Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-hfldf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-01T12:27:01.464Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

16 - Why Taï mangabeys do not use tools to crack nuts like sympatric-living chimpanzees: a cognitive limitation on monkey feeding ecology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 November 2019

Christophe Boesch
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für Evolutionäre Anthropologie, Germany
Roman Wittig
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für Evolutionäre Anthropologie, Germany
Catherine Crockford
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für Evolutionäre Anthropologie, Germany
Linda Vigilant
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für Evolutionäre Anthropologie, Germany
Tobias Deschner
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für Evolutionäre Anthropologie, Germany
Fabian Leendertz
Affiliation:
Robert Koch-Institut, Germany
Get access

Summary

Nuts are high in energetic and nutritional value, but the kernel inside is difficult to access. In the Taï forest, it is estimated that chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) can eat 6–10 times as many nuts with the help of a tool as to when they crack them with their own teeth. However, sympatric-living terrestrial monkeys never crack nuts using tools. So this begs the question, why not? In this chapter, we report on the foraging behaviour of the sooty mangabeys (Cercocebus atys atys). A quick synopsis goes as follows: they observe nut-cracking chimpanzees at a distance of 5–10 metres, relish in the leftovers of the freshly cracked nuts, and then continue to follow the chimpanzees to different nut-cracking sites. With this information, we go on to consider the underlying reasons for the absence of nut-cracking in sooty mangabeys, with a particular focus on cognitive limitations, and then discuss the implications of field observations for studies on imitation in the laboratory.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest
40 Years of Research
, pp. 261 - 271
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aisner, R. & Terkel, J. (1992). Ontogeny of pine cone opening behaviour in the black rat (Rattus rattus). Animal Behaviour, 44, 327336.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1990). Use of objects as hammers to open nuts by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Folia Primatologica, 54, 138145.Google Scholar
Bergmüller, R. (1998). Die Nahrungsökologie der Rauchgrauen Mangabe – Schlüssel zur sozialen Organisation? MSc thesis, Univesität Erlangen-Nürnberg, Erlangen.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. & Boesch, H. (1983). Optimisation of nut-cracking with natural hammers by wild chimpanzees. Behaviour, 83, 265286.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. & Boesch, H. (1984a). Possible causes of sex differences in the use of natural hammers by wild chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution, 13, 415440.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. & Boesch, H. (1984b). Mental map in wild chimpanzees: an analysis of hammer transports for nut-cracking. Primates, 25, 160170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C. & Boesch-Achermann, H. (2000). The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest: Behavioural Ecology and Evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bugnyar, T. & Huber, L. (1997). Push or pull: An experimental study on imitation in marmosets. Animal Behaviour, 54, 817831.Google Scholar
Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2007). Enculturated chimpanzees imitate rationally. Developmental Science, 10(4), F31F38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2013). Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, recognize successful actions, but fail to imitate them. Animal Behaviour, 86, 755761.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, R. W. (2003). Imitation as behaviour parsing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 358(1431), 529536.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W. & Russon, A. E. (1998). Learning by imitation: A hierarchical approach. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 667684.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Call, J. (2001). Body imitation in an enculturated orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus). Cybernetics and Systems Journal, 32, 97119.Google Scholar
Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (1996). The effect of humans on the cognitive development of apes. In Russon, A. E., Bard, K. & Parker, S. T. (eds.), Reaching into Thought (pp 371403). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Canale, R. G, Guidorizzi, C. E., Martins Kierulff, M. C. & Gatto, C. A. F. R. (2009). First records of tool use by wild populations of the yellow-breasted capuchin monkey (Cebus xanthosternos) and new records for the bearded capuchin (Cebus libidinosus). American Journal of Primatology, 71, 366372.Google Scholar
Carpenter, A. (1887). Monkeys opening oysters. Nature, 36, 53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clayton, N. S. & Dickinson, A. (1998). Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrub jays. Nature, 395, 272274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Custance, D., Whiten, A. & Fredman, T. (1999). Social learning of an artificial fruit task in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Comparative Psychology, 113, 1323.Google Scholar
