hunley, K. L. and Murdoch, M. E. (1999). Geographic variation in the calls of wild chimpanzees: a reassessment. American Journal of Primatology, 47(2), 133–151.3.0.CO;2-I>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, R. W. and Anderson, J. R. (1997). Pointing, withholding information, and deception in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 111(4), 351–361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Møller, G. W., Harlow, H. F., and Mitchell, G. D. (1968). Factors affecting agonistic communication in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Behaviour, 31(3/4), 339–357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mollon, J. D. (1989). ‘Tho’ she kneel’d in that place where they grew. . .’ The uses and origins of primate colour vision. Journal of Experimental Biology, 146(1), 21–38.Google Scholar
Moore, C. and Corkum, V. (1994). Social understanding at the end of the first year of life. Developmental Review, 14(4), 349–372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, R. (in press). Ontogenetic constraints on Grice's Theory of Communication. In Matthews, D. (ed.), Pragmatic Development in First Language Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Morecraft, R. J., Louie, J. L., Herrick, J. L. and Stilwell-Morecraft, K. S. (2001). Cortical innervation of the facial nucleus in the non-human primate. Brain, 124(1), 176–208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morimoto, Y. and Fujita, K. (2011). Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) modify their own behaviors according to a conspecific’s emotional expressions. Primates, 52(3), 279–286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrill, R. J., Paukner, A., Ferrari, P. F. and Ghazanfar, A. A. (2012). Monkey lipsmacking develops like the human speech rhythm. Developmental Science, 15(4), 557–568.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morton, E. S. (1975). Ecological sources of selection on avian sounds. The American Naturalist, 109(965), 17–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mulcahy, N. J. and Call, J. (2009). The performance of bonobos (Pan paniscus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) in two versions of an object-choice task. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 123(3), 304–309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mulcahy, N. J. and Hedge, V. (2012). Are great apes tested with an abject object-choice task?Animal Behaviour, 83(2), 313–321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Munger, S. D., Leinders-Zufall, T. and Zufall, F. (2009). Subsystem organization of the mammalian sense of smell. Annual Review of Physiology, 71, 115–140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Munoz, N. E. and Blumstein, D. T. (2012). Multisensory perception in uncertain environments. Behavioral Ecology, 23(3), 457–462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myowa, M. (1996). Imitation of facial gestures by an infant chimpanzee. Primates, 37(2), 207–213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myowa-Yamakoshi, M., Tomonaga, M., Tanaka, M. and Matsuzawa, T. (2004). Imitation in neonatal chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Developmental Science, 7(4), 437–442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narins, P. M. and Capranica, R. R. (1976). Sexual differences in the auditory system of the tree frog Eleutherodactylus coqui. Behavioral and Brain Science, 192(4237), 378–380.Google ScholarPubMed
Narins, P. M., Grabul, D. S., Soma, K. K., Gaucher, P. and Hödl, W. (2005). Cross-modal integration in a dart-poison frog. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(7), 2425–2429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nieuwenhuys, R., Donkelaar, H. J. and Nicholson, C. (eds). (1998). The Central Nervous System of Vertebrates, Vol. 3. Heidelberg, Germany: Springer Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (1968). The social group of wild chimpanzees in the Mahali Mountains. Primates, 9(3), 167–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (1979). The Social Structure of the Chimpanzees of the Mahali Mountains. Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin-Cummings Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. (1980). The leaf-clipping display: a newly discovered expressive gesture in wild chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution, 9(2), 117–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. and Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M. (1987). Chimpanzees and bonobos: Cooperative relationships among males. In Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W. and Struhsaker, T. T. (eds), Primate Societies. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press, pp. 165–177.Google Scholar
Nishida, T., Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M., Hasegawa, T. and Takahata, Y. (1985). Group extinction and female transfer in wild chimpanzees in the Mahale National Park, Tanzania. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 67(1–4), 284–301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Notman, H. and Rendall, D. (2005). Contextual variation in chimpanzee pant hoots and its implications for referential communication. Animal Behaviour, 70(1), 177–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Connell, S. (1994). Joint attention in chimpanzees, bonobos and squirrel monkeys. Paper presented at the Hang Seng Center for Cognitive Studies conference on Theories of Mind.
Olzanowslei, M., Pochwatko, G., Kukliński, K., Ścibor-Rylski, M. and Ohme, R. (2008). Warsaw set of emotional facial expression pictures: validation study. Opatija, Croatia: EAESP General Meeting.
Onishi, K. H., Baillargeon, R. and Leslie, A. M. (2007). 15-month-old infants detect violations in pretend scenarios. Acta Psychologica, 124(1), 106–128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ouattara, K., Lemasson, A. and Zuberbühler, K. (2009a). Campbell’s monkeys use affixation to alter call meaning. PLoS One, 4(11), e7808.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ouattara, K., Lemasson, A. and Zuberbühler, K. (2009b). Campbell’s monkeys concatenate vocalizations into context-specific call sequences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(51), 22026–22031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owings, D. H. and Virginia, R. A. (1978). Alarm calls of California ground squirrels (Spermophilus beecheyi). Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 46(1), 58–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owren, M. J. and Rendall, D. (2001). Sound on the rebound: Bringing form and function back to the forefront in understanding nonhuman primate vocal signaling. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 10(2), 58–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owren, M. J., Dieter, J. A., Seyfarth, R. M. and Cheney, D. L. (1992). ‘Food’ calls produced by adult female rhesus (Macaca mulatta) and Japanese (M. fuscata) macaques, their normally-raised offspring, and offspring cross-fostered between species. Behaviour, 120(3/4), 218–231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owren, M. J., Dieter, J. A., Seyfarth, R. M. and Cheney, D. L. (1993). Vocalizations of rhesus (Macaca mulatta) and Japanese (M. fuscata) macaques cross-fostered between species show evidence of only limited modification. Developmental Psychobiology, 26(7), 389–406.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palagi, E. and Mancini, G. (2011). Playing with the face: playful facial ‘chattering’ and signal modulation in a monkey species (Theropithecus gelada). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 125(1), 11–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palagi, E. and Norscia, I. (2009). Multimodal signaling in wild Lemur catta: Economic design and territorial function of urine marking. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 139(2), 182–192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palagi, E., Dapporto, L., and Borgognini Tarli, S. B. (2005). The neglected scent: on the marking function of urine in Lemur catta. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 58(5), 437–445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palombit, R. A. (1996). Pair bonds in monogamous apes: a comparison of the siamang Hylobates syndactylus and the white-handed gibbon Hylobates lar. Behaviour, 133, 321–356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Panksepp, J. (1998). The periconscious substrates of consciousness: affective states and the evolutionary origins of the SELF. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 5(5–6), 566–582.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. and Burgdorf, J. (2003). ‘Laughing’ rats and the evolutionary antecedents of human joy?Physiology and Behavior, 79(3), 533–547.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parker, S. T. and Gibson, K. R.(eds) (1990). Language and Intelligence in Monkeys and Apes: Comparative Developmental Perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parr, L. A. (2001). Cognitive and physiological markers of emotional awareness in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Animal Cognition, 4(3–4), 223–229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parr, L. A. (2004). Perceptual biases for multimodal cues in chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) affect recognition. Animal Cognition, 7(3), 171–178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parr, L. A. and Hecht, E. E. (2011). Facial perception in non-human primates. In Calder, A. J., Rhodes, G., Johnston, M. H. and Haxby, J. V. (eds), The Oxford Handbook of Face Perception. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 691–706.Google Scholar
