Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-r6qrq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-28T17:50:52.069Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

22 - Personal Control and Successful Aging

from Part II - Psychosocial Factors

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 January 2019

Rocío Fernández-Ballesteros
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid
Athanase Benetos
Affiliation:
Université de Lorraine and Institut national de la santé et de la recherche médicale (INSERM) Nancy
Jean-Marie Robine
Affiliation:
INSERM
Get access

Summary

A broad range of studies conducted over the past 50 years suggests that the perception of personal control is an important construct to successful aging. When people feel that they can exert control, they tend to experience better psychological and physical health. For example, those who experience perceived control demonstrate better immune responses, cardiovascular functioning, physical strength, increased life satisfaction, and decreased anxiety and depressive symptoms. We will describe the literature surrounding the relationship between perceived control and successful aging. We will also survey the literature on self-efficacy, mastery, attributional style, and locus of control, which have all often been used interchangeably with the notion of perceived control. We will also discuss how perceived control can be maximized through the lens of mindfulness without meditation (Langerian mindfulness). In this framework, mindfulness is defined as the simple process of noticing new things, a process that promotes flexible responding to the demands of the environment and perception of control. Both lack of perceived control and mindlessness are rooted in rigidity and a view of the world as unchangeable. We will present suggestions about how the simple act of noticing new things improves the perception of control and facilitates successful aging.
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adler, A. (1927). Understanding Human Nature. New York: Greenburg.Google Scholar
Affleck, G., Tennen, H., Croog, S., et al. (1987). Causal attribution, perceived control, and recovery from a heart attack. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 5(3), 339–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Avorn, J., Langer, E. (1982). Induced disability in nursing home patients: a controlled trial. Journal of American Geriatric Society, 30, 397400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baltes, M. M., Wahl, H. W. (1992). The dependency-support script in institutions: generalization to community settings. Psychology and Aging, 7(3), 409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unified theory of behavioral change. Psychological Reviews, 84, 191215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonnesen, J. L., Burgess, E. O. (2004). Senior moments: The acceptability of an ageist phrase. Journal of Aging Studies, 18(2), 123–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carmon, Z., Ariely, D. (2000). Focusing on the forgone: How value can appear so different to buyers and sellers. Journal of Consumer Research, 27(3), 360–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chanowitz, B., Langer, E. (1980). Knowing more (or less) than you can show: Understanding control through the mindlessness-mindfulness distinction. In Seligman, M. E. P. & Garber, J. (Eds.), Human Helplessness: Theory and Applications. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Chipperfield, J. G., Greenslade, L. (1999). Perceived control as a buffer in the use of health care services. Psychological Sciences, 54B(3), 146–54.Google Scholar
Compas, B. E., Boyer, M. C., Stanger, C., et al. (2006). Latent variable analysis of coping, anxiety/depression, and somatic symptoms in adolescents with chronic pain. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 1132–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cook, S., Marsiske, M. (2006). Subjective memory beliefs and cognitive performance in normal and mildly impaired older adults. Aging and Mental Health, 10(4), 413–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuddy, A. J., Fiske, S. T. (2004). Doddering but dear: Process, content, and function in stereotyping of older persons. In Nelson, T. D. (Ed.), Ageism: Stereotyping and Prejudice Against Older Persons (pp. 326). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cuddy, A. J., Fiske, S. T., Glick, P. (2008). Warmth and competence as universal dimensions of social perception: The stereotype content model and the BIAS map. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 61149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehrlinger, J., Dunning, D. (2003). How chronic self-views influence (and potentially mislead) estimates of performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ercoli, L., Siddarth, P., Huang, S. C., et al. (2006). Perceived loss of memory ability and cerebral metabolic decline in persons with the apolipoprotein E-IV genetic risk for Alzheimer disease. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(4), 442–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., Glick, P., et al. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(6), 878.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenfield, E. A., Marks, N. F. (2004). Formal volunteering as a protective factor for older adults' psychological well-being. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 59(5), S258S264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayflick, L. (2007). Entropy explains aging, genetic determinism explains longevity, and undefined terminology explains misunderstanding both. PLoS Genetics, 3(12), e220. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.0030220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henslin, J. M. (1967). Craps and magic. American Journal of Sociology, 316–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hsu, L. M., Chung, J., Langer, E. J. (2010). The influence of age-related cues on health and longevity. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 632–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isaacs, L. W., Bearison, D. J. (1986). The development of children's prejudice against the aged. The International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 23(3), 175–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kozela, M., Dorynska, A., Stepaniak, U., et al. (2015). Perceived control as a predictor of cardiovascular disease mortality in Poland: The Hapiee study. Cardiology Journal, 22, 404–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lachman, M. E., Firth, K. M. (2004). The adaptive value of feeling in control during midlife. In Brim, O. G., Ryff, C. D. & Kessler, R. (Eds.), How healthy are we? A national study of well-being at midlife (pp. 320–49). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Langer, E. J. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32(2), 311–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langer, E. J. (1979). The illusion of incompetence. In Perlmuter, L. & Monty, R. (Eds.), Choice and Perceived Control. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Langer, E. J. (1989a). Minding matters: The consequences of mindlessness mindfulness. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 22(12), 137–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langer, E. J. (1989b). Mindfulness. Reading: Addison-Wesley/Addison Wesley Longman.Google Scholar
Langer, E. J. (2009). Counterclockwise: Mindful health and the power of possibility. New York, NY: Ballantine Books.Google Scholar
