Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-xfwgj Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-15T12:18:46.188Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

19 - Creativity in Children’s Museums

from Part IV - Environments and Contexts

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 November 2021

Sandra W. Russ
Affiliation:
Case Western Reserve University, Ohio
Jessica D. Hoffmann
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
James C. Kaufman
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
Get access

Summary

Children’s museums attract millions of families every year, providing opportunities for learning through play and for developing children’s creativity. Many of these museums actively collaborate with local communities, educators, and researchers to help improve the experiences of visitors. What makes such informal learning environments unique is how the facilitators, and the exhibits themselves, are mostly designed to provide children with an experience of guided discovery. Children actively engage with the exhibits by tinkering with materials and testing hypotheses about what will happen next, and develop creativity by interpreting novel experiences and solving discovered problems. Some museums go so far as to develop curricula and programs that explicitly promote the creativity of children. Collaborations with academic researchers influence approaches to encouraging creativity, while also evaluating the impacts of museum exhibits and programs.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alfieri, L., Brooks, P. J., Aldrich, N. J., & Tenenbaum, H. R. (2011). Does discovery-based instruction enhance learning? Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(1), 118. doi:10.1037/a0021017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amabile, T. M. (1985). Motivation and creativity: Effects of motivational orientation on creative writers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(2), 393399. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.48.2.393CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bassok, D., Latham, S., & Rorem, A. (2016). Is kindergarten the new first grade? AERA Open, 1(4), 131. doi:10.1177/2332858415616358Google Scholar
Beghetto, R. A. (2006). Creative self-efficacy: Correlates in middle and secondary students. Creativity Research Journal, 18(4), 447457. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj1804_4Google Scholar
Benjamin, N., Haden, C. A., & Wilkerson, E. (2010). Enhancing building, conversation, and learning through caregiver–child interactions in a children’s museum. Developmental Psychology, 46(2), 502515. doi:10.1037/a0017822CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonawitz, E. B., Shafto, P., Gweon, H., Goodman, N. D., Spelke, E., & Schulz, L. (2011). The double-edged sword of pedagogy: Instruction limits spontaneous exploration and discovery. Cognition, 120(3), 322330. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.10.001Google Scholar
Bonawitz, E. B., van Schijndel, T., Friel, D., & Schulz, L. (2012). Balancing theories and evidence in children’s exploration, explanations, and learning. Cognitive Psychology, 64(4), 215234. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2011.12.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Callanan, M. A. (2012). Conducting cognitive developmental research in museums: Theoretical issues and practical considerations. Journal of Cognition and Development, 13(2), 137151. doi:10.1080/15248372.2012.666730CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Callanan, M. A., Castañeda, C. L., Luce, M. R., & Martin, J. L. (2017). Family science talk in museums: Predicting children’s engagement from variations in talk and activity. Child Development, 88(5), 14921504. doi:10.1111/cdev.12886Google Scholar
Callanan, M. A., Cervantes, C., & Loomis, M. (2011). Informal learning. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2(6), 646655. doi:10.1002/wcs.143Google ScholarPubMed
Callanan, M. A., Legare, C. H., Sobel, D. M., Jaeger, G. J., Letourneau, S., McHugh, S. R., … Watson, J. (2020). Exploration, explanation, and parent–child interaction in museums. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 85(1), 7137.Google Scholar
Caniëls, M. C., & Rietzschel, E. F. (2015). Organizing creativity: Creativity and innovation under constraints. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(2), 184196. doi:10.1111/caim.12123CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Center for Childhood Creativity (2017). The CREATE Framework: Learning environments to develop creativity. Bay Area Discovery Museum. Retrieved from https://centerforchildhoodcreativity.org/Google Scholar
Choi, K., Grados, M., & Bonawitz, E. (2019). Observing child-led exploration improves parents’ causal inferences. In Goel, A. K., Seifert, C. M., & Freska, C. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 41st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Montreal: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Crowley, K., & Callanan, M. (1998). Describing and supporting collaborative scientific thinking in parent–child interactions. Journal of Museum Education, 23(1) (pp. 1513–1519), 1217. doi:10.1080/10598650.1998.11510365CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crowley, K., Callanan, M. A., Tenenbaum, H. R., & Allen, E. (2001). Parents explain more often to boys than to girls during shared scientific thinking. Psychological Science, 12(3), 258261. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00347CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). The general causality orientations scale: Self-determination in personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 19(2), 109134. doi:10.1016/0092-6566(85)90023-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dierking, L. D., & Falk, J. H. (2003). Optimizing out-of-school time: The role of free-choice learning. New Directions for Student Leadership, 2003(97), 7588. doi:10.1002/yd.36Google Scholar
Elkind, D. (2008). Can we play? Greater Good Magazine. Retrieved from https://greatergood.berkeley.edu/article/item/can_we_playGoogle Scholar