Parr, L. A. and Waller, B. M. (2006). Understanding chimpanzee facial expression: insights into the evolution of communication. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1(3), 221–228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parr, L. A., Cohen, M. and de Waal, F. B. M. (2005a). Influence of social context on the use of blended and graded facial displays in chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology, 26(1), 73–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parr, L. A., Hecht, E., Barks, S. K., Preuss, T. M. and Votaw, J. R. (2009). Face processing in the chimpanzee brain. Current Biology, 19(1), 50–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parr, L. A., Hopkins, W. D. and de Waal, F. B. M. (1998). The perception of facial expressions by chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Evolution of Communication, 2(1), 1–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parr, L. A., Waller, B. M., Burrows, A. M., Gothard, K. M. and Vick, S. J. (2010). Brief communication. MaqFACS: a muscle-based facial movement coding system for the rhesus macaque. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 143(4), 625–630.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parr, L. A., Waller, B. M. and Fugate, J. (2005b). Emotional communication in primates: implications for neurobiology. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15(6), 716–720.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parr, L. A., Waller, B. M. and Heintz, M. (2008). Facial expression categorization by chimpanzees using standardized stimuli. Emotion, 8(2), 216–231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parr, L. A., Waller, B. M., Vick, S. J. and Bard, K. A. (2007). Classifying chimpanzee facial expressions using muscle action. Emotion, 7(1), 172–181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Partan, S. and Marler, P. (1999). Communication goes multimodal. Science, 283(5406), 1272–1273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Partan, S., Yelda, S., Price, V. and Shimizu, T. (2005). Female pigeons, Columba livia, respond to multisensory audio/video playbacks of male courtship behaviour. Animal Behaviour, 70(4), 957–966.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Partan, S. R. (1998). Multimodal communication: The integration of visual and acoustic signals by macaques. Unpublished PhD thesis, University of California, Davis.Google Scholar
Partan, S. R. (2002). Single and multichannel signal composition: Facial expressions and vocalizations of rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Behaviour, 139, 993–1028.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Partan, S. R. and Marler, P. (2005). Issues in the classification of multimodal communication signals. The American Naturalist, 166(2), 231–245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Partan, S. R., Fulmer, A. G., Gounard, M. A. M. and Redmond, J. E. (2010). Multimodal alarm behavior in urban and rural gray squirrels studied by means of observation and a mechanical robot. Current Zoology, 56(3), 313–326.Google Scholar
Partan, S. R., Larco, C. P. and Owens, M. J. (2009). Wild tree squirrels respond with multisensory enhancement to conspecific robot alarm behaviour. Animal Behaviour, 77(5), 1127–1135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patel, E. R. and Girard-Buttoz, C. (2008). Non-nutritive tree gouging in wild Milne-Edwards’ sifakas (Propithecus edwardsi): description and potential communicative functions. Primate Eye, 96, 283.Google Scholar
Patterson, F. G. (1978). The gestures of a gorilla: language acquisition in another pongid. Brain and Language, 5(1), 72–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patterson, F. G. (1980). Innovative uses of language by a gorilla: A case study. In Nelson, K. E. (ed.), Children’s Language, Vol. 2. New York: Gardner Press, pp. 497–561.Google Scholar
Patterson, F. (1981). Ape language. Science, 211(4477), 86–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patterson, F. G. and Linden, F. (1981). The Education of Koko. New York: Holt, Rinehardt and Winston.Google Scholar
Paukner, A. and Anderson, J. R. (2006). Video-induced yawning in stumptail macaques (Macaca arctoides). Biology Letters, 2(1), 36–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paukner, A., Ferrari, P. F. and Suomi, S. J. (2011). Delayed imitation of lipsmacking gestures by infant rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). PLoS One, 6(12), e28848.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pelé, M., Dufour, V., Thierry, B. and Call, J. (2009). Token transfers among great apes (Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus, Pan paniscus, and Pan troglodytes): species differences, gestural requests, and reciprocal exchange. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 123(4), 375–384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penn, D. J. and Potts, W. K. (1999). The evolution of mating preferences and major histocompatibility complex genes. The American Naturalist, 153(2), 145–164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pereira, M. E. and Macedonia, J. M. (1991). Ringtailed lemur anti-predator calls denote predator class, not response urgency. Animal Behaviour, 41(3), 543–544.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peres, C. A. (1989). Costs and benefits of territorial defense in wild golden lion tamarins, Leontopithecus rosalia. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 25(3), 227–233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perret, M. (1995). Chemocommunication in the reproductive function of mouse lemurs. In Alterman, L., Doyle, G. A. and Izard, M. K. (eds), Creatures of the Dark: The Nocturnal Prosimians. New YorkPlenum Press, pp. 377–392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perrett, D. I. and Mistlin, A. J. (1990). Perception of facial characteristics by monkeys. In Stebbins, W. C. and Berkley, M. A. (eds), Comparative Perception, Complex Signals, Vol 2. New York: Wiley, pp. 187–215.Google Scholar
Perry, S. (2011). Social traditions and social learning in capuchin monkeys (Cebus). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 988–996.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perry, S. and Manson, J. H. (2003). Traditions in monkeys. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 12(2), 71–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petersen, M. R., Beecher, M. D., Zoloth, S. R., Green, S., Marler, P. R., Moody, D. B. and Stebbins, W. C. (1984). Neural lateralization of vocalizations by Japanese macaques: Communicative significance is more important than acoustic structure. Behavioral Neuroscience, 98(5), 779–790.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, M. R., Beecher, M. D., Zoloth, S. R., Moody, D. B. and Stebbins, W. C. (1978). Neural lateralization of species-specific vocalizations by Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Science, 202(4365), 324–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petkov, C. I., Kayser, C., Steudel, T., Whittingstall, K., Augath, M. and Logothetis, N. K. (2008). A voice region in the monkey brain. Nature Neuroscience, 11(3), 367–374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrulis, A., Peng, M. and Johnston, R. E. (1999). Effects of vomeronasal organ removal on individual odor discrimination, sex-odor preference, and scent marking by female hamsters. Physiology & Behavior, 66(1), 73–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pfefferle, D., Brauch, K., Heistermann, M., Hodges, J. K. and Fischer, J. (2008). Female Barbary macaque (Macaca sylvanus) copulation calls do not reveal the fertile phase but influence mating outcome. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 275(1634), 571–578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pfefferle, D., Heistermann, M., Hodges, J. K. and Fischer, J. (2008). Male Barbary macaques eavesdrop on mating outcome: a playback study. Animal Behaviour, 75(6), 1885–1891.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, K. A., Buzzell, C. A., Holder, N. and Sherwood, C. C. (2011). Why do capuchin monkeys urine wash? An experimental test of the sexual communication hypothesis using fMRI. American Journal of Primatology, 73(6), 578–584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pierce, Jr, J. D. (1985). A review of attempts to condition operantly alloprimate vocalizations. Primates, 26(2), 202–213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pika, S. and Mitani, J. (2006). Referential gestural communication in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Current Biology, 16(6), R191–R192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pika, S., Liebal, K. and Tomasello, M. (2003). Gestural communication in young gorillas (Gorilla gorilla): gestural repertoire, learning, and use. American Journal of Primatology, 60(3), 95–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pika, S., Liebal, K. and Tomasello, M. (2005). Gestural communication in subadult bonobos (Pan paniscus): repertoire and use. American Journal of Primatology, 65(1), 39–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pilcher, D. L., Hammock, E. A. D. and Hopkins, W. D. (2001). Cerebral volumetric asymmetries in non-human primates: a magnetic resonance imaging study. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 6(2), 165–179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plooij, F. X. (1978). Some basic traits of language in wild chimpanzees? In Lock, A. (ed.), Action, Gesture and Symbol: The Emergence of Language. London: Academic Press, pp. 111–131.Google Scholar