Defler, T. R. (1979). On the ecology and behavior of Cebus albifrons in eastern Colombia II. Behaviour. Primates, 20, 491502.Google Scholar
de Moura, A. C. A. & Lee, P. C. (2004). Capuchin stone tool use in caatinga dry forest. Science, 306(5703), 1909.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V. & Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor events, a neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research, 91, 176180.Google Scholar
Doran, M. D. & Hunt, K. D. (1994). Comparative locomotor behavior of chimpanzees and bonobos. In Wrangham, R. W., McGrew, W. C., de Waal, F. B. M. & Heltne, P. G. (eds.), Chimpanzee Cultures (pp. 93108). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Ferrari, P. F., Maiolini, C., Addessi, E., Fogassi, L. & Visalberghi, E. (2005). The observation and hearing of eating actions activates motor programs related to eating in macaque monkeys. Behavioral Brain Research, 161, 95101.Google Scholar
Ferrari, P. F., Visalberghi, E., Paukner, A., Fogassi, L., Ruggiero, A. & Suomi, S. J. (2006). Neonatal imitation in rhesus macaques. PLoS Biology, 4(9), e302.Google Scholar
Förderer, T. (2001). Orientierung bei Rauchgrauen Mangaben (Cercocebus torquatus atys) im Taï-Nationalpark, Elfenbeinküste. MSc thesis, Universität Münster, Münster.Google Scholar
Fragaszy, D., Izar, P., Visalberghi, E., Ottoni, E. B. & de Oliveira, M. G. (2004a). Wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) use anvils and stone pounding tools. American Journal of Primatology, 64, 359366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fragaszy, D. M., Visalberghi, E. & Fedigan, L. M. (2004b). Learning together: Socially biased learning. In Visalberghi, D. M., Fedigan, E. & Fragaszy, L. M. (eds.), The Complete Capuchin. The Biology of the Genus Cebus (pp. 244259). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fragaszy, D., Visalberghi, E. & Galloway, A. (1997). Infant tufted capuchin monkeys’ behaviour with novel foods: Opportunism, not selectivity. Animal Behaviour, 53, 13371343.Google Scholar
Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L. & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593609.Google Scholar
Guillaume, P. & Meyerson, I. (1934). Recherches l’usage de l’instument chez les singes, III: L’intermediaire independant de l’objet. Journal of Psychology, 31, 497554Google Scholar
Gumert, M. D., Kluck, M. & Malaivijitnond, S. (2009). The physical characteristics and usage patterns of stone axe and pounding hammers used by long-tailed macaques in the Andaman sea region of Thailand. American Journal of Primatology, 71, 594608.Google Scholar
Hausberger, M. (1993). How studies on vocal communication in birds contribute to a comparative approach of cognition. Revista de Etologia, 3, 171185.Google Scholar
Horn, A. D. (1987). The socioecology of the black mangabey (Cercocebus aterrimus) near lake Tumba, Zaire. American Journal of Primatology, 12, 165180.Google Scholar
Horner, V., Whiten, A., Flynn, E. & de Waal, F. B. M. (2006). Faithful replication of foraging techniques along cultural transmission chains by chimpanzees and children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103, 13878–13883, http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.0606015103.Google ScholarPubMed
Hulme, T. & Matsuzawa, T. (2004). Oil palm use by adjacent communities of chimpanzees at Bossou and Nimba mountains, West Africa. International Journal of Primatology, 25, 551581.Google Scholar
Hylander, W. L. (1975). Incisor size and diet in anthropoids with special reference to Cercopithecidae. Science, 189, 10951098.Google Scholar
Hylander, W. L. (1979). The functional significance of primate mandibular form. Journal of Morphology 160, 223240.Google Scholar
Izawa, K. & Mizuno, A. (1977). Palm-fruit cracking behavior of wild black-capped capuchin (Cebus apella). Primates, 18, 773792.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janmaat, K. R. L., Byrne, R. W. & Zuberbühler, K. (2006a). Primates take weather into account when searching for fruit. Current Biology, 16, 12321237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janmaat, K. R. L., Byrne, R. W. & Zuberbühler, K. (2006b). Evidence for a spatial memory of fruiting states of rain forest trees in wild ranging mangabeys. Animal Behaviour, 72, 797807.Google Scholar
Janson, C. H. & van Schaik, C. (2000). Ecological risk aversion in juvenile primates: Slow and steady wins the race. In Pereira, M. E. & Fairbanks, L. A. (eds.), Juvenile Primates: Life History, Development, and Behavior (pp 5774). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kyes, R. C. (1988). Grooming with a stone in sooty mangabeys (Cercocebus atys). American Journal of Primatology, 16, 171175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, P. C. & de Moura, A. C. A. (2005). Terrestriality and tool use (response). Science, 308(5724), 951952.Google Scholar
Mannu, M. & Ottoni, E. B. (1996). Observações preliminares das técnicas de forrageamento e uso espontâneo de ferramentas por um grupo de macacos-prego (Cebus apella) em condições de semi-cativeiro [Preliminary observations on foraging techniques and tool use by a semi-captive capuchin group]. Anais Etologia, 14, 384.Google Scholar