Evans, N. S., Todaro, R. D., Schlesinger, M. A., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2021). Examining the impact of children's exploratory behaviors on creativity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 207, 105091.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falk, J. H., & Dierking, L. D. (2002). Lessons without limit: How free-choice learning is transforming education. Landham, MD: Rowman Altamira.Google Scholar
Falk, J. H., & Dierking, L. D. (2010). The 95 Percent Solution School is not where most Americans learn most of their science. American Scientist, 98(6), 486493. doi:10.1511/2010.87.486CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fung, G., & Callanan, M. (2013, April). Pedagogy versus exploration: Parent–Child interactions in a museum setting [Poster presentation]. Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Seattle, WA.Google Scholar
Gray, P. (2011). The decline of play and the rise of psychopathology in children and adolescents. American Journal of Play, 3(4), 443463.Google Scholar
Hadani, H., & Jaeger, G. J. (2015). Inspiring a generation to create: Critical components of creativity in children [White paper]. Retrieved from http://centerforchildhoodcreativity.org/research/published/Google Scholar
Haden, C. A. (2010). Talking about science in museums. Child Development Perspectives, 4(1), 6267. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2009.00119.xGoogle Scholar
Haden, C. A., Jant, E. A., Hoffman, P. C., Marcus, M., Geddes, J. R., & Gaskins, S. (2014). Supporting family conversations and children’s STEM learning in a children’s museum. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(3), 333344. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.04.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrington, D. M. (1975). Effects of explicit instructions to “be creative” on the psychological meaning of divergent thinking test scores. Journal of Personality, 43(3), 434454. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1975.tb00715.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawkins, D. (1965). Messing about in science. Science and Children, 2(5), 59.Google Scholar
Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., Berk, L., & Singer, D. G. (2009). A mandate for playful learning in preschool: Presenting the evidence. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Jant, E. A., Haden, C. A., Uttal, D. H., & Babcock, E. (2014). Conversation and object manipulation influence children’s learning in a museum. Child Development, 85(5), 20292045. doi:10.1111/cdev.12252Google Scholar
Jensen, H., Pyle, A., Zosh, J. M., Ebrahim, H. B., Scherman, A. Z., Reunamo, J., & Hamre, B. K. (2019). Play facilitation: The science behind the art of engaging young children [White paper]. Denmark: The LEGO Foundation.Google Scholar
Jordanous, A., & Keller, B. (2016). Modelling creativity: Identifying key components through a corpus-based approach. PLOS One, 11(10). doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0162959Google Scholar
Knutson, K., & Crowley, K. (2005a). Museum as learning laboratory: Developing and using a practical theory of informal learning (Part 1 of 2). Hand to Hand, 18 (4), 45.Google Scholar
Knutson, K., & Crowley, K. (2005b). Museum as learning laboratory: Bridging research and practice together (Part 2 of 2). Hand to Hand, 19 (1), 36.Google Scholar
Kurkul, K. E., & Corriveau, K. H. (2018). Question, explanation, follow-up: A mechanism for learning from others? Child Development, 89(1), 280294. doi:10.1111/cdev.12726Google Scholar
Legare, C. H. (2012). Exploring explanation: Explaining inconsistent information guides hypothesis-testing behavior in young children. Child Development, 83(1), 173185. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01691.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Legare, C. H. (2014). The contributions of explanation and exploration to children’s scientific reasoning. Child Development Perspectives, 8(2), 101106. doi:10.1111/cdep.12070Google Scholar
Legare, C. H., & Lombrozo, T. (2014). Selective effects of explanation on learning during early childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 126, 198212.Google Scholar
Legare, C. H., Sobel, D. M., & Callanan, M. (2017). Causal learning is collaborative: Examining explanation and exploration in social contexts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(5), 15481554. doi:10.3758/s13423–017-1351-3Google Scholar
Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973). Undermining children’s intrinsic interest with extrinsic reward: A test of the “overjustification” hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28(1), 129137. doi:10.1037/h0035519CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, C., Solis, S., Jensen, H., Hopkins, E., Neale, D., Zosh, J., & Whitebread, D. (2017). Neuroscience and learning through play: A review of the evidence [Research summary]. Denmark: The LEGO Foundation.Google Scholar
Luce, M. R., Callanan, M. A., & Smilovic, S. (2013). Links between parents’ epistemological stance and children’s evidence talk. Developmental Psychology, 49(3), 454461. doi:10.1037/a0031249Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., Kuhl, P. K., Movellan, J., & Sejnowski, T. J. (2009). Foundations for a new science of learning. Science, 325(5938), 284288. doi:10.1126/science.1175626Google Scholar
Miller, E., & Almon, J. (2009). Crisis in the kindergarten: Why children need to play in school. College Park, MD: Alliance for Childhood.Google Scholar
Nakamura, J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2014). The concept of flow. Flow and the foundations of positive psychology (pp. 239263). Dordrecht: Springer.Google Scholar
Nusbaum, E. C., Silvia, P. J., & Beaty, R. E. (2014). Ready, set, create: What instructing people to “be creative” reveals about the meaning and mechanisms of divergent thinking. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8(4), 423432. doi:10.1037/a0036549CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osberg, S. (1998). Shared lessons and self-discoveries: What research has taught Children’s Discovery Museum. Journal of Museum Education, 23(1), 1920. doi:10.1080/10598650.1998.11510367Google Scholar