Plooij, F. X. (1979). How wild chimpanzee babies trigger the onset of mother-infant play – and what the mother makes of it. In Bullowa, M. (ed.), Before Speech: The Beginning of Interpersonal Communication. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 223–244.Google Scholar
Plooij, F. X. (1984). The behavioral development of free-living chimpanzee babies and infants. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Pollard, K. A. and Blumstein, D. T. (2012). Evolving communicative complexity: Insights from rodents and beyond. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1597), 1869–1878.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollick, A. S. and de Waal, F. B. M. (2007). Ape gestures and language evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(19), 8184–8189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poremba, A., Malloy, M., Saunders, R. C., Carson, R. E., Herscovitch, P. and Mishkin, M. (2004). Species-specific calls evoke asymmetric activity in the monkey’s temporal poles. Nature, 427(6973), 448–451.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porter, R. H. (1998). Olfaction and human kin recognition. Genetica, 104(3), 259–263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poss, S., Kuhar, C., Stoinski, T. S. and Hopkins, W. D. (2006). Differential use of attentional and visual communicative signaling by orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) and gorillas (Gorilla gorilla) in response to the attentional status of a human. American Journal of Primatology, 68(10), 978–992.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Povinelli, D. J., Bering, J. M. and Giambrone, S. (2000). Towards a science of other minds: escaping the argument of analogy. Cognitive Science, 24(3), 501–541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Povinelli, D. J. and Eddy, T. J. (1996). Factors influencing young chimpanzees’ (Pan troglodytes) recognition of attention. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 110(4), 336–345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., Bierschwale, D. T. and Cech, C. G. (1999). Comprehension of seeing as a referential act in young children, but not juvenile chimpanzees. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17(1), 37–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Powzyk, J. A. and Mowry, C. B. (2003). Dietary and feeding differences between sympatric Propithecus diadema diadema and Indri indri. International Journal of Primatology, 24(6), 1143–1162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Premack, D. (1971). Language in chimpanzees. Science, 172, 808–822.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Premack, D. and Premack, A. J. (1983). The Mind of an Ape: Norton New York.Google Scholar
Preuschoft, S. (1992). Laughter and smile in Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus). Ethology, 91(3), 220–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preuschoft, S. (2004). Power and communication. In Thierry, B., Singh, M. and Kaumanns, W. (eds) Macaque Societies: A Model for the Study of Social Organization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 56–60.Google Scholar
Preuschoft, S. and van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (1995). Homologizing primate facial displays: a critical review of methods. Folia Primatologica, 65(3), 121–137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pullen, S. L., Bearder, S. K. and Dixson, A. F. (2000). Preliminary observations on sexual behavior and the mating system in free-ranging lesser galagos (Galago moholi). American Journal of Primatology, 51(1), 79–88.3.0.CO;2-B>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rahlfs, M. and Fichtel, C. (2010). Anti-predator behaviour in a nocturnal primate, the grey mouse lemur (Microcebus murinus). Ethology, 116(5), 429–439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rajanarayanan, S. and Archunan, G. (2011). Identification of urinary sex pheromones in female buffaloes and their influence on bull reproductive behaviour. Research in Veterinary Science, 91(2), 301–305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rasoloharijaona, S., Randrianambinina, B. and Joly-Radko, M. (2010). Does nonnutritive tree gouging in a rainforest-dwelling lemur convey resource ownership as does loud calling in a dry forest-dwelling lemur?American Journal of Primatology, 72(12), 1062–1072.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rauschecker, J. P. and Scott, S. K. (2009). Maps and streams in the auditory cortex: nonhuman primates illuminate human speech processing. Nature Neuroscience, 12(6), 718–724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rauschecker, J. P. and Tian, B. (2000). Mechanisms and streams for processing of ‘what’ and ‘where’ in auditory cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97(22), 11800–11806.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redshaw, M. and Locke, K. (1976). The development of play and social behaviour in two lowland gorilla infants. Journal of the Jersey Wildlife Preservation Trust, 13th Annual Report, 71–86.
Reichard, U. H. and Boesch, C. (eds) (2003). Monogamy Mating Strategies and Partnerships in Birds, Humans and other Mammals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reichert, K. E., Heistermann, M., Hodges, J. K., Boesch, C. and Hohmann, G. (2002). What females tell males about their reproductive status: Are morphological and behavioural cues reliable signals of ovulation in bonobos (Pan paniscus)?Ethology, 108(7), 583–600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rekwot, P. I., Ogwu, D., Oyedipe, E. O. and Sekoni, V. O. (2001). The role of pheromones and biostimulation in animal reproduction. Animal Reproduction Science, 65(3–4), 157–170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rendall, D., Owren, M. J. and Ryan, M. J. (2009). What do animal signals mean?Animal Behaviour, 78(2), 233–240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Restrepo, D., Arellano, J., Oliva, A. M., Schaefer, M. L. and Lin, W. (2004). Emerging views on the distinct but related roles of the main and accessory olfactory systems in responsiveness to chemosensory signals in mice. Hormones and Behavior, 46(3), 247–256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riede, T., Bronson, E., Hatzikirou, H. and Zuberbühler, K. (2005). Vocal production mechanisms in a non-human primate: morphological data and a model. Journal of Human Evolution, 48(1), 85–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rijksen, H. D. (1978). A Fieldstudy on Sumatran Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus abelii, Lesson 1827): Ecology, Behaviour And Conservation. Wageningen, the Netherlands: Veenman, H. and Zonen B. V.Google Scholar
Rilling, J. K. (2006). Human and nonhuman primate brains: are they allometrically scaled versions of the same design?Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 15(2), 65–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rinberg, D. and Gelperin, A. (2006). Olfactory neuronal dynamics in behaving animals. Seminars in Cell and Developmental Biology, 17(4), 454–461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rinberg, D., Koulakov, A. and Gelperin, A. (2006). Sparse odor coding in awake behaving mice. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(34), 8857–8865.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rinn, W. E. (1984). The neuropsychology of facial expression: a review of the neurological and psychological mechanisms for producing facial expressions. Psychological Bulletin, 95(1), 52–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rivas, E. (2005). Recent use of signs by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in interactions with humans. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119(4), 404–417.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzolatti, G. and Arbib, M. A. (1998). Language within our grasp. Trends in Neuroscience, 21(5), 188–194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzolatti, G. and Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V. and Fogassi, L. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research, 3(2), 131–141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L. and Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(9), 661–670.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, A. I., Vick, S.-J. and Buchanan-Smith, H. M. (2012a). Usage and comprehension of manual gestures in wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 84(2), 459–470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, A. I., Vick, S.-J., Roberts, S. G. B., Buchanan-Smith, H. M. and Zuberbühler, K. (2012b). A structure-based repertoire of manual gestures in wild chimpanzees: statistical analyses of a graded communication system. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(5), 578–589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, J. A., Taylor, P. W. and Uetz, G. W. (2007). Consequences of complex signaling: predator detection of multimodal cues. Behavioral Ecology, 18(1), 236–240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, J. G. (1979). An analysis of the organization of vocal communication in the titi monkey Callicebus moloch. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 49(4), 381–405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, J. G. (1984). Syntactic structures in the vocalizations of wedge-capped capuchin monkeys, Cebus olivaceus. Behaviour, 90(1/3), 46–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, J. G., Wright, P. C. and Kinzey, W. G. (1987). Monogamous cebids and their relatives: intergroup calls and spacing. In Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W. and Struhsaker, T. T. (eds), Primate Societies. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press, pp. 44–53.Google Scholar