Mannu, M. & Ottoni, E. B. (2009). The enhanced tool-kit of two groups of wild bearded capuchin monkeys in the caatinga: Tool making, associative use, and secondary tools. American Journal of Primatology, 71, 242251.Google Scholar
Matsuzawa, T. (1994). Field experiments on use of stone tools by chimpanzees in the wild. In Wrangham, R. W., McGrew, W. C., de Waal, F. B. M & Heltne, P. G. (eds.), Chimpanzee Cultures (pp. 351370). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
McGraw, S. (1996). Cercopithecid locomotion, support use and support availability in the Taï Forest, Ivory Coast. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 100, 507522.Google Scholar
McGraw, W. S., Vick, A. & Daegling, D. J. (2011). Sex and age differences in the diet and ingestive behaviors of sooty mangabeys (Cercocebus atys) in the Tai Forest, Ivory Coast. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 144, 140153.Google Scholar
McGrew, W. C. & Marchant, L. F. (1997). Using the tools at hand: Manual lateratily and elementary technology in Cebus spp. and Pan spp. International Journal of Primatology, 18, 787810.Google Scholar
Meulman, E. J., Sanz, C. M., Visalberghi, E. & van Schaik, C. P. (2012). The role of terrestriality in promoting primate technology. Evolutionary Anthropology, 21(2), 5868.Google Scholar
Meulman, E. J. M. & van Schaik, C. P. (2011). What orangutan tool use can tell us about the evolution of technology. Presented at the 12th Conference Gesellschaft für Primatologie.Google Scholar
Noser, R. & Byrne, R. W. (2007). Travel routes and planning of visits to out-of-sight resources in wild chacma baboons, Papio ursinus. Animal Behaviour, 73, 257266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ottoni, E. B. & Mannu, M. (2001). Semifree-ranging tufted capuchins (Cebus apella) spontaneously use tools to crack open nuts. International Journal of Primatology, 22, 347358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ottoni, E. B., de Resende, B. D. & Izar, P. (2005). Watching the best nutcrackers: what capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) know about others’ tool-using skills. Animal Cognition, 24, 215219.Google Scholar
Range, F. (1999). The social system and mode of competition in female sooty mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus atys). MSc thesis, University of Bayreuth, Bayreuth.Google Scholar
Range, F. (2004). Social behavior and social knowledge of sooty mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus atys). PhD thesis, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia.Google Scholar
Range, F. & Huber, L. (2007). Attention in common marmosets: Implications for social-learning experiments. Animal Behaviour, 73, 10331041.Google Scholar
Rutte, C. (1998). Strategien der Nahrungssuche bei der Rauchgrauen Mangabe (Cercocebus torquatuss atys). MSc thesis, Friedrich-Alexamder-Univesität, Erlangen-Nürnberg.Google Scholar
Sirianni, G., Mundry, R. & Boesch, C. (2015). When to choose which tool: Multidimensional and conditional selection of nut-cracking hammers in wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 100, 152165.Google Scholar
Spagnoletti, N. (2009). Uso di strumenti in una popolazione di Cebus libidinosus allo stato selvatico in Piaui, Brasile. PhD dissertation, Dipartimento di Bologia Animale e dell’Uomo, Universita La Sapienza di Roma, Rome, Italy.Google Scholar
Spagnoletti, N., Visalberghi, E., Ottoni, E., Izar, P. & Fragaszy, D. (2011). Stone tool use by adult wild bearded capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus). Frequency, efficiency and tool selectivity. Journal of Human Evolution, 61, 97107.Google Scholar
Sugiyama, Y. & Koman, J. (1979). Tool using and making behavior in wild chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea. Primates, 20, 513524.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1898). Animal intelligence: An experimental study of the associative process in animals. Psychological Review Monograph, 2, 551553.Google Scholar
van Schaik, C. P., Deaner, R. O. & Merrill, M. Y. (1999). The conditions for tool use in primates implications for the evolution of material culture. Journal of Human Evolution, 36, 719741.Google Scholar
Visalberghi, E. & Fragaszy, D. M. (1989). Social influences on the acquisition of tool-using behaviors in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 103, 159170.Google Scholar
Visalberghi, E. & Fragaszy, D. M. (1990). Do monkeys ape? In Parker, S. T. & Gibson, K. R. (eds.), Language and Intelligence in Monkeys and Apes (pp. 247273). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Visalberghi, E. & McGrew, W. C. (1997). Cebus meets Pan. International Journal of Primatology, 18, 677681.Google Scholar
Visalberghi, E., Sirianni, G., Fragaszy, D. & Boesch, C. (2015). Percussive tool use by Taï Western chimpanzees and Fazenda Boa Vista bearded capuchin monkeys: A comparison. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 370(1682).Google Scholar
Voelkl, B. & Huber, L. (2000). True imitation in marmosets. Animal Behaviour, 60, 195202.Google Scholar
Vogel, G. (2002). Can chimps ape ancient hominid toolmakers? Science, 296, 1380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waser, P. M. (1977). Feeding, ranging and group size in the mangabey Cercocebus albigena. In Clutton-Brock, T. H. (ed.), Primate Ecology (pp. 183222). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×