Langer, E., Roth, J. (1975). Heads I win, tails it's chance: The illusion of control as a function of the sequence of outcomes in a purely chance task. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 951–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langer, E. J., Rodin, J. (1976). The effects of choice and enhanced personal responsibility for the aged: a field experiment in an institutional setting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34(2), 191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langer, E. J., Moldoveanu, M. (2000). The construct of mindfulness. Journal of Social Issues, 56(1), 19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langer, E. J., Janis, I. L., Wolfer, J. A. (1975). Reduction of psychological stress in surgical patients. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 11(2), 155–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langer, E., Blank, A., Chanowitz, B. (1978). The mindlessness of ostensibly thoughtful action: The role of placebic information in interpersonal interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 635–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langer, E., Rodin, J., Beck, P., et al. (1979). Environmental determinants of memory improvement in late adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 2003–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langer, E., Perlmuter, L., Chanowitz, B., et al. (1988). Two new applications of mindlessness theory: Aging and alcoholism. Journal of Aging Studies, 2, 289–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langer, E., Chanowitz, B., Jacobs, S., et al. (1990). Nonsequential development and aging. In Alexander, C. & Langer, E. (Eds.), Higher Stages of Human Development. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Levy, B. (2009). Stereotype embodiment a psychosocial approach to aging. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(6), 332–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, B., Langer, E. (1994). Aging free from negative stereotypes: Successful memory in China among the American deaf. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(6), 989.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, B. R., Banaji, M. R. (2004). Implicit ageism. In Nelson, T.D. (Ed.), Ageism: Stereotyping and prejudice against older persons (pp. 4975). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Levy, B. R., Slade, M. D., Kunkel, S. R., et al. (2002). Longevity increased by positive self-perceptions of aging. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(2), 261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, B. R., Pilver, C., Chung, P. H., et al. (2014). Subliminal strengthening improving older individuals’ physical function over time with an implicit-age-stereotype intervention. Psychological Science, 25(12), 2127–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, B. R., Ferrucci, L., Zonderman, A. B., et al. (2016). A culture–brain link: Negative age stereotypes predict Alzheimer's disease biomarkers. Psychology and Aging, 31(1), 82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marottoli, R. A., de Leon, C. F. M., Glass, T. A., et al. (2000). Consequences of driving cessation decreased out-of-home activity levels. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 55(6), S334S340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marquié, J. C., Jourdan-Boddaert, L., Huet, N. (2002). Do older adults underestimate their actual computer knowledge?. Behaviour and Information Technology, 21(4), 273–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAndrew, L. M., Horowitz, C. R., Lancaster, K. J., et al. (2010). Factors related to perceived diabetes control are not related to actual glucose control for minority patients with diabetes. Diabetes Care, 33(4), 736–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKay, R. T., Dennett, D. C. (2009). The evolution of misbelief. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(06), 493510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mellor, D., Hayashi, Y., Firth, L., et al. (2008). Volunteering and well-being: Do self-esteem, optimism, and perceived control mediate the relationship?. Journal of Social Service Research, 34(4), 6170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monty, R. A., Rosenberger, M. A., Perlmuter, L. C. (1973). Amount and locus of choice as sources of motivation in paired-associate learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 97, 1621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myers, J. L., Fort, J. G. (1963). Sequential analysis of gambling behavior. Journal of General Psychology, 69, 299309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, T. D. (2004). Ageism: Stereotyping and prejudice against older persons. MIT press.Google Scholar
Ng, R., Allore, H. G., Trentalange, M., et al. (2015). Increasing negativity of age stereotypes across 200 years: evidence from a database of 400 million words. PloS One, 10(2), e0117086.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pagnini, F., Phillips, D., Bosma, C. M., et al. (2015). Mindfulness, physical impairment and psychological well-being in people with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Psychology and Health, 30(5), 503–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pagnini, F., Phillips, D., Bosma, C. M., et al. (2016). Mindfulness as a protective factor for the burden of caregivers of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis patients. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 72(1), 101–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penley, J. A., Tomaka, J., Wiebe, J. S. (2002). The association of coping to physical and psychological health outcomes: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 25(6), 551603.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perlmuter, L., Langer, E. (1983). The effects of behavioral monitoring on the perception of control. The Clinical Gerontologist, 1, 3743.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, C., Stunkard, A. J. (1992). Cognates of personal control: Locus of control, self-efficacy, and explanatory style. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 1, 111–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pew Research Center Social and Demographics Trends. (2009). Growing old in America: Expectations vs. reality. Washington, DC. http://assets.pewresearch.org/wp-content/uploads/sites/3/2010/10/Getting-Old-in-America.pdf.Google Scholar
Presson, P. K., Benassi, V. A. (1996). Illusion of control: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 11(3), 493510.Google Scholar
Rodin, J., Langer, E. J. (1977). Long-term effects of a control-relevant intervention with the institutionalized aged. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35(12), 897.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodin, J., Langer, E. J. (1980). Aging labels: The decline of control and the fall of self-esteem. Journal of Social Issues, 36(2), 1229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothbaum, F., Weisz, J. R., Snyder, S. S. (1982). Changing the world and changing the self: A two-process model of perceived control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42(1), 5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothermund, K. (2005). Effects of age stereotypes on self views and adaptation. In Greve, W., Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (Eds.), The adaptive self: Personal continuity and intentional self-development (pp. 223242). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe & Huber.Google Scholar
Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Journal of Educational Research, 74(3), 185–90.Google Scholar
Rowe, J. W., Kahn, R. L. (1987). Human aging: Usual and successful. Science, 237(4811), 143–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seligman, M. E. (1972). Learned helplessness. Annual Review of Medicine, 23(1), 407–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, S. E., Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103(2), 193210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, S. E., Lichtman, R. R., Wood, J. V. (1984). Attributions, beliefs in control, and adjustment to breast cancer. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 489502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Windsor, T. D., Anstey, K. J., Butterworth, P., et al. (2007). The role of perceived control in explaining depressive symptoms associated with driving cessation in a longitudinal study. The Gerontologist, 47(2), 215–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×