Rogers, L. J. and Kaplan, G. (2000). Songs, Roars, and Rituals: Communication in Birds, Mammals, and Other Animals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Romanski, L. M. (2012). Integration of faces and vocalizations in ventral prefrontal cortex: Implications for the evolution of audiovisual speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(Supplement 1), 10717–10724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenthal, G. G., Rand, A. S. and Ryan, M. J. (2004). The vocal sac as a visual cue in anuran communication: an experimental analysis using video playback. Animal Behaviour, 68(1), 55–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, C. F. (1995). Allometric and functional influences on primate orbit orientation and the origins of the Anthropoidea. Journal of Human Evolution, 29(3), 201–227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosvold, H. E., Mirsky, A. F. and Pribram, K. H. (1954). Influence of amygdalectomy on social behavior in monkeys. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 47(3), 173–178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roush, R. S. and Snowdon, C. T. (2000). Quality, quantity, distribution and audience effects on food calling in cotton-top tamarins. Ethology, 106(8), 673–690.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rowe, C. (1999). Receiver psychology and the evolution of multicomponent signals. Animal Behaviour, 58(5), 921–931.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, C. and Guilford, T. (1996). Hidden colour aversions in domestic chicks triggered by pyrazine odours of insect warning displays. Nature, 383(6600), 520–522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rumbaugh, D. M. and Gill, T. V. (1977). Lana’s acquisition of language skills. In Rumbaugh, D. M. (ed.), Language Learning by a Chimpanzee: The Lana Project. New York: Academic Press, pp. 165–192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rumbaugh, D. M., Warner, H. and von Glasersfeld, E. (1977). The LANA project: Origins and tactics. In Rumbaugh, D. M. (ed.), Language Learning by a Chimpanzee: The Lana Project. New York: Academic Press, pp. 87–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, C. L., Bard, K. A. and Adamson, L. B. (1997). Social referencing by young chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 111(2), 185–191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, J. L., Lyn, H., Schaeffer, J. A. and Hopkins, W. D. (2011). The role of socio-communicative rearing environments in the development of social and physical cognition in apes. Developmental Science, 14(6), 1459–1470.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rylands, A. B. (1986). Ranging behaviour and habitat preference of a wild marmoset group, Callithrix humeralifer (Callitrichidae, Primates). Journal of Zoology, 210(4), 489–514.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rylands, A. B. (1993). Marmosets and Tamarins: Systematics, Behaviour, and Ecology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Savage, A., Ziegler, T. E. and Snowdon, C. T. (1988). Sociosexual development, pair bond formation, and mechanisms of fertility suppression in female cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus oedipus). American Journal of Primatology, 14(4), 345–359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. and Lewin, R. (1994). Kanzi: The Ape at the Brink of the Human Mind. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Murphy, J., Sevcik, R. A., Brakke, K. E., Williams, S. L., Rumbaugh, D. M. and Bates, E. (1993). Language comprehension in ape and child. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58(314), 1–252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. and Rumbaugh, D. M. (1998). Perspectives on consciousness, language, and other emergent processes in apes and humans. In Hameroff, S. J., Kaszniak, A. W. and Scott, A. C. (eds), Towards a Science of Consciousness II: The Second Tuscon Discussions and Debates. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 533–549.Google Scholar
Scarantino, A. (2010). Animal communication between information and influence. Animal Behaviour, 79(6), E1–E5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaller, G. B. (1963). The Mountain Gorilla: Ecology and Behavior. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Schaller, G. B. (1972). The Serengeti Lion: A Study of Predator – Prey Relations. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Schel, A. M., Candiotti, A. and Zuberbühler, K. (2010). Predator-deterring alarm call sequences in Guereza colobus monkeys are meaningful to conspecifics. Animal Behaviour, 80(5), 799–808.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schel, A., Machanda, Z., Townsend, S., Zuberbühler, K. and Slocombe, K. (in press). Chimpanzee food calls are directed at specific individuals. Animal Behaviour.
Schel, A., Townsend, S., Machanda, Z., Zuberbühler, K. and Slocombe, K. (in press). Chimpanzee alarm call production fulfills three key criteria for intentionality. PLoS One.
Schel, A. M., Tranquilli, S. and Zuberbühler, K. (2009). The alarm call system of two species of black-and-white colobus monkeys (Colobus polykomos and Colobus guereza). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 123(2), 136–150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schenk, T. and McIntosh, R. D. (2010). Do we have independent visual streams for perception and action?Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(1), 52–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schilling, A. (1974). Olfactory communication in prosimians. In Doyle, G. A. and Martin, R. D. (eds), The Study of Prosimian Behavior. New York: Academic Press, pp. 461–542.Google Scholar
Schilling, A. (1979). A study of the marking behavior in Lemur catta. In Martin, R. D., Doyle, G. A. and Walker, A. C. (eds.), Prosimian Biology. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press, pp. 347–362.Google Scholar
Schilling, A., Perret, M. and Predine, J. (1984). Sexual inhibition in a prosimian primate: a pheromone-like effect. Journal of Endocrinology, 102(2), 143–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, C., Call, J. and Liebal, K. (2012a). Onset and early use of gestural communication in nonhuman great apes. American Journal of Primatology, 74(2), 102–113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, C., Call, J. and Liebal, K. (2012b). What role do mothers play in the gestural acquisition of bonobos (Pan paniscus) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)?International Journal of Primatology, 33(1), 246–262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schultz, A. H. (1969). The Life of Primates. London: Weidenfeld and Nicholson.Google Scholar
Scott-Phillips, T. C. (2010). Animal communication: insights from linguistic pragmatics. Animal Behaviour, 79(1), e1–e4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott-Phillips, T. C., Blythe, R. A., Gardner, A. and West, S. A. (2012). How do communication systems emerge?Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279(1735), 1943–1949.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott-Phillips, T. C., Dickins, T. E. and West, S. A. (2011). Evolutionary theory and the ultimate-proximate distinction in the human behavioral sciences. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(1), 38–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sebeok, T. A. and Sebeok, J. U. (eds) (1991). Speaking of Apes: A Critical Anthology of Two-way Communication with Man. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Semendeferi, K., Lu, A., Schenker, N. and Damásio, H. (2002). Humans and great apes share a large frontal cortex. Nature neuroscience, 5(3), 272–276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Semple, S. (2001). Individuality and male discrimination of female copulation calls in the yellow baboon. Animal Behaviour, 61(5), 1023–1028.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Semple, S. and McComb, K. (2000). Perception of female reproductive state from vocal cues in a mammal species. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 267(1444), 707–712.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Semple, S., McComb, K., Alberts, S. and Altmann, J. (2002). Information content of female copulation calls in yellow baboons. American Journal of Primatology, 56(1), 43–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sereno, M. I. and Allman, J. M. (1991). Cortical visual areas in mammals. In Leventhal, A. G. (ed.) The Neural Basis of Visual Function. London: Macmillan, pp. 160–172.Google Scholar
