Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-84b7d79bbc-fnpn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-25T08:34:21.814Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

15 - Sing Me Something Smart: Does Song Signal Cognition?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 June 2017

Carel ten Cate
Affiliation:
Universiteit Leiden
Susan D. Healy
Affiliation:
University of St Andrews, Scotland
Get access
Type
Chapter
Information
Avian Cognition , pp. 296 - 313
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Amici, F., Barney, B., Johnson, V. E. et al. (2012). A modular mind? A test using individual data from seven primate species. PLoS ONE, 7(12), e51918.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aplin, L. M., Sheldon, B. C. and Morand-Ferron, J. (2013). Milk bottles revisited: social learning and individual variation in the blue tit, Cyanistes caeruleus. Animal Behaviour, 85(6), 12251232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, K. E., Ramsay, S. L., Donaldson, C. et al. (2007). Parental prey selection affects risk-taking behaviour and spatial learning in avian offspring. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 274(1625), 25632569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolhuis, J. J., Okanoya, K. and Scharff, C. (2010). Twitter evolution: converging mechanisms in birdsong and human speech. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(11), 747759.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonaparte, K. M., Riffle-Yokoi, C. and Burley, N. T. (2011). Getting a head start: diet, sub-adult growth, and associative learning in a seed-eating passerine. PLoS ONE, 6(9), 23775.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bond, A. B., Kamil, A. C. and Balda, R. P. (2007). Serial reversal learning and the evolution of behavioral flexibility in three species of North American corvids (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus, Nucifraga columbiana, Aphelocoma californica). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 121(4), 372379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boogert, N. J., Anderson, R. C., Peters, S. et al. (2011). Song repertoire size in male song sparrows correlates with detour reaching, but not with other cognitive measures. Animal Behaviour, 81(6), 12091216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boogert, N. J., Giraldeau, L.-A. and Lefebvre, L. (2008a). Song complexity correlates with learning ability in zebra finch males. Animal Behaviour, 76(5), 17351741.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boogert, N. J., Monceau, K. and Lefebvre, L. (2010). A field test of behavioural flexibility in Zenaida doves (Zenaida aurita). Behavioural Processes, 85(2), 135141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boogert, N. J., Reader, S. M., Hoppitt, W. et al. (2008b). The origin and spread of innovations in starlings. Animal Behaviour, 75(4), 15091518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bouchard, J., Goodyer, W. and Lefebvre, L. (2007). Social learning and innovation are positively correlated in pigeons (Columba livia). Animal Cognition, 10(2), 259266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brumm, H., Zollinger, S. A. and Slater, P. J. B. (2009). Developmental stress affects song learning but not song complexity and vocal amplitude in zebra finches. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 63(9), 13871395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brust, V., Krüger, O., Naguib, M. et al. (2014). Lifelong consequences of early nutritional conditions on learning performance in zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata). Behavioural Processes, 103, 320326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buchanan, K. L., Catchpole, C. K., Lewis, J. W. et al. (1999). Song as an indicator of parasitism in the sedge warbler. Animal Behaviour, 57, 307314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buchanan, K. L., Spencer, K. A., Goldsmith, A. R. et al. (2003). Song as an honest signal of past developmental stress in the European starling (Sturnus vulgaris). Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 270(1520), 11491156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byers, B. E. and Kroodsma, D. E. (2009). Female mate choice and songbird song repertoires. Animal Behaviour, 77(1), 1322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catchpole, C. K. (1996). Song and female choice: good genes and big brains? Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 11(9), 358360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catchpole, C. K. and Slater, P. J. B. (2008). Bird Song: Biological Themes and Variations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coleman, S. W., Patricelli, G. L., Coyle, B. et al. (2007). Female preferences drive the evolution of mimetic accuracy in male sexual displays. Biology Letters, 3(5), 463466.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deary, I. J., Penke, L. and Johnson, W. (2010). The neuroscience of human intelligence differences. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(3), 201211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeVoogd, T. J. (2004). Neural constraints on the complexity of avian song. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 63(4), 221232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeVoogd, T. J., Krebs, J. R., Healy, S. D. et al. (1993). Relations between song repertoire size and the volume of brain nuclei related to song: comparative evolutionary analyses amongst oscine birds. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 254, 7582.Google Scholar
Doupe, A. J. and Kuhl, P. K. (1999). Birdsong and human speech: common themes and mechanisms. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 22, 567631.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farrell, T. M., Neuert, M. A. C., Cui, A. et al. (2015). Developmental stress impairs a female songbird's behavioural and neural response to a sexually selected signal. Animal Behaviour, 102, 157167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farrell, T. M., Weaver, K., An, Y.-S. et al. (2012). Song bout length is indicative of spatial learning in European starlings. Behavioral Ecology, 23(1), 101111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisher, M. O., Nager, R. G. and Monaghan, P. (2006). Compensatory growth impairs adult cognitive performance. PLoS Biology, 4(8), e251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffin, A., Healy, S. D. and Guillette, L. M. (2015). Cognition and personality: An analysis of an emerging field. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 30(4), 207214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holveck, M.-J., de Castro, A. C. V., Lachlan, R. F. et al. (2008). Accuracy of song syntax learning and singing consistency signal early condition in zebra finches. Behavioral Ecology, 19(6), 12671281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holveck, M.-J. and Riebel, K. (2010). Low-quality females prefer low-quality males when choosing a mate. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 277(1678), 153160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isden, J., Panayi, C., Dingle, C. et al. (2013). Performance in cognitive and problem-solving tasks in male spotted bowerbirds does not correlate with mating success. Animal Behaviour, 86(4), 829838.