Papert, S. (1980). Mindstorms: Children, computers, and powerful ideas. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Puchner, L., Rapoport, R., & Gaskins, S. (2001). Learning in children’s museums: Is it really happening? Curator: The Museum Journal, 44(3), 237259. doi:10.1111/j.2151-6952.2001.tb01164.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rathunde, K., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2005). Middle school students’ motivation and quality of experience: A comparison of Montessori and traditional school environments. American Journal of Education, 111(3), 341371. doi:10.1086/428885Google Scholar
Resnick, M., & Robinson, K. (2017). Lifelong kindergarten: Cultivating creativity through projects, passion, peers, and play. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Resnick, M., Myers, B., Nakakoji, K., Shneiderman, B., Pausch, R., Selker, T., & Eisenberg, M. (2005). Design principles for tools to support creative thinking. Institute for Software Research, Paper 816. Retrieved from http://repository.cmu.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1822&context=isr.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A., Illies, J. J., & Reiter-Palmon, R. (2005). Explicit instructions to be creative and original: A comparison of strategies and criteria as targets with three types of divergent thinking tests. The Korean Journal of Creativity & Problem Solving, 15(1), 515.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A., & Jaeger, G. J. (2012). The standard definition of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 24(1), 9296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russ, S. W. (1986). Children’s fantasy play, problem solving, and emotional development. Clinical Child Psychology Newsletter, 1, 5.Google Scholar
Russ, S. W. (2003). Play and creativity: Developmental issues. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 47, 291303.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A., & Okuda, S. M. (1991). The instructional enhancement of the flexibility and originality scores of divergent thinking tests. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 5(5), 435441. doi:10.1002/acp.2350050505CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: Classic definitions and new directions. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25(1), 5467. doi:10.1006/ceps.1999.1020Google Scholar
Schulz, L. E., & Bonawitz, E. B. (2007). Serious fun: Preschoolers engage in more exploratory play when evidence is confounded. Developmental Psychology, 43(4), 10451050. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.1045CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simons, J., Dewitte, S., & Lens, W. (2000). Wanting to have vs. wanting to be: The effect of perceived instrumentality on goal orientation. British Journal of Psychology, 91(3), 335351. doi:10.1348/000712600161862Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2012). Taking the US Patent Office criteria seriously: A quantitative three-criterion creativity definition and its implications. Creativity Research Journal, 24(2–3), 97106. doi:10.1080/10400419.2012.676974Google Scholar
Singer, D. G., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (Eds.). (2006). Play = learning: How play motivates and enhances children’s cognitive and social-emotional growth. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sobel, D., & Jipson, J. (Eds.). (2016). Relating research and practice: Cognitive development in museum settings. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Song, L., Golinkoff, R. M., Stuehling, A., Resnick, I., Mahajan, N., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Thompson, N. (2017, March–April). Parents’ and experts’ awareness of learning opportunities in children’s museum exhibits. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 49, 3945. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2017.01.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szechter, L. E., & Carey, E. J. (2009). Gravitating toward science: Parent–child interactions at a gravitational-wave observatory. Science Education, 93(5), 846858. doi:10.1002/sce.20333Google Scholar
Tierney, P., & Farmer, S. M. (2011). Creative self-efficacy development and creative performance over time. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96(2), 277293. doi:10.1037/a0020952Google Scholar
Torrance, E. P. (1968). A longitudinal examination of the fourth grade slump in creativity. Gifted Child Quarterly, 12(4), 195199. doi:10.1177/001698626801200401Google Scholar
van Schijndel, T. J., Visser, I., van Bers, B. M., & Raijmakers, M. E. (2015). Preschoolers perform more informative experiments after observing theory-violating evidence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 131, 104119. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2014.11.008Google Scholar
Willard, A. K., Busch, J. T., Cullum, K. A., Letourneau, S. M., Sobel, D. M., Callanan, M., & Legare, C. H. (2019). Explain this, explore that: A study of parent–child interaction in a children’s museum. Child Development, 90(5), e598e617. doi:10.1111/cdev.13232Google Scholar
Wolf, B., & Wood, E. (2012). Integrating scaffolding experiences for the youngest visitors in museums. Journal of Museum Education, 37(1), 2937. doi:10.1080/10598650.2012.11510715CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yu, Y., Bonawitz, E., & Shafto, P. (2019). Pedagogical questions in parent–child conversations. Child Development, 90(1), 147161. doi:10.1111/cdev.12850CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zosh, J. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Hopkins, E. J., Jensen, H., Liu, C., Neale, D., … Whitebread, D. (2018). Accessing the inaccessible: Redefining play as a spectrum. Frontiers in Psychology, 9(1124). doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01124Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×