Sereno, M. I., Dale, A. M., Reppas, J. B. et al. (1995). Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Science, 268(5212), 889–893.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Setchell, J. M., Vaglio, S., Moggi-Cecchi, J. et al. (2010). Chemical composition of scent-gland secretions in an Old World monkey (Mandrillus sphinx): influence of sex, male status, and individual identity. Chemical Senses, 35(3), 205–220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seyama, J. and Nagayama, R. S. (2002). Perceived eye size is larger in happy faces than in surprised faces. Perception, 31(9), 1153–1155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seyfarth, R. M. and Cheney, D. L. (1986). Vocal development in vervet monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 34(6), 1640–1658.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M. and Cheney, D. L. (1990). The assessment by vervet monkeys of their own and another species' alarm calls. Animal Behaviour, 40(4), 754–764.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M. and Cheney, D. L. (1993). Meaning, reference, and intentionality in the natural vocalizations of monkeys. In Roitblat, H. H., Herman, L. M. and Nachtigall, P. E. (eds), Language and Communication: Comparative Perspectives. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 195–220.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L. and Bergman, T. J. (2005). Primate social cognition and the origins of language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(6), 264–266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L., Bergman, T., Fischer, J., Zuberbühler, K. and Hammerschmidt, K. (2010). The central importance of information in studies of animal communication. Animal Behaviour, 80(1), 3–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L. and Marler, P. (1980a). Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. Science, 210(4471) 801–803.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L. and Marler, P. (1980b). Vervet monkey alarm calls: semantic communication in a free-ranging primate. Animal Behaviour, 28(4), 1070–1094.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shannon, C. E. (1948). A mathematical theory of communication. Bell System Technical Journal, 27(3), 379–423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sherman, R. W., Jarvis, J. U. M. and Alexander, R. D. (1991). The Biology of the Naked Mole-rat. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Sherwood, C. C., Hof, P. R., Holloway, R. L. et al. (2005). Evolution of the brainstem orofacial motor system in primates: a comparative study of trigeminal, facial and hypoglossal nuclei. Journal of Human Evolution, 48(1), 45–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sherwood, C. C., Holloway, R. L., Erwin, J. M. and Hof, P. R. (2004a). Cortical orofacial motor representation in Old World monkeys, great apes, and humans: II. Stereologic analysis of chemoarchitecture. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 63(2), 82–106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sherwood, C. C., Holloway, R. L., Erwin, J. M. et al. (2004b). Cortical orofacial motor representation in Old World monkeys, great apes, and humans: I. Quantitative analysis of cytoarchitecture. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 63(2), 61–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shultz, S. and Dunbar, R. I. M. (2007). The evolution of the social brain: Anthropoid primates contrast with other vertebrates. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 274(1624), 2429–2436.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shultz, S., Opie, C. and Atkinson, Q. D. (2011). Stepwise evolution of stable sociality in primates. Nature, 479(7372), 219–222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simpson, G. G. (1961). Principles of Animal Taxonomy. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Simpson, J. A. and Weiner, E. S. C. (eds). (1998). The Oxford English Dictionary (2nd edn), Vol. VI. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Slocombe, K. E. and Zuberbühler, K. (2005a). Functionally referential communication in a chimpanzee. Current Biology, 15(19), 1779–1784.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slocombe, K. E. and Zuberbühler, K. (2005b). Agonistic screams in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) vary as a function of social role. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119(1), 67–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slocombe, K. E. and Zuberbühler, K. (2006). Food-associated calls in chimpanzees: responses to food types or food preferences?Animal Behaviour, 72(5), 989–999.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slocombe, K. E. and Zuberbühler, K. (2007). Chimpanzees modify recruitment screams as a function of audience composition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(43), 17228–17233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slocombe, K. E. and Zuberbühler, K. (2010). Vocal communication in chimpanzees. In Lonsdorf, E., Ross, S. and Matsuzawa, T. (eds), The Mind of the Chimpanzee Ecological and Empirical Perspectives. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press, pp. 192–207.Google Scholar
Slocombe, K. E, Kaller, T., Call, J. and Zuberbühler, K. (in prep). Chimpanzee food-calls refer to specific food items in captivity: Evidence from a playback study.
Slocombe, K. E., Kaller, T., Call, J. and Zuberbühler, K. (2010a). Chimpanzees extract social information from agonistic screams. PLoS One, 5(7), e11473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slocombe, K. E., Kaller, T., Turman, L., Townsend, S. W., Papworth, S., Squibbs, P. and Zuberbühler, K. (2010b). Production of food-associated calls in wild male chimpanzees is dependent on the composition of the audience. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 64(12), 1959–1966.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slocombe, K. E., Townsend, S. W. and Zuberbühler, K. (2009). Wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) distinguish between different scream types: evidence from a playback study. Animal Cognition, 12(3), 441–449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slocombe, K. E., Waller, B. M. and Liebal, K. (2011). The language void: the need for multimodality in primate communication research. Animal Behaviour, 81(5), 919–924.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smear, M., Shusterman, R., O’Connor, R., Bozza, T. and Rinberg, D. (2011). Perception of sniff phase in mouse olfaction. Nature, 479(7373), 397–400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, C. L. and Evans, C. S. (2008). Multimodal signaling in fowl, Gallus gallus. Journal of Experimental Biology, 211(13), 2052–2057.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, C. L. and Evans, C. S. (2009). Silent tidbitting in male fowl, Gallus gallus: A referential visual signal with multiple functions. Journal of Experimental Biology, 212(6), 835–842.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, C. L., Taylor, A. and Evans, C. S. (2011). Tactical multimodal signalling in birds: facultative variation in signal modality reveals sensitivity to social costs. Animal Behaviour, 82(3), 521–527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, T. (2006). Individual olfactory signatures in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). American Journal of Primatology, 68(6), 585–604.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, T. D. and Bhatnagar, K. P. (2004). Microsmatic primates: Reconsidering how and when size matters. The Anatomical Record Part B: The New Anatomist, 279B(1), 24–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, T. D. and Rossie, J. B. (2006). Primate olfaction: anatomy and evolution. In Brewer, W. J., Castle, D. and Pantelis, C. (eds), Olfaction and the Brain. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 135–166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, T. D., Dennis, J. C., Bhatnagar, K. P. et al. (2011). Olfactory marker protein expression in the vomeronasal neuroepithelium of tamarins (Saguinus spp.). Brain Research, 1375, 7–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, T. D., Rossie, J. B. and Bhatnagar, K. P. (2007). Evolution of the nose and nasal skeleton in primates. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 16(4), 132–146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, T. D., Siegel, M. I. and Bhatnagar, K. P. (2001). Reappraisal of the vomeronasal system of catarrhine primates: ontogeny, morphology, functionality, and persisting questions. The Anatomical Record, 265(4), 176–192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, W. J. (1977). The Behavior of Communicating: An Ethological Approach. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Smith, W. J. (1981). Referents of animal communication. Animal Behaviour, 29(4), 1273–1275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snowdon, C. T. and Elowson, A. M. (1999). Pygmy marmosets modify call structure when paired. Ethology, 105(10), 893–908.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spocter, M. A., Hopkins, W. D., Garrison, A. R. et al. (2010). Wernicke’s area homologue in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and its relation to the appearance of modern human language. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 277(1691), 2165–2174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stammbach, E. (1987). Desert, forest and montane baboons: Multi-level societies. In Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W. and Struhsaker, T. T. (eds), Primate Societies. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press, pp. 112–120.Google Scholar