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keagy, J., Savard, J.-F. and Borgia, G. (2011). Complex relationship between multiple measures of cognitive ability and male mating success in satin bowerbirds, Ptilonorhynchus violaceus. Animal Behaviour, 81(5), 10631070.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitaysky, A. S., Kitaiskaia, E. V., Piatt, J. F. et al. (2003). Benefits and costs of increased levels of corticosterone in seabird chicks. Hormones and Behavior, 43(1), 140149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kitaysky, A. S., Kitaiskaia, E. V., Piatt, J. F. et al. (2006). A mechanistic link between chick diet and decline in seabirds? Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 273(1585), 445450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kriengwatana, B., Farrell, T. M., Aitken, S. D. T. et al. (2015). Early-life nutritional stress affects associative learning and spatial memory but not performance on a novel object test. Behaviour, 152, 195218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lachlan, R. F., Anderson, R. C., Peters, S. et al. (2014). Typical versions of learned swamp sparrow song types are more effective signals than are less typical versions. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 281, 20140252.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lachlan, R. F. and Nowicki, S. (2012). How reliable is song learning accuracy as a signal of male early condition? The American Naturalist, 180(6), 751761.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lampe, H. M. and Espmark, Y. O. (2003). Mate choice in Pied Flycatchers Ficedula hypoleuca: can females use song to find high-quality males and territories? Ibis, 145(1), E24E33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lauay, C., Gerlach, N. M., Adkins-Regan, E. et al. (2004). Female zebra finches require early song exposure to prefer high-quality song as adults. Animal Behaviour, 68, 12491255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacLean, E. L., Hare, B., Nunn, C. L. et al. (2014). The evolution of self-control. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 111(20), E2140E2148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marler, P. (1990). Song learning: the interface between behaviour and neuroethology. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 329(1253), 109114.Google ScholarPubMed
Matzel, L. D. and Kolata, S. (2010). Selective attention, working memory, and animal intelligence. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(1), 2330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nowicki, S., Hasselquist, D., Bensch, S. et al. (2000). Nestling growth and song repertoire size in great reed warblers: evidence for song learning as an indicator mechanism in mate choice. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 267(1460), 24192424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nowicki, S., Peters, S. and Podos, J. (1998). Song learning, early nutrition and sexual selection in songbirds. American Zoologist, 38(1), 179190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nowicki, S., Searcy, W. A. and Peters, S. (2002). Quality of song learning affects female response to male bird song. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 269(1503), 19491954.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Overington, S. E., Morand-Ferron, J., Boogert, N. J. et al. (2009). Technical innovations drive the relationship between innovativeness and residual brain size in birds. Animal Behaviour, 78(4), 10011010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peters, S., Searcy, W. A. and Nowicki, S. (2014). Developmental stress, song-learning, and cognition. Integrative and Comparative Biology, 54(4), 555567.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pfaff, J. A., Zanette, L., MacDougall-Shackleton, S. A. et al. (2007). Song repertoire size varies with HVC volume and is indicative of male quality in song sparrows (Melospiza melodia). Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 274(1621), 20352040.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pravosudov, V. V., Lavenex, P. and Omanska, A. (2005). Nutritional deficits during early development affect hippocampal structure and spatial memory later in life. Behavioral Neuroscience, 119(5), 13681374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reid, J. M., Arcese, P., Cassidy, A. L. E. V. et al. (2005). Fitness correlates of song repertoire size in free-living song sparrows (Melospiza melodia). The American Naturalist, 165(3), 299310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, C. and Healy, S. D. (2014). Measuring variation in cognition. Behavioral Ecology, 25(6), 12871292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, K. L., McCallum, E. S., MacDougall-Shackleton, E. A. et al. (2013a). Early-life stress affects the behavioural and neural response of female song sparrows to conspecific song. Animal Behaviour, 85(4), 825837.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, K. L., Moore, S. D., MacDougall-Shackleton, E. A. et al. (2013b). Early-life stress affects song complexity, song learning and volume of the brain nucleus RA in adult male song sparrows. Animal Behaviour, 86(1), 2535.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sewall, K. B., Soha, J. A., Peters, S. et al. (2013). Potential trade-off between vocal ornamentation and spatial ability in a songbird. Biology Letters, 9, 20130344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaw, R. C., Boogert, N. J., Clayton, N. S. et al. (2015). Wild psychometrics: evidence for “general” cognitive performance in wild New Zealand robins, Petroica longipes. Animal Behaviour, 109, 101111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shettleworth, S. J. (2010). Cognition, Evolution, and Behavior. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stamps, J. A. and Groothuis, T. G. G. (2010). Developmental perspectives on personality: implications for ecological and evolutionary studies of individual differences. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 365, 40294041.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Templeton, C. N., Laland, K. N. and Boogert, N. J. (2014). Does song complexity correlate with problem-solving performance in flocks of zebra finches? Animal Behaviour, 92, 6371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thornton, A., Isden, J. and Madden, J. R. (2014). Toward wild psychometrics: linking individual cognitive differences to fitness. Behavioral Ecology, 25(6), 12991301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodgate, J. L., Mariette, M. M., Bennett, A. T. D. et al. (2012). Male song structure predicts reproductive success in a wild zebra finch population. Animal Behaviour, 83(3), 773781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zann, R. and Cash, E. (2008). Developmental stress impairs song complexity but not learning accuracy in non-domesticated zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 62(3), 391400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zann, R. A. (1996). The zebra finch: A synthesis of field and laboratory studies. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×