Stein, B. E. and Meredith, M. A. (1993). The Merging of the Senses. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Sterck, E. H. M., Watts, D. P. and van Schaik, C. P. (1997). The evolution of female social relationships in nonhuman primates. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 41(5), 291–309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sterling, E. J. and Richard, A. F. (1995). Social organization in the aye-aye (Daubentonia madagascarensis) and the perceived distinctiveness of nocturnal primates. In Alterman, L., Doyle, G. A. and Izard, M. K. (eds), Creatures of the Dark: The Nocturnal Prosimians. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 439–451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, J. M. G., Vervaecke, H., de Vries, H. and van Elsacker, L. (2005). Peering is not a formal indicator of subordination in bonobos (Pan paniscus). American Journal of Primatology, 65(3), 255–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stoddart, D. M. (1980). The Ecology of Vertebrate Olfaction. London: Chapman and Hall.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strier, K. B. (1992). Atelinae adaptations: behavioral strategies and ecological constraints. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 88(4), 515–524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Struhsaker, T. T. (1967). Auditory communication among vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). In Altmann, S. A. (ed.), Social Communication Among Primates. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press, pp. 281–324.Google Scholar
Struhsaker, T. T. (1975). The Red Colobus Monkeys. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Struhsaker, T. T. and Leland, L. (1987). Colobines: infanticide by adult males. In Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W. and Struhsaker, T. T. (eds), Primate Societies. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press, pp. 83–97.Google Scholar
Struhsaker, T. T. and Leland, L. (1988). Group fission in redtail monkeys (Cercopithecus ascanius) in the Kibale Forest, Uganda. In Gautier-Hion, A., Bourlière, F., Gautier, J. P. and Kingdon, J. (eds.), A Primate Radiation: Evolutionary Biology of the African Guenons. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 364–388.Google Scholar
Stumpf, R. (2007). Chimpanzees and bonobos: diversity within and between species. In Campbell, C. J., Fuentes, A., MacKinnon, K. C., Panger, M. and Bearder, S. K. (eds), Primates in Perspective. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 321–344.Google Scholar
Sugihara, T., Diltz, M. D., Averbeck, B. B. and Romanski, L. M. (2006). Integration of auditory and visual communication information in the primate ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26(43), 11138–11147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Surridge, A. K., Osorio, D. and Mundy, N. I. (2003). Evolution and selection of trichromatic vision in primates. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 18(4), 198–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Susman, R. L. (ed.). (1984). The Pygmy Chimpanzee: Evolutionary Biology and Behavior. New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sussman, R. W. (1999). Primate Ecology and Social Structure. Volume 1: Lorises, Lemurs and Tarsiers. Needham Heights, MA: Pearson Custom Publishing.Google Scholar
Sussman, R. W. and Kinzey, W. G. (1984). The ecological role of the Callitrichidae: a review. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 64(4), 419–449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutton, D., Larson, C., Taylor, E. M. and Lindeman, R. C. (1973). Vocalization in rhesus monkeys: conditionability. Brain Research, 52, 225–231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swaney, W. T. and Keverne, E. B. (2009). The evolution of pheromonal communication. Behavioural Brain Research, 200(2), 239–247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taglialatela, J. P. (2007). Functional and structural asymmetries for auditory perception and vocal production in nonhuman primates. Special Topics in Primatology, 5, 120–145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taglialatela, J. P., Cantalupo, C. and Hopkins, W. D. (2006). Gesture handedness predicts asymmetry in the chimpanzee inferior frontal gyrus. Neuroreport, 17(9), 923–927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taglialatela, J. P., Reamer, L., Schapiro, S. J. and Hopkins, W. D. (2012). Social learning of a communicative signal in captive chimpanzees. Biology Letters, 8(4), 498–501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taglialatela, J. P., Russell, J. L., Schaeffer, J. A. and Hopkins, W. D. (2008). Communicative signaling activates ‘Broca’s’ homologue in chimpanzees. Current Biology, 18(5), 343–348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taglialatela, J. P., Russell, J. L., Schaeffer, J. A. and Hopkins, W. D. (2009). Visualizing vocal perception in the chimpanzee brain. Cerebral Cortex, 19(5), 1151–1157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taglialatela, J. P., Russell, J. L., Schaeffer, J. A. and Hopkins, W. D. (2011). Chimpanzee vocal signaling points to a multimodal origin of human language. PLoS One, 6(4), e18852.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taglialatela, J. P., Savage-Rumbaugh, S. and Baker, L. A. (2003). Vocal production by a language-competent Pan paniscus. International Journal of Primatology, 24(1), 1–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tanner, J. E. and Byrne, R. W. (1993). Concealing facial evidence of mood: perspective-taking in a captive gorilla. Primates, 34(4), 451–457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tanner, J. E. (1998). Gestural Communication in a Group of Zoo-living Lowland Gorillas. Unpublished Dissertation, University of St Andrews.Google Scholar
Tanner, J. E. (2004). Gestural phrases and gestural exchanges by a pair of zoo-living lowland gorillas. Gesture, 4(1), 1–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tardif, S. D., Harrison, M. L. and Simek, M. A. (1993). Communal infant care in marmosets and tamarins: relation to energetics, ecology, and social organization. In Rylands, A. B. (ed.), Marmosets and Tamarins: Systematics, Behaviour, and Ecology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 220–234.Google Scholar
Tardif, S. D., Santos, C. V., Baker, A. J., Van Elsacker, L., Feistner, A. T. C., Kleiman, D. G., . . . and De Vleeschouwer, K. (2002). Infant care in lion tamarins. In Kleiman, D. G. and Rylands, A. B. (eds), Lion Tamarins: Biology and Conservation. Washington DC: Smithsonian Institution Press, pp. 213–232.Google Scholar
Tardif, S. D., Smucny, D. A., Abbott, D. H., Mansfield, K., Schultz-Darken, N. and Yamamoto, M. E. (2003). Reproduction in captive common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). Comparative Medicine, 53(4), 364–368.Google Scholar
Taylor, R. C., Klein, B. A., Stein, J. and Ryan, M. J. (2008). Faux frogs: multimodal signalling and the value of robotics in animal behaviour. Animal Behaviour, 76(3), 1089–1097.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, R. C., Klein, B. A., Stein, J. and Ryan, M. J. (2011). Multimodal signal variation in space and time: how important is matching a signal with its signaler?Journal of Experimental Biology, 214(5), 815–820.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tempelmann, S. and Liebal, K. (2012). Spontaneous use of gesture sequences in orangutans: A case for strategy? In Pika, S. and Liebal, K. (eds), Recent Developments in Primate Gesture Research. Herndon VA: John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 73–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tempelmann, S., Kaminski, J. and Liebal, K. (2011). Focus on the essential: all great apes know when others are being attentive. Animal Cognition, 14(3), 433–439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tempelmann, S., Kaminski, J. and Liebal, K. (2013). When apes point the finger – Three great apes species fail to use a conspecific’s imperative pointing gesture. Interaction Studies, 14(1), 7–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Terborgh, J. and Goldizen, A. W. (1985). On the mating system of the cooperatively breeding saddle-backed tamarin (Saguinus fuscicollis). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 16(4), 293–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Terrace, H. S. (1979). Nim: A Chimpanzee Who learned Sign Language. New York: Columbia Press.Google Scholar
Terrace, H. S., Petitto, L. A., Sanders, R. J. and Bever, T. G. (1979). Can an ape create a sentence. Science, 206(4421), 891–902.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teufel, C., Gutmann, A., Pirow, R. and Fischer, J. (2010). Facial expressions modulate the ontogenetic trajectory of gaze-following among monkeys. Developmental Science, 13(6), 913–992.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Theall, L. A. and Povinelli, D. J. (1999). Do chimpanzees tailor their gestural signals to fit the attentional states of others?Animal Cognition, 2(4), 207–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thierry, B. (2007). Unity in diversity: lessons from macaque societies. Evolutionary Anthropology, 16(6), 224–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thierry, B. (2008). Primate socioecology, the lost dream of ecological determinism. Evolutionary Anthropology, 17(2), 93–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thierry, B., Demaria, C., Preuschoft, S. and Desportes, C. (1989). Structural convergence between silent bared-teeth display and relaxed open-mouth display in the Tonkean macaque (Macaca tonkeana). Folia Primatologica, 52(3–4), 178–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thierry, B., Iwaniuk, A. N. and Pellis, S. M. (2000). The influence of phylogeny on the social behaviour of macaques (Primates: Cercopithecidae, genus Macaca). Ethology, 106(8), 713–728.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (1952). ‘Derived’ activities; their causation, biological significance, origin, and emancipation during evolution. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 27(1), 1–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 20(4), 410–433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tirindelli, R., Dibattista, M., Pifferi, S. and Menini, A. (2009). From pheromones to behavior. Physiological Reviews, 89(3), 921–956.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1996). Do apes ape? In Heyes, C. and Galef, B. (eds), Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, pp. 319–346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of Human Communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. and Call, J. (2004). The role of humans in the cognitive development of apes revisited. Animal Cognition, 7(4), 213–215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. and Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10(1), 121–125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. and Zuberbühler, K. (2002). Primate vocal and gestural communication. In Bekoff, M., Allen, C. S. and Burghardt, G. (eds), The Cognitive Animal: Empirical and Theoretical Perspectives on Animal Cognition. Cambridge: MIT Press, pp. 293–299.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Call, J., Nagell, K., Olguin, R. and Carpenter, M. (1994). The learning and use of gestural signals by young chimpanzees: a trans-generational study. Primates, 35(2), 137–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Call, J., Warren, J. et al. (1997). The ontogeny of chimpanzee gestural signals: A comparison across groups and generations. Evolution of Communication, 1(2), 223–259.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T. and Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(5), 675–735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M. and Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78(3), 705–722.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., George, B. L., Kruger, A. C., Farrar, M. J. and Evans, E. (1985). The development of gestural communication in young chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution, 14(2), 175–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Gust, D. and Frost, G. T. (1989). A longitudinal investigation of gestural communication in young chimpanzees. Primates, 30(1), 35–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomonaga, M. (2010). Do the chimpanzee eyes have it? Social cognition on the basis of gaze and attention from the comparative–cognitive–developmental perspective. In Lornsdorf, E. V., Ross, S. R. and Matsuzawa, T. (eds), The Mind of the Chimpanzee: Ecological and Empirical Perspectives. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press, pp. 42–53.Google Scholar
Townsend, S. W. and Manser, M. B. (2013). Functionally referential communication in mammals: the past, present and the future. Ethology, 119(1), 1–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Townsend, S. W. and Zuberbühler, K. (2009). Audience effects in chimpanzee copulation calls. Communicative and Integrative Biology, 2(3), 282–284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Townsend, S. W., Deschner, T. and Zuberbühler, K. (2008). Female chimpanzees use copulation calls flexibly to prevent social competition. PLoS One, 3(6), e2431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Townsend, S. W., Deschner, T. and Zuberbühler, K. (2011). Copulation calls in female chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) convey identity but do not accurately reflect fertility. International Journal of Primatology, 32(4), 914–923.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsao, D. Y., Freiwald, W. A., Tootell, R. B. H. and Livingstone, M. S. (2006). A cortical region consisting entirely of face-selective cells. Science, 311(5761), 670–674.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsao, D. Y., Moeller, S. and Freiwald, W. A. (2008). Comparing face patch systems in macaques and humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(49), 19514–19519.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uetz, G. W., Roberts, J. A. and Taylor, P. W. (2009). Multimodal communication and mate choice in wolf spiders: female response to multimodal versus unimodal signals. Animal Behaviour, 78(2), 299–305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uhlenbroek, C. (1995). The structure and function of the long-distance calls given by male chimpanzees in Gombe National Park. Unpublished Ph.D. Thesis, University of Bristol.Google Scholar
Ungerleider, G. and Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual systems. In Ingle, D. J., Goodale, M. A. and R. J. W. Mansfield (eds), Analysis of Visual Behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 549–586.Google Scholar
van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (1967). The facial displays of the catarrhine monkeys and apes. In Morris, D. (ed.), Primate Ethology. London: Weidenfeld and Nicholson, pp. 7–68.Google Scholar
van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (1972). A comparative approach to the phylogeny of laughter and smiling. In Hinde, R. A. (ed.), Non-verbal Communication. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 209–237.Google Scholar
van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (1973). A structural analysis of the social behaviour of a semi-captive group of chimpanzees. In von Cranach, M. and Vine, I. (eds), Social Communication and Movement: Studies of Interaction and Expression in Man and Chimpanzee. London and New York: Academic Press, pp. 75–162.Google Scholar
van Lawick-Goodall, J. (1968). The behaviour of free-living chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Reserve. Animal Behaviour Monographs, 1(3), 163–311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Leeuwen, E. J. C., Cronin, K. A., Haun, D. B. M., Mundry, R. and Bodamer, M. D. (2012). Neighbouring chimpanzee communities show different preferences in social grooming behaviour. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279(1746), 4362–4367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Roosmalen, M. G. M. and Klein, L. L. (1988). The spider monkeys, genus Ateles. In Mittermeier, R. A., Rylands, A. B., Coimbra-Filho, A. F. and de Fonseca, G. A. B. (eds), Ecology and Behavior of Neotropical Primates, Vol. 2. Washington DC: World Wildlife Fund, pp. 455–537.Google Scholar
van Schaik, C. P. (1983). Why are diurnal primates living in groups?Behaviour, 87(1/2), 120–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Schaik, C. P. (1989). The ecology of social relationships amongst female primates. In Standen, V. and Foley, R. A. (eds), Comparative Socioecology The Behavioral Ecology of Humans and Other Animals. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications, pp. 195–218.Google Scholar
van Schaik, C. P. (1999). The socioecology of fission–fusion sociality in orangutans. Primates, 40(1), 69–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Schaik, C. P. and van Hooff, J. (1983). On the ultimate causes of primate social systems. Behaviour, 85(1–2), 91–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veà, J. J. and Sabater-Pi, J. (1998). Spontaneous pointing behaviour in the wild pygmy chimpanzee (Pan paniscus). Folia Primatologica, 69(5), 289–290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vick, S. J., Waller, B. M., Parr, L. A., Smith Pasqualini, M. C. and Bard, K. A. (2007). A cross-species comparison of facial morphology and movement in humans and chimpanzees using the Facial Action Coding System (FACS). Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 31(1), 1–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Visalberghi, E. and Fragaszy, D. M. (2002). ‘Do monkeys ape?’ Ten years after. In Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. (eds), Imitation in Animals and Artefacts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 471–499.Google Scholar
Visalberghi, E., Valenzano, D. R. and Preuschoft, S. (2006). Facial displays in Cebus apella. International Journal of Primatology, 27(6), 1689–1707.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
von Frisch, K. (1974). Decoding the language of the bee. Science, 185(4152), 663–668.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waller, B. M. and Cherry, L. (2012). Facilitating play through communication: significance of teeth exposure in the gorilla play face. American Journal of Primatology, 74(2), 157–164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waller, B. M. and Dunbar, R. I. M. (2005). Differential behavioural effects of silent bared teeth display and relaxed open mouth display in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Ethology, 111(2), 129–142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waller, B. M. and Micheletta, J. (2013). Facial expressions in nonhuman animals. Emotion Review, 5(1), 54–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waller, B. M., Bard, K. A., Vick, S.-J. and Smith Pasqualini, M. C. (2007). Perceived differences between chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and human (Homo sapiens) facial expressions are related to emotional interpretation. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 121(4), 398–404.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waller, B. M., Cray, J. and Burrows, A. M. (2008). Selection for universal emotion. Emotion, 8(3), 435–439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waller, B. M., Lembeck, M., Kuchenbuch, P., Burrows, A. M. and Liebal, K. (2012). GibbonFACS: A muscle-based facial movement coding system for hylobatids. International Journal of Primatology, 33(4), 809–821.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waller, B. M., Liebal, K., Burrows, A. M. and Slocombe, K. E. (2013a). How can a multimodal approach to primate communication helps us to understand the evolution of communication?Evolutionary Psychology, 11(3), 538–549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waller, B. M., Parr, L., Gothard, K., Burrows, A. M. and Fuglevand, A. (2008). Mapping the contribution of single muscles to facial movements in the rhesus macaque. Physiology and Behavior, 95(1–2), 93–100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waller, B. M., Vick, S.-J., Parr, L. A. et al. (2006). Intramuscular electrical stimulation of facial muscles in humans and chimpanzees: Duchenne revisited and extended. Emotion, 6(3), 367–382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waller, B. M., Warmelink, L., Liebal, K., Micheletta, J. and Slocombe, K. E. (2013b). Pseudoreplication: a widespread problem in primate communication research. Animal Behaviour, 86(2), 483–488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallez, C., Schaeffer, J., Meguerditchian, A. et al. (2012). Contrast of hemispheric lateralization for oro-facial movements between learned attention-getting sounds and species-typical vocalizations in chimpanzees: extension in a second colony. Brain and Language, 123(1), 75–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walls, G. L. (1942). The Vertebrate Eye and its Adaptive Radiation. Oxford: Cranbrook Institute of Science.Google Scholar
Wang, X. (2000). On cortical coding of vocal communication sounds in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97(22), 11843–11849.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waser, P. M. and Brown, C. H. (1986). Habitat acoustics and primate communication. American Journal of Primatology, 10(2), 135–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watzlawick, P., Bavelas, J. B. and Jackson, D. D. (1967). Pragmatics of Human Communication: A Study of Interactional Patterns, Pathologies, and Paradoxes: New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Wedekind, C. and Furi, S. (1997). Body odour preferences in men and women: do they aim for specific MHC combinations or simply heterozygosity?Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 264(1387), 1471–1479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wedekind, C., Seebeck, T., Bettens, F. and Paepke, A. J. (1995). MHC-dependent mate preferences in humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 260(1359), 245–249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wheeler, B. C. (2010). Production and perception of situationally variable alarm calls in wild tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella nigritus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 64(6), 989–1000.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wheeler, B. C. and Fischer, J. (2012). Functionally referential signals: A promising paradigm whose time has passed. Evolutionary Anthropology, 21(5), 195–205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whinnett, A. and Mundy, N. I. (2003). Isolation of novel olfactory receptor genes in marmosets (Callithrix): insights into pseudogene formation and evidence for functional degeneracy in non-human primates. Gene, 304, 87–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C. et al. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399(6737), 682–685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitham, J. C., Gerald, M. S. and Maestripieri, D. (2007). Intended receivers and functional significance of grunt and girney vocalizations in free-ranging female rhesus macaques. Ethology, 113(9), 862–874.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wich, S. A. and de Vries, H. (2006). Male monkeys remember which group members have given alarm calls. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 273(1587), 735–740.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wich, S. A. and Sterck, E. H. (2003). Possible audience effect in Thomas langurs (Primates; Presbytis thomasi): an experimental study on male loud calls in response to a tiger model. American Journal of Primatology, 60(4), 155–159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wich, S. A., Krützen, M., Lameira, A. R. et al. (2012). Call cultures in orang-utans?PLoS One, 7(5), e36180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wich, S. A., Swartz, K. B., Hardus, M. E. et al. (2009). A case of spontaneous acquisition of a human sound by an orangutan. Primates, 50(1), 56–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilcox, S. (1999). The invention and ritualization of language. In King, B. J. (ed.), The Origins of Language: What Nonhuman Primates Can Tell Us, Vol. 351. Santa Fe, NM: School of American Research Press, pp. 351–384.Google Scholar
Wilson, M. L. and Wrangham, R. W. (2003). Intergroup relations in chimpanzees. Annual Review of Anthropology, 32, 363–392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winter, P., Handley, P., Ploog, D. and Schott, D. (1973). Ontogeny of squirrel monkey calls under normal conditions and under acoustic isolation. Behaviour, 47(3/4), 230–239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wollberg, Z. and Newman, J. D. (1972). Auditory cortex of squirrel monkey: response patterns of single cells to species-specific vocalizations. Science, 175(4018), 212–214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woo, K. L. and Rieucau, G. (2011). From dummies to animations: a review of computer-animated stimuli used in animal behavior studies. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 65(9), 1671–1685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodruff, G. and Premack, D. (1979). Intentional communication in the chimpanzee: The development of deception. Cognition, 7(4), 333–362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wrangham, R. W. (1980). An ecological model of female-bonded primate groups. Behaviour, 75(3/4), 262–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wrangham, R. W. (1987). Evolution of social structure. In Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W. and Struhsaker, T. T. (eds), Primate Societies. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press, pp. 282–296.Google Scholar
Yahr, P. and Stephens, D. R. (1987). Hormonal control of sexual and scent marking behaviors of male gerbils in relation to the sexually dimorphic area of the hypothalamus. Hormones and Behavior, 21(3), 331–346.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yarger, R. G., Smith, A. B., Preti, G. and Epple, G. (1977). The major volatile constituents of the scent mark of a South American primate Saguinus fuscicollis, Callitrichidae. Journal of Chemical Ecology, 3(1), 45–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, A. W., Hellawell, D. and Hay, D. C. (1987). Configurational information in face perception. Perception, 16(6), 747–759.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, J. Z. (1957). The Life of Mammals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Young, J. Z. (1962). The Life of Vertebrates. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, M. P. and Yamane, S. (1992). Sparse population coding of faces in the inferotemporal cortex. Science, 256(5061), 1327–1331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zald, D. H. and Pardo, J. V. (1997). Emotion, olfaction, and the human amygdala: amygdala activation during aversive olfactory stimulation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 94(8), 4119–4124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zatorre, R. J. and Belin, P. (2001). Spectral and temporal processing in human auditory cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 11(10), 946–953.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zatorre, R. J., Belin, P. and Penhune, V. B. (2002). Structure and function of auditory cortex: Music and speech. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(1), 37–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ziegler, T. E., Snowdon, C. T. and Uno, H. (1990). Social interactions and determinants of ovulation on tamarins (Saguinus). In Ziegler, T. and Berkovitch, F. (eds), Socioendocrinology of Primate Reproduction. New York: Wiley Liss, pp. 113–133.Google Scholar
Zimmermann, F., Zemke, F., Call, J. and Gómez, J. C. (2009). Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) and bonobos (Pan paniscus) point to inform a human about the location of a tool. Animal Cognition, 12(2), 347–358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2000a). Interspecies semantic communication in two forest primates. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 267(1444), 713–718.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuberbühler, K. (2000b). Referential labelling in Diana monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 59(5), 917–927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuberbühler, K. (2001). Predator-specific alarm calls in Campbell’s monkeys, Cercopithecus campbelli. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 50(5), 414–422.Google Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2002). A syntactic rule in forest monkey communication. Animal Behaviour, 63(2), 293–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2005). The phylogenetic roots of language evidence from primate communication and cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(3), 126–130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuberbühler, K., Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (1999). Conceptual semantics in a nonhuman primate. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113(1), 33–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuberbühler, K., Noë, R. and Seyfarth, R. M. (1997). Diana monkey long-distance calls: messages for conspecifics and predators. Animal Behaviour, 53(3), 589